• 沒有找到結果。

Modeling motivation and habit in driving behavior under lifetime driver's license revocation

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Modeling motivation and habit in driving behavior under lifetime driver's license revocation"

Copied!
8
0
0

加載中.... (立即查看全文)

全文

(1)

ContentslistsavailableatSciVerseScienceDirect

Accident

Analysis

and

Prevention

jo u r n al h om ep ag e :w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e / a a p

Modeling

motivation

and

habit

in

driving

behavior

under

lifetime

driver’s

license

revocation

Chien-Ming

Tseng

a,∗

,

Hsin-Li

Chang

b,1

,

T.

Hugh

Woo

b,2

aMinistryofTransportationandCommunications,6ShengfuRoad,Chung-ShinVillage,NantouCity54045,Taiwan,ROC

bDepartmentofTransportationTechnologyandManagement,NationalChiaoTungUniversity,1001UniversityRoad,Hsinchu30010,Taiwan,ROC

a

r

t

i

c

l

e

i

n

f

o

Articlehistory:

Received8March2011

Receivedinrevisedform5November2012

Accepted20November2012

Keywords:

Licenserevocation

Drivingbehaviors Drivinghabit

Theplannedbehavioraltheory

Theorderedlogitmodel

a

b

s

t

r

a

c

t

Thepurposeofthepresentstudywastoverifythemotivationalfactorsunderlyingthetheoryofplanned behavior(TPB)predictingthedrivingbehavioroflifetimedrivinglicenserevokedoffenders.Ofatotalof

639driverswhoselicenseshadbeenpermanentlyrevoked,544offenderscompletedaquestionnaire

constructedtomeasureattitudestowardbehaviors,subjectivenorms,perceivedbehavioral control,

behavioralintentions(thekeyconstructsoftheTPB),andpreviousdrivinghabitstrength.Thefinding

ofthestudyrevealedthatanoffenders’drivingbehaviorafteralifetimelicenserevocationwas

sig-nificantlycorrelatedtobehavioralintention(R=0.60,p<0.01),perceivedbehavioralcontrol(R=0.61, p<0.01),previousdrivinghabit(R=0.44,p<0.01),andattitude(R=0.41,p<0.01).Therewasno evi-dencethatsubjectivenormsincludingroadregulation,societyethics,andpeopleimportanