• 沒有找到結果。

Learning paragraph structure with online annotations: An interactive approach to enhancing EFL reading comprehension

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Learning paragraph structure with online annotations: An interactive approach to enhancing EFL reading comprehension"

Copied!
15
0
0

加載中.... (立即查看全文)

全文

(1)

Learning paragraph structure with online annotations: An interactive

approach to enhancing EFL reading comprehension

Jia-Jiunn Lo

a

, Shiou-Wen Yeh

b,

*

, Chao-Shien Sung

a a

Department of Information Management, Chung Hua University, Taiwan

b

Graduate Institute of Teaching English to Speakers of Other Languages, National Chiao Tung University, 1001 Ta-Hsueh Rd., Hsin-chu City 300, Taiwan

Received 31 October 2011; revised 9 January 2013; accepted 15 March 2013 Available online 17 April 2013

Abstract

An important element of EFL (English as a Foreign Language) reading instruction is helping students to recognize paragraph elements and to comprehend the main idea of a paragraph. This study proposed an interactive approach and constructed an online annotation system, Paragraph Annotator, to assist EFL students in learning paragraph structure. It allows readers to analyze paragraphs of text on web pages by noting any paragraph element (topic sentence, controlling idea, or supporting details) in the paragraph. In addition, users can use annotation tools to add their personal ideas to the highlighted element. An experiment was conducted to evaluate the effect of Paragraph Annotator on reading comprehension for EFL students. The results reveal that students using Paragraph Annotator had significantly better performance in both the Cued Recall Test and the Free Recall Test. Most notably, the effects of the system were more evident when tested with the Free Recall Test, when students were not provided with any cues. Participants using the system responded positively to Paragraph Annotator, with respect to perceived ease of use, perceived usefulness, attitude to use, and intention to use. Limitations and opportunities for future research are also discussed. Ó 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Paragraph structure; Reading comprehension; Online annotation; Computer-assisted language learning

1. Introduction

Text structure refers to the ways that writers organize information in text. It is used to identify the structural

features of texts as an aid to understanding and recall. Pardo (2004) suggested that the structure of the text

makes each text unique. By learning text structures, “students will be able to access a schema for a certain genre when they begin to read a new text and can begin to make text-to-text connections for a particular genre,

which will help them more easily make meaning” (p. 275). Moss (2004) also noted that, if students are to

* Corresponding author. Tel.:þ886 921 706 889; fax: þ886 3 571 9033.

E-mail addresses:jlo@chu.edu.tw(J.-J. Lo),shiouwen@mail.nctu.edu.tw(S.-W. Yeh),m09610027@chu.edu.tw(C.-S. Sung). 0346-251X/$ - see front matterÓ 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2013.03.003

www.elsevier.com/locate/system System 41 (2013) 413e427

(2)

survive in the Information Age, they must be equipped with the ability and knowledge to read expository text, because most of the text found on Internet websites is expository. Researchers have further highlighted the importance of reading from expository texts in the fields of science and social studies for young learners. They

argued, “[comprehension of] such text is critical for academic success” (Meyer et al., 2010, p. 62). In view of

the importance of text structure, teachers are encouraged to teach text structure to help students comprehend the different forms of text organization.

A paragraph is a sequence of sentences that supports one main point. It may stand by itself as in academic writing, or be one part of a longer piece of text, such as an essay or a book (Oshima and Hougue, 2006). Paragraphs usually contain elements, such as a topic sentence, a controlling idea, and supporting details. A topic sentence is a complete sentence, which sometimes appears at the beginning of a paragraph, stating or suggesting the main idea of a paragraph. It clearly states the topic and the controlling idea of the paragraph. The part of the topic sentence that announces the specific area to be discussed is called the controlling idea. For example, in the sentence “Diabetes is a disease that prevents your body from properly using the food you eat”, “Diabetes” is the topic, and “prevents your body from properly using the food” serves as the controlling idea of the topic sentence. Supporting sentences then develop, explain, or prove the topic sentence (Gillie et al., 2001).

It has been claimed that learning to identify paragraph elements can assist students in recognizing and under-standing the main idea of the paragraph (Duke and Pearson, 2002;Lorch et al., 1993;Meyer et al., 1980) and that an understanding of paragraph structure is essential for EFL (English as a Foreign Language) reading and writing in-struction (Gillie et al., 2001;Oshima and Hougue, 2006). If students can comprehend a paragraph, they can better comprehend a text. However, students often are not consciously aware of these elements. Therefore, an important task of reading instruction is helping students to recognize paragraph elements and to comprehend the main idea of a paragraph.

In the context of EFL reading, the nature of reading poses a challenge for learners. According to schema theory, reading is an active and constructive process, whereby learners generate meaning for information by accessing and applying existing knowledge (Carrell and Eisterhold, 1988). The reading process involves “the reader, the text, and the interaction between the two” (Butler-Pascoe and Wiburg, 2003, p. 114). When reading is learned in a second/foreign language, “the complexities increase exponentially because all the subskills required for each operation entail two

languages and their interactions” (Koda, 2012, p. 158). When reading English texts, many EFL students lack the

knowledge and ability to focus on the structure of the text or on the logical relationships among the ideas (Mohd Din,

1997). In addition, EFL teachers commonly find themselves dealing with a passive class, where students are

unre-sponsive and avoid interaction with the teacher and the reading materials (Hsu, 2006;Snell, 2011). This can be a frustrating experience for both students and teachers. Therefore, it is in urgent need for researchers and teachers to find out ways to help students read successfully in English.

In the Information Age, students need more than exposure to informational texts (Montelongo et al., 2006). They must be equipped with the ability and knowledge to read in the digital world (Schmar-Dobler, 2003). Nonetheless, many EFL learners complain that they cannot understand the meaning of online texts, even when they check hy-perlinks and online tools (Fan, 2008). For this reason, it is imperative to offer a supportive online reading environment for EFL learners, to allow them to achieve better reading comprehension. To meet these challenges, the current study developed an online system, Paragraph Annotator, and investigated the features and effects of online annotations, with reference to EFL reading strategies and reading comprehension.

By definition, annotation is a critical or explanatory note, or body of notes added to a text, especially with regard to literary work (Wolfe, 2002). When students read, they often underline important parts of a document or write notes in the margin. Many researchers have claimed that making reading annotations is a useful strategy for

the development of language in learners (e.g.,Bargeron et al., 2001; Kiewra, 1989;Lunsford and Ruszkiewicz,

1999; Salvatori, 1996; Wolfe, 2002; Yeh and Lo, 2009). For the purpose of language learning, online annota-tions increase the opportunities for interactive language practice in a motivating environment. They also facilitate

broad access to authentic information and rapid exchange of information (Wolfe, 2002; Wolfe and Neuwirth,

2001).

In this study, an online annotation system, Paragraph Annotator, was developed to capitalize on the advantages of the interactive reading approach. This system offers several supportive reading facilities, including Highlight (Topic Sentence, Controlling Idea, and Supporting Detail), Comment, and Dictionary, which help to engage learners in interactive reading. An experimental study was also conducted to investigate the effects of the system on EFL reading

(3)

comprehension. It is proposed that online annotations can be used as an interactive tool, to encourage EFL learners to identify paragraph structure and enhance reading comprehension.

2. Literature review

2.1. Paragraph structure and reading comprehension

The ability to read English is important to EFL students, because a great deal of professional and technical literature is written in English. EFL students not only need to read English textbooks, they also obtain knowledge from English journals or magazines (Hsu, 2006). In academic EFL settings, English reading is a foundation skill that fa-cilitates other learning. However, as students progress through university or college, they may experience difficulties in reading. For instance, the texts usually contain unfamiliar structures that make comprehension difficult. As claimed byMohd Din (1997)andHsu (2006), without the fundamental ability to read and comprehend paragraphs and texts, students can hardly benefit from university education.

Schema theory suggests that reading is an interactive and constructive process, whereby readers generate meaning

for information by applying existing knowledge (see Carrell and Eisterhold, 1988) and by constructing a holistic

description of the text (Bransford and Johnson, 1972;Gunnarsson, 1984). Efficient comprehension requires the ability to relate the textual material to one’s own knowledge (Carrell and Eisterhold, 1988). According toJonassen (1985), an instructional model that demonstrates the above principles of interactive reading is the generative hypothesis, which states that meaning for material presented by any medium is generated by activating the existing knowledge structures, in order to interpret what is presented.

Research has also shown that reading comprehension and the recall of information are dependent on a student’s ability to recognize organizational structures (Cook and Mayer, 1988;Goldman, & Rakestraw, 2000). Specifically, students’ knowledge of the structure of formal discourse assists them in understanding and remembering the text (Butler-Pascoe and Wiburg, 2003;Grabe, 1991). The recognition of an organizational pattern enables the student to form a mental representation of the information and to see the logical relationships advanced by the author. Good readers use text structure to abstract main ideas and to help them remember propositions gained from their reading (Montelongo et al., 2006). In related studies, many researchers have stressed the importance of teaching text structures to students (e.g.,Carrell, 1985;Duke and Pearson, 2002;Grabe, 1991;Lannon, 1998;Meyer et al., 2010;Mohd Din, 1997;Moss, 2004;Pardo, 2004).

Being aware of the demands of “living in an era when information is increasing at an alarming rate” and “students who learn to use the organization and structure of informational texts are better able to comprehend and retain the information found in them” (p. 710e711),Moss (2004)suggested that teachers can use various strategies to improve student comprehension of expository text structure. She further proposed a two-phase model to teach text structuree Phase 1: Teacher modeling of retellings; and Phase 2: Students practice retelling. She explained that, because expository text may be unfamiliar to students, teacher modeling is a critical first step in involving students in the recognition of expository structure. It was stressed that “teachers need to provide extensive scaffolding for students, as they develop understanding of the process” (p. 715). After the modeling phase, students need opportunities to practice the retellings. The interactive learning steps include: (1) encouraging students to think about the organizational pattern of the text; (2) supporting students in the recall of text, with scaffolds and prompts, such as pictures, for the text, or questions; and (3) encouraging students to identify information in the text and to make personal connections between their lives and the text.

In a more recent study,Meyer et al. (2010)created a web-based tutoring system, to help students develop a reading strategy (the structure strategy) for comprehending expository texts. Their study examined the effectiveness of different versions of web-based instruction, which focused on text structure and on students’ reading comprehension. In the two-factor experimental design, the factors were the type of feedback provided by the web-based tutor and the motivational factor of the choice of text topics in practice lessons. Their study showed that “non human, pedagogical agents [the ITSS tutor] can teach the structure strategy” (p. 88) by successfully addressing both parts of structure strategy instruction: (1) identifying text structure and (2) enhancing reading comprehension. They also claimed that there is an urgent need for educators and researchers to understand the impact of technology on text structure, in order to facilitate reading comprehension.

(4)

2.2. Online annotations and reading comprehension

According to interactive reading theories, reading comprehension is the interaction of many sub processes, including (1) access to background knowledge, (2) identification of the relative importance of ideas within text, (3)

prediction of upcoming information, and (4) inferential relation of concepts within and without the text (Eskey,

1986).Duke and Pearson (2002)also suggested that visual representation can help readers understand, organize, and remember what they read. For instance, highlighting information in the text can assist readers to extract important elements and remember what they read before. Highlighting the text is a supportive reading strategy to achieve better comprehension of the text and is regarded as a way to increase interactions between the computer and readers.

It appears that some characteristics of annotations conform to the reading comprehension theories. For instance, online annotation techniques can be developed to highlight texts within web documents and used to provide a visual representation of summaries of key ideas within articles (Du, 2004; Fan, 2008; Lo et al., 2005).Kiewra (1989)

indicated that annotations made while reading can aid rereading by offering readers effective and efficient retrieval cues, when they review the text and allow them to identify specific information within the text.Wolfe and Neuwirth (2001)also noted that annotations not only enhance readers’ memory and monitor their reading comprehension, but also help to clarify the main idea of the passages that are read.

Lannon (1998)suggested that there are three levels of reading, which answer different questions about reading. Firstly, reading to analyze identifies the basic principles and how they all fit together by breaking the article down and systematically examining its parts. Secondly, reading to summarize identifies the main ideas by retrieving information and understanding facts from the article. Thirdly, in reading to respond, readers join a “conversation”. They read in order to explore their own thinking and to deduce the special meaning that the article has for them. They then reinvent that material with a force and passion that will make a difference to readers of their own. Because online annotations allow interaction and the rapid storage, manipulation, and retrieval of information, online annotations indeed conform to the reading to analyze and reading to summarize levels of reading as

sug-gested byLannon (1998).

In the past ten years, a wide variety of systems that support online annotations in digital documents have been developed for learners and instructors, such as Virtual Notes (Koch and Schneider, 2000), the WATs (Rau et al., 2004),

Online Annotator (Yeh and Lo, 2009), andChang and Hsu’s (2011)system. Online annotations may be highlights,

comments, notes, explanations, or other types of external remarks that can be attached to a web document, or a selected part of the document. Online annotations have four primary uses: “to remember, to think, to clarify and to share” (Ovsiannikov et al., 1999, p. 336). Online annotations also provide interactive reading opportunities that help students identify the key elements of paragraphs. Of the studies examining the effects of text structure on ESL/EFL reading, only a few have investigated the effects of teaching paragraph structures on reading comprehension. In spite of the advantages mentioned above, the question of how online annotations might facilitate paragraph structure learning in the context of EFL reading has not been addressed.

2.3. Design parameters and research questions of the study

The current study developed an online annotation system, Paragraph Annotator, to facilitate paragraph structure learning for EFL students (seeLo et al., 2009for more details). The design parameters of the experimental system were drawn from the rationale and studies that are reviewed in the Introduction and Literature Review sections of this paper. In summary, reading comprehension can be enhanced by teaching students to use specific cognitive strategies, such as reading to analyze (Lannon, 1998), recognizing text structure (e.g.,Goldman and Rakestraw, 2000;Moss, 2004), highlighting texts with different colors to raise awareness (Du, 2004; Fan, 2008; Lo et al., 2005), visual-izing text structure (Duke and Pearson, 2002), and generating and manipulating text (Butler-Pascoe and Wiburg, 2003;

DeWinstanley and Bjork, 2004;Jonassen, 1985).

An experimental study was also conducted to investigate the following questions: (1) Will EFL students who read the article using Paragraph Annotator have higher Cued Recall Test scores than the control group students? (2) Will EFL students who read the article using Paragraph Annotator have higher Free Recall Test scores than the control group students? Specifically, the following hypotheses were tested, with regard to students’ reading comprehension:

(5)

H1.The experimental group has higher Cued Recall Test scores than the control group.

H2. The experimental group has higher Free Recall Test scores than the control group.The third research question

concerned students’ attitudes toward using Paragraph Annotator for reading: (3) What are EFL students’ attitudes toward the use of Paragraph Annotator for reading purposes?

3. Materials and method

3.1. Instructional program for the experimental group

In Paragraph Annotator, the annotation tools include three highlight buttons, in three different colors: yellow for topic sentence, blue for controlling idea, and green for supporting detail. Paragraph Annotator allows readers to analyze paragraphs, by marking down any paragraph element (topic sentence, controlling idea, or supporting details) and using the annotation tools to add their personal ideas to the text. Highlighting a paragraph element involved three steps: (1) select the element, (2) apply the corresponding annotation tool, and (3) add comments to the highlighted element. Specifically, while making an annotation, a pop-up window allows additional comments to be input for each annotation (Fig. 1). When identifying paragraphs’ key elements, the student first selects the target text and then clicks one of the annotation tools to activate the corresponding function. The topic sentence, controlling idea, and supporting details can then be highlighted with different colors so that students can visualize the paragraph structure and get the key elements of the text more easily (Fig. 2). These comments are shown later, when the user moves the cursor over the annotation mark. Paragraph Annotator presents visual representations of text and allows readers to identify re-lationships and to understand the organization of the text. A Cancel button is also available if students want to cancel an annotation mark.

In addition to the three highlight buttons in different colors, which allow students to identify the key elements of each paragraph, the system also includes a supportive reading strategy button, Dictionary. As discussed earlier,

vo-cabulary knowledge is important for reading. Vovo-cabulary and reading comprehension are inextricably linked (Duke

and Pearson, 2002). Reading ability is strictly constrained by vocabulary knowledge and the use of a dictionary enables readers to comprehend the text. By highlighting unknown words and clicking Dictionary button, students can look up these unknown words in the Yahoo Online Dictionary (Fig. 3).

3.2. Instructional program for the control group

Although the essay for the control group was the same as that used for the Paragraph Annotator, the control group was only provided with the online Dictionary (Fig. 4). Since it is important that students are prevented from changing the original content of an online reading mechanism, in this study, both Paragraph Annotator and the system for the control group were restricted to a read-only mode. In both systems, the content of the original essay could not be changed.

(6)

3.3. Reading material

The essay used for both the experimental group and the control group (entitled Stress) was selected from Gillie, Ingle, & Mumford’s “Read to Write: An Integrated Course for Nonnative Speakers of English” (2001, p. 232)(see

Appendix A). The six-paragragh essay follows a clear cause-effect organizational structure with logical relationships among the ideas. Paragraph (A) provides an introduction of the essay. Paragraphs (B) and (C) focus on the effects of stress. The remaining paragraphs discuss solutions to the problem of stress. Each paragraph of the essay contains the elements of a good paragraph, such as a topic sentence, a controlling idea, and the supporting details. This essay is written for intermediate-level nonnative speakers of English, which is appropriate for the English proficiency level of the participants.

Fig. 2. Screenshot of Paragraph Annotator: Paragraph elements highlighted in three different colors.

(7)

3.4. Experimental design and participants

The experiment was conducted in Chung Hua University in northern Taiwan. When students entered the University, they were required to take an English placement test in order to assess their levels of English language proficiency. Based on the test results, students were assigned to English classes, classified into three different levels, Level A, Level B, and Level C. To ensure that all participants had similar levels of English proficiency, this study used two Level-A classes (each with thirty-two students), taught by the same instructor. During the experiment, Class I comprised the experimental group and Class II the control Group. The control variable was the provision of online annotation tools (with online annotation tools or without the online annotation tools). The online annotation tools included: Topic Sentence Highlight, Controlling Idea Highlight, and Supporting Details Highlight, as supported learning strategies.

The experiment involved a reading phase and a testing phase. In the reading phase, the experimental group of students read the essay, Stress, using Paragraph Annotator and the control group read the same essay in a web-based environment with only an online dictionary. The experiment took one class hour (50 min) for both groups. Students took 30 min to read the essay. The testing phase immediately followed the reading phase. Both a Cued Recall Test and a Free Recall Test were used to assess students’ comprehension. The dependent variables were the recall scores collected after students had read the English essay. An attitude questionnaire was also administered to students in the experimental group to assess their attitudes to the system.

3.5. Instruments and data analysis

In second language research, immediate recall test is a common technique to elicit data immediately after the completion of the task to be recalled (Mackey and Gass, 2005). In this study, immediately after the reading phase of the experimental procedure, students’ reading comprehension was assessed, using a Cued-Recall Test and a Free Recall Test. By combining the two methods, the researchers were able to present a more detailed picture of how the Paragraph Annotator system affected students’ reading comprehension.

The Cued Recall Test was modeled on a measure developed byCarpenter et al. (2006), which investigated what

types of learning benefit from a Cued Recall Test. The Cued Recall Test used in the current study consisted of eight

multiple-choice items from the passage, Stress (Appendix B). The items were scored as correct or incorrect as a

measure of general comprehension. The student received one point for each correct item. After the scoring process, descriptive statistics were reported to describe the data collected from the Cued Recall Test, and t-test was employed to test if the means of the two groups were significantly different from one another. Cohen’s d was used as a measure of effect size to represent the difference of cued recall scores between the experimental group and the control group.

The Free Recall Test included open questions, which asked participants to recall the purpose of the essay and elements of each paragraph, such as topic sentences, controlling ideas, and supporting details, without any cues (see

(8)

Appendix B). To analyze and score students’ free recalls, each paragraph in the essay was firstly divided into para-graph elements, by two independent judges. Any disagreements were settled by a third judge. Each parapara-graph con-sisted of a topic sentence, a controlling idea, and supporting details. Once this segregation had been completed, students’ free recalls were scored for the presence of each paragraph element of the original text. In scoring, each free recall was blindly judged by two independent graders. Correlation analysis was conducted to test the degree of repeatability of the two graders. The graders achieved a correlation coefficient of r¼ 0.864 for the experimental group and r¼ 0.854 for the control group. The correlation coefficients were found to be significant at the P < 0.01 level. After the scoring process, descriptive statistics were reported to describe the data collected from the Free Recall Test, and t-test was employed to test if the means of the two groups were significantly different from one another. Cohen’s d was used as a measure of effect size to represent the difference of free recall scores between the experimental group and the control group.

Students in the experimental group also completed an Attitude Questionnaire after they completed the recall tests. The Likert scale questionnaire was used to investigate students’ attitudes toward the system they had just used. After reading each of the statements in the questionnaire, the student indicated how much he/she agreed or disagreed by choosing from the following choices: (5) Strongly Agree (4) Agree, (3) Undecided, (2) Disagree, and (1) Strongly

Disagree (seeAppendix C).

The questionnaire included fifteen items, which were divided into four constructs: perceived ease of use, perceived usefulness, attitude to use, and intention to use (Davis, 1989). Perceived ease of use (PEU) refers to the extent to which a person believes that using a system would be free of mental effort. Perceived usefulness (PU) refers to the extent to which a person believes that the functionality and information provided by a system would be useful. Attitude to use (AU) refers to the appraisal and extent of satisfaction with a system. Intention to use (IU) is the subjective possibility that users will use the system. In the questionnaire, perceived ease of use included three items, perceived usefulness included five items, attitude to use included two items, and intention to use included five items. After students completed the Attitude Questionnaire, the descriptive statistics for the attitude questionnaire were reported and one sample t-test was employed to analyze the data set.

4. Results

4.1. Research question (1): will EFL students who read the article using Paragraph Annotator have higher cued recall test scores than the control group students?

The experimental data were analyzed using SPSS statistical package.Table 1summarizes the results for the Cued-Recall Test. It shows that the mean score of the experimental group [Mean¼ 3.8125] is higher than the mean score of the control group [Mean¼ 3.0625]. Results of the t-test are presented inTable 2. The results show thatH1was not rejected, which suggests that the experimental group performed significantly better in the Cued Recall Test than the control group [P¼ 0.037](Table 2). The Cohen’s d for the Cued Recall Test was 0.5374. According to the criteria

Table 1

Descriptive statistics of cued recall test scores.

Group Number Mean S.D.

Experimental group 32 3.8125 1.7309

Control group 32 3.0625 0.9483

Table 2

t-test of cued recall test scores.

Equality of variance F-test for variance t-test for mean

F P t d.f. P

Equal 17.896 0.000*** 2.150 62 0.035*

Unequal 2.150 48.071 0.037*

(9)

recommended by Cohen (1992), effect sizes as 0.20, 0.50, and 0.80 are considered small, medium, and large respectively. Based on Cohen’s scale, 0.5374 is considered a medium effect size.

4.2. Research question (2): will EFL students who read the article using Paragraph Annotator have higher free recall test scores than the control group students?

The descriptive statistics of the Free Recall Test were computed for further analysis.Tables 3and4illustrate the descriptive statistics and the t-test results for the Free Recall Test scores. As can be seen inTable 3, the mean score of

the Experimental Group [Mean¼ 4.391] is higher than the mean score of the Control Group [Mean ¼ 2.688]. Results

of the t-test suggest thatH2was not rejected [P¼ 0.003](Table 4). The experimental group performed significantly

better in the Free Recall Test. The Cohen’s d for the Free Recall Test was 0.792. Based on Cohen’s scale (Cohen,

1992), high effect of testing was found for the Free Recall Test.

4.3. Research question (3): what are students’ attitudes toward the use of Paragraph Annotator for reading purposes?

Unlike the Cued and Free Recall Tests, which were used for both groups, the questionnaire was only completed by the experimental group to investigate their attitudes to Paragraph Annotator. The descriptive statistics for the Attitude Questionnaire are listed inTable 5. Since the items of the questionnaire required the participants to evaluate Para-graph Annotator, one sample t-test was used to test whether the mean values of these four variables equaled 3. The results of the t-test are listed inTable 6. The results are as expected. Namely, the mean values for all constructs of the attitude questionnaire were significantly greater than 3. That is, the experimental group had a positive attitude to Paragraph Annotator, with respect to perceived ease of use, perceived usefulness, attitude to use, and intention to use. 5. Discussion

The results of the experiment indicate that students in the experimental group significantly outperformed students in the control group in both the Cued Recall Test and the Free Recall Test. Importantly, the difference in mean scores between the experimental group and the control group is much higher for the Free Recall Test than for the Cued Recall Test [(P value of t-test for the Free Recall Test¼ 0.003) < (P value of t-test for the Cued Recall Test ¼ 0.037)](see

Tables 2and4). The effect sizes (Cohen’s d) for the Cued Recall Test and the Free Recall Test were 0.5374 and 0.792 respectively. The results show that the effects of the system on reading comprehension are more evident, if measured with a Free Recall Test, when students were not provided with cues (Fig. 5).

These findings suggest that the use of the system, Paragraph Annotator, is effective in enhancing EFL students’

knowledge of paragraph structure and their reading comprehension. This supports researchers’ (Butler-Pascoe and

Wiburg, 2003;Grabe, 1991) view that knowledge of paragraph structure assists the learner in understanding and

Table 3

Descriptive statistics of free recall test scores.

Group Number Mean S.D.

Experimental group 32 4.391 2.675

Control group 32 2.688 1.441

Table 4

t-test results of Free recall test scores.

Equality of variance F-test for variance t-test for mean

F P t d.f. P

Equal 7.341 0.009** 3.171 2 0.002**

Unequal 3.171 47.592 0.003**

(10)

remembering the text. These findings are also consistent with previous research (e.g., Carrell, 1985; Duke and Pearson, 2002; Grabe, 1991; Lannon, 1998;Meyer et al., 2010;Mohd Din, 1997;Moss, 2004;Pardo, 2004) that learning to identify text structure and paragraph elements can help students understand the main idea of a text and thus develop reading comprehension.

This study has taken a step in the direction of applying online annotation technology in the design of learning tools to identify text structure and paragraph elements. The Paragraph Annotator designed for the experimental group contains many interactive features that were not available to the control group. Three differently colored highlight buttons (Topic Sentence, Controlling Idea, and Supporting Detail) allowed students to identify the key elements of each paragraph. With these annotation tools, readers could highlight the information and graphically display their analysis of paragraph elements in the corresponding colors, in order to get the gist of the text more easily and to achieve better understanding of the essay. Students also could add comments to the highlighted element. In this sense, Paragraph Annotator accommodated not only interactive tools to support the identification of text structure,

visu-alization of text representations, and generation of reading strategies as suggested by researchers (Goldman and

Rakestraw, 2000; Moss, 2004; Duke and Pearson, 2002; Reutzel, 1985), but also for reading to respond, as

sug-gested byLannon (1998).

Researchers believe that those learners who use a greater variety of strategies tend to be the most successful (e.g.,

Oxford, 1990) and that learning strategies can be trained (Brunig, 1983). It is also agreed that “good readers use comprehension strategies to facilitate the construction of meaning” (McLaughlin, 2003, p. 7;McLaughlin and Allen,

2002, p. 9). These strategies include previewing, self-questioning, making connections, visualizing, knowing how

words work, monitoring, summarizing, and evaluating. For students in the experimental group, Paragraph Annotator became a vehicle for practicing specific reading skills and strategies, such as highlighting and identifying paragraph elements. The Paragraph Annotator provided more learning facilities than those available to the control group. If the learner makes use of those learning tools, they may stimulate, or substitute for, the student’s own learning strategies. This leads us to believe that online annotations in a web-based environment are effective in training EFL students who are not so proficient in English reading.

The results are also in agreement with the generative hypothesis. According to generative hypothesis, the gener-ation effect refers to the fact that people typically remember informgener-ation better when they have taken an active part in

producing it, rather than having it provided by an external source (DeWinstanley and Bjork, 2004). In reading

comprehension, “when presented with items that must be generated versus read at encoding, individuals typically remember better those items that they generated versus those that they only read” (DeWinstanley and Bjork, 2004, p. 945). In this study, using the reading support to guide students in their analysis of paragraph structure by highlighting

elements with different colors, allows “reading to analyze” as suggested byLannon (1998). Such an approach also

supportsSpolsky’s (1989)summary of conditions for successful second language acquisition (SLA), which states that a focus on form through manipulation of attention is important for effective task-based instruction.

Table 5

Descriptive statistics of attitude questionnaire.

No. of subjects Mean S.D.

Perceived ease of use 32 3.74 0.5723

Perceived usefulness 32 3.56 0.4212

Attitude to use 32 3.31 0.657

Intention to use 32 3.35 0.6565

Table 6

One sample t-tests of Attitude questionnaire.

t d.f. P (two-tailed)

Perceived ease of use 7.307 31 0.000***

Perceived usefulness 7.471 31 0.000***

Attitude to use 2.691 31 0.011*

Intention to use 3.016 31 0.005**

(11)

As discussed previously, reading in English is a frustrating experience for many EFL students. Grabe (1991)

identified six component skills of fluent reading for students who had not attained fluency, but aspired to do so for academic purposes: (1) automatic recognition skills, (2) vocabulary and structural knowledge, (3) formal discourse structural knowledge, (4) content/world knowledge, (5) synthesis and evaluation skills, and (6) metacognitive

knowledge and skills (cited inButler-Pascoe and Wiburg, 2003, p. 118). The Paragraph Annotator provide several

features (Highlight, Vocabulary, and Comment) which are suitable to assist EFL learners in developing structural knowledge, vocabulary knowledge, and evaluation skills. If the learner is likely to use those learning facilities, the system could help him/her develop reading fluency which is important for EFL students to acquire knowledge from English textbooks or other reading materials.

With respect to students’ attitudes to reading English with Paragraph Annotator, students in the experimental group showed positive attitudes to the system. The results suggest that even though the highlighting of paragraph elements required extra work, the proposed online annotation system was still user-friendly and did not impede the reading processes of the learners. The use of color can enhance learning and motivation. It is helpful, not only for attracting attention to important information, but also for increasing the information capacity (Alessi and Trollip,

2001). Since people tend to be eye-minded, effective visual aids can emphasize important information to help

readers analyze whatever is said in the text.

Due to time constraints, this study administered two immediate recall tests to assess students’ reading compre-hension (Cued Recall Test and Free Recall Test). It is suggested that future research could administer delayed post-tests (about 4 weeks after the training) to determine the persistence of the effect. It should also be noted that this study is primarily concerned with EFL students. In general, EFL readers have a wide range of learning needs and learning strategies (Oxford, 1990). They also need good user-oriented instruction and interactive language learning

experi-ences (Larsen-Freeman, 2000; Nunan, 1993). The authors recommend that future research could replicate the

approach outlined in this study with non-EFL students. In addition, this study obtained permission of 64 EFL students who registered as freshmen in the University. The number of subjects in each group was limited to 32 because of practical reasons. Future research could replicate this study with more subjects to further test the hypothesis of this

study. Finally, as stated byButler-Pascoe and Wiburg (2003, p. 118), “developing reading fluency is a long-term

process that results from continuous effort and gradual improvement”. For future studies, online annotations could offer many other supportive tools, such as examples and sharing, to scaffold various reading strategy training.

6. Conclusions

EFL reading instruction must equip students with active and effective reading strategies, which allow them to become fluent readers. Online annotation technology provides a vehicle for learners to practice specific reading skills and strategies. In this study, Paragraph Annotator was proposed to help EFL students learn how to read essays, using interactive annotation strategies to enhance students’ reading comprehension. Paragraph Annotator provides several interactive tools that allow students to recognize and identify the underlying structure of paragraphs. While using the

(12)

system, students can learn to identify a paragraph’s structure by highlighting the topic sentence, the controlling idea and the supporting details. Such tools can help students focus on the key elements of a paragraph and allow them to manually mark up the elements. The marked text structure visually represents the relationships between paragraph elements and helps students to visualize and monitor their comprehension as they read. The experimental results show that students using Paragraph Annotator perform significantly better in both Cued and Free Recall Tests. The results of this study suggest that online annotation technology allows EFL readers the flexibility to interact with the text in ways not possible with books alone.

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge the research support of the National Science Council of Taiwan (Grant No: NSC 98-2511-S-216-003-MY3) and Chung-Hua University (Grant No: CHU-NSC 98-98-2511-S-216-003-MY3). We would also like to thank the anonymous reviewers for insightful comments on an earlier version of this paper.

Appendix A The article Stress

(A) All people have some stress in their lives. A little stress makes life interesting, and it keeps you on your toes. However, too much stress can be unhealthy. You may find your life becoming stressful whenever there are changes in your environment, in your relationships, or in the demands you put on yourself. Stress can be caused by having too many problems in your life, such as trouble with money, school, job, or family. Stress can also be caused by good things in your life, such as getting a new job, moving to a new place, or getting married.

(B) How can you tell when your life is getting too “stressful”? You will probably find yourself always feeling rushed, as though you must hurry everywhere you go. There is never enough time to do everything that you need to do. You may find yourself walking, talking, and eating more quickly than usual. If you find that you eat more or less than usual, or that you don’t sleep very well, it may be that you are under a lot of stress. Too much stress can also make you feel depressed or bored; it can rob you of your excitement for life.

(C) Besides causing emotional problems, stress can also cause real physical problems. If you have too much stress in your life, you may suffer from stomach problems, headaches, or neck and shoulder pain. Too much stress can make you more vulnerable to illness and can

even make you more likely to have accidents! Doctors estimate that 50e80% of all disease is

directly related to stress. The cost of treating both stress and illness caused by stress is said to be in the billions of dollars each year.

(D) Prevention is the best way to deal with stress. There are many things that you can do to keep stress under control so that it will help you, not hurt you. First of all, be organized-there is nothing so stressful as a disorganized life. Put your life and your surroundings (your home, apartment, or desk at work or school) in order. Don’t procrastinate. Stop putting things off. Do what needs to be done today. On the other hand, if there are things that you can put off for another day, do. Don’t always feel that you have to do everything right now. Learn to say “no” to activities and commitments that will only add more stress to your life. Remember that you are only human, and that you can only do one thing at a time.

(13)

Appendix B

The cued recall test and the free recall test

(E) Learn to relax. Slow down. Take a break or a walk when you feel yourself getting stressed. Find a quiet place where you can go and meditate, breathe deeply, and bring back the balance in your life. Find a hobby that you enjoy. Reading, listening to music, or creating some thing with your hands are all great ways to bring some peace to a hectic life. Learn to laugh at yourself and your life. Try to look on the bright side of life. Even when things seem pretty dark and stressful, there is usually a place for some humor and a smile. Perhaps that is the most important time to find something to laugh about.

(F) Finally, one of the best ways to deal with stress is to take care of yourself physically and mentally. You will be able to handle the stress in your life much better if you eat a healthy diet, get plenty of rest each day, and find time for some exercise such as a brisk walk several times a week. Keep mentally healthy by reminding yourself of your talents and abilities (everybody has them!) and by appreciating the good things in your life each day.

The Cued Recall Test

1. ( ) Stress can be caused by having too many problems in your life, such as (a) job, relationship (b) families, money (c) school, exam (d) moving, life pressure 2. ( ) Stress can also be caused by good things in your life, such as

(a) job, relationship (b) families, money (c) school, exam (d) moving, marriage 3. ( ) How can you tell when your life is getting too “stressful”? Which of the followings is

not mentioned in the article?

(a) You always feel rushed. (b) You don’t sleep well. (c) You feel depressed. (d) You eat more or less than usual.

4. ( ) Doctors estimate that __________ of all disease is directly related to stress. (a) 20~50% (b) 30~60% (c) 40~70% (d) 50~80%

5. ( ) How much is the cost of treating both stress and illness caused by stress each year? (a) one hundred million US dollars (b) ten million US dollars (c) billions of US dollars (d) one million US dollars

6. ( ) What is the best way to deal with stress?

(a) Seeing a doctor (b) Exercise (c) Prevention (d) Complaint

7. ( ) There are many things that you can do to keep stress under control so that it will help you, not hurt you.

(a) Doing mountain climbing and travelling. (b) Stop putting things off. Learn to say “no”. (c) Doing exercises. Learn to relax. (d) Listening to music. Find a hobby that you enjoy.

8. ( ) One of the best ways to deal with stress is to take care of yourself ______. (a) physically and mentally (b) mentally and spiritually (c) cognitively and mentally (d) logically and psychologically

The Free Recall Test

1. What is the main idea of this article?

2. What are the Topic sentence, Controlling idea, and Supporting details of each paragraph of this article?

(14)

Appendix C

The attitude questionnaire

References

Alessi, S.M., Trollip, S.R., 2001. Multimedia for Learning: Methods and Development. Allyn & Bacon, Boston, MA.

Bargeron, D., Gupta, A., Grudin, J., Sanocki, E., 2001. Annotations for Streaming Video on the Web: System Design and Usage Studies. Microsoft Research Report. Retrieved January 5, 2012, from the World Wide Web:http://research.microsoft.com/en-us/um/people/jgrudin/publications/ multimedia/asynch-annotate/mraswww.pdf.

Bransford, J.D., Johnson, M.K., 1972. Contextual prerequisites for understanding: some investigations of comprehension and recall. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 4 (2), 717e726.

Brunig, I.L., 1983. An information processing approach to a theory of instruction. Educ. Commun. Technol. J. 31, 91e101. Butler-Pascoe, M.E., Wiburg, K.M., 2003. Technology and Teaching English Language Learners. Pearson Education, Inc, New York. Carpenter, S.K., Pashler, H., Vul, E., 2006. What types of learning are enhanced by a cued recall test? Psychon. Bull. Rev. 13, 826e830. Carrell, P.L., 1985. Facilitating ESL reading by teaching text structure. TESOL Q. 19 (4), 727e752.

Carrell, P.L., Eisterhold, J.C., 1988. Schema theory and ESL reading pedagogy. In: Carrell, P.L., Devine, J., Eskey, D.E. (Eds.), Interactive Approaches to Second Language Reading. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 73e92.

Chang, C.K., Hsu, C.K., 2011. A mobile-assisted synchronously collaborative translationeannotation system for English as a foreign language (EFL) reading comprehension. Comput. Assist. Lang. Learn. 24 (2), 155e180.

Cohen, J., 1992. A power primer. Psychol. Bull. 112 (1), 155e159.

Cook, L., Mayer, R., 1988. Teaching readers about the structure of scientific text. J. Educ. Psychol. 80 (4), 448e456.

Davis, F.D., 1989. Perceived usefulness, perceived ease of use, and user acceptance of information technology. MIS Q. 13 (3), 319e340. DeWinstanley, P.A., Bjork, E.L., 2004. Processing strategies and the generation effect: implications for making a better reader. Mem. Cogn. 32 (6),

945e955. Statements (5) S trongly Agree (4) Ag re e (3) Undecided (2) Disagree (1) S trongly Disagree

1. The functions of the system are easy to use. 2. This system has a well-designed interface. 3. This system is helpful to improve my English reading

comprehension.

4. The system makes my English reading become easier. 5. The system helps me to recognize Topic sentence, Controlling

idea, and Supporting details of paragraphs in the article. 6. I feel using this system is a joyful learning experience. 7. I am satisfied with the functions provided by the system. 8. I am willing to use this system as a tool for English reading. 9. It is a wise decision to use this system to assist learning. 10. I will continue using this system in the future.

11. I have a good recall of the content of reading from using this system.

12. I can understand the procedures to use the system. 13. The system helps me to recognize the main ideas of the article. 14. The system raises my motivation to use the online dictionary. 15. I will recommend this system to others for assisting English

(15)

Du, M.C., 2004. Personalized Annotation Management for Web-based Learning Service. National Central University, Chungli, Taiwan. Master thesis.

Duke, N.K., Pearson, P., 2002. Effective practices for developing reading comprehension. In: Farstrup, A.E., Samuels, S. (Eds.), What Research Has to Say About Reading Instruction. International Reading Association, Newark, DE, pp. 205e242.

Eskey, D., 1986. Theoretical foundations. In: Dubin, F., Eskey, D., Grabe, W. (Eds.), Teaching Second Language Reading for Academic Purpose. Addison-Wesley Publishing Company, Reading, MA, pp. 3e23.

Fan, Z.Y., 2008. An Investigation of Supportive Reading Strategy Use and Reading Comprehension in Multi-user Online Annotation System. Chung Yuan Christian University, Chungli, Taiwan. Master thesis.

Gillie, J.W., Ingle, S., Mumford, H., 2001. Read To Write: an Interactive Course for Non-native Speakers of English. The McGraw-Hill Com-panies, Singapore.

Goldman, S.R., Rakestraw, J., 2000. Structural aspects of constructing meaning from text. In: Kamil, M.L., Mosenthal, P.B., Pearson, P.D., Barr, R. (Eds.), Handbook of Reading Research. Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 311e335.

Grabe, W., 1991. Current developments in second language reading research. TESOL Q. 25 (3), 375e406.

Gunnarsson, B.L., 1984. Functional comprehensibility of legislative texts: experiments with a Swedish act of parliament. Text-Interdiscip. J. Study Discourse 4 (1e3), 71e106.

Hsu, S.C., 2006. The reading strategies used by EFL technical students. J. Nanya 26, 159e174.

Jonassen, D.H., 1985. Learning Strategies: a new educational technology. Programmed Learn. Educ. Technol. 22 (1), 26e34. Kiewra, K.A., 1989. A review of note-taking: the encoding-storage paradigm and beyond. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 1, 147e172.

Koch, S., Schneider, G., 2000, 19e21 January. Implementation of an Annotation Service on the WWW-virtual Notes. Paper presented at the 8th Euromicro Workshop on Parallel and Distributed Processing. Retrieved April 2, 2013 from IEEE Xplore.

Koda, K., 2012. How to do research on second language reading. In: Mackey, A., Gass, S.M. (Eds.), Research Methods in Second Language Acquisition: a Practical Guide. Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Oxford, pp. 158e179.

Lannon, J.M., 1998. Writing Process: a Concise Rhetoric. Longman, New York.

Larsen-Freeman, D., 2000. Techniques and Principles in Language Teaching. Oxford University Press, Oxford.

Lo, J.J., Tsao, C.H., Yeh, S.W., 2005. Using online annotations to facilitate reading: system development and empirical studies. Chung Yuan J. 33 (2), 193e214.

Lo, J.J., Sung, C.S., Yeh, S.W., 2009, 30 Novembere4 December. Reading Annotator: A Web-based Annotation System for Reading Support. Proceedings of the 17th International Conference on Computers in Education [CDROM]. Asia-Pacific Society for Computers in Education, Hong Kong.

Lorch, R.F., Lorch, E.P., Inman, W.E., 1993. Effects of signaling topic structure on text recall. J. Educ. Psychol. 85 (2), 281e290. Lunsford, A.A., Ruszkiewicz, J.J., 1999. The Presence of Others. St. Martin’s, New York.

Mackey, A., Gass, S., 2005. Second Language Research: Methodology and Design. Routledge, New York.

McLaughlin, M., 2003. Guided Comprehension in the Primary Grades. International Reading Association, Newark, DE.

McLaughlin, M., Allen, M.B., 2002. Guided Comprehension a Teaching Model for Grades 3-8. International Reading Association, Newark, DE. Meyer, B.J.F., Brandt, D., Bluth, G., 1980. Use of top-level structure in text: key for reading comprehension of ninth-grade students. Reading Res.

Q. 16 (1), 72e103.

Meyer, B.J.F., Wijekumar, K., Middlemiss, W., Higley, K., Lei, P.W., Meier, C., Spielvogel, J., 2010. Web-based tutorial of the structure strategy with or without elaborated feedback or choice for fifth- and seventh-grade readers. Reading Res. Q. 45 (1), 62e92.

Mohd Din, F.D., 1997. The Effects of Teaching Text Structure on the Reading and Writing Ability of ESL Learners. Universiti Teknologi MARA (UiTM). Doctoral dissertation.

Montelongo, J., Berber-Jimenez, L., Hernandez, A.C., Hosking, D., 2006. Teaching expository text structures. Sci. Teach. 73 (2), 28e31. Moss, B., 2004. Teachng expository text structures through information trade book retellings. Reading Teach. 57 (8), 710e718. Nunan, D., 1993. Collaborative Language Learning and Teaching. Cambridge University Press, New York.

Oshima, A., Hogue, A., 2006. Writing Academic English. Pearson Longman, New York.

Ovsiannikov, I.A., Arbib, M.A., Mcneill, T.H., 1999. Annotation technology. Int. J. Human-Computer Stud. 50, 329e362. Oxford, R.L., 1990. Language Learning Strategies: What Every Teacher Should Know. Heinle & Heinle, Boston. Pardo, L.S., 2004. What every teacher needs to know about comprehension. Reading Teach. 58 (3), 272e280.

Rau, P.L.P., Chen, S.H., Chin, Y.T., 2004. Developing web annotation tools for learners and instructors. Interacting Comput. 16 (2), 163e181. Reutzel, D.R., 1985. Reconciling schema theory and the basal reading lesson. Reading Teach. 39 (2), 194e198.

Salvatori, M.R., 1996. Conversations with texts: reading in the teaching of composition. Coll. English 58, 440e454.

Schmar-Dobler, E., 2003. Reading on the Internet: the link between literacy and technology. J. Adolesc. Adult Literacy 47, 80e85.

Snell, J., 2011. Improving Teacher-student Interaction in the EFL Classroom: an Action Research Report. Retrieved July 26, 2011 from the World Wide Web:http://iteslj.org/Articles/Snell-Interaction.html(by Jonathan Snell).

Spolsky, B., 1989. Conditions for Second Language Learning: Introduction to a General Theory. Oxford University Press, Oxford. Wolfe, J., 2002. Annotation technologies: a software and research review. Comput. Compost. 19, 471e497.

Wolfe, J., Neuwirth, C.M., 2001. From the margins to the center-the future of annotation. J. Bus. Tech. Commun. 15 (3), 333e371.

數據

Fig. 1. Screenshot of Paragraph Annotator: Three steps to highlight a paragraph element.
Fig. 3. Screenshot of Paragraph Annotator: Two steps to use Dictionary.
Fig. 4. Screenshot of control group system.
Fig. 5. Difference of mean scores between experimental group and control group in Free recall test and in cued recall test.

參考文獻

相關文件

In addition to the new measures to be implemented in the 2020/21 school year set out in paragraph 5 above, the EDB will continue to provide public sector ordinary primary

Understanding and inferring information, ideas, feelings and opinions in a range of texts with some degree of complexity, using and integrating a small range of reading

Reading Task 6: Genre Structure and Language Features. • Now let’s look at how language features (e.g. sentence patterns) are connected to the structure

Teachers may encourage students to approach the poem as an unseen text to practise the steps of analysis and annotation, instead of relying on secondary

• Students annotate a text using an annotation tool that identifies their authorship. • Advantage: student annotations may

In the third paragraph, please write a 100-word paragraph to talk about what you’d do in the future to make this research better and some important citations if any.. Please help

In addition to the new measures to be implemented in the 2019/20 school year set out in paragraph 10 above, the EDB will continue to provide public sector ordinary primary

In the first paragraph, how does the writer convince us that many people think the classroom of the future will be based on technology.. (A) She describes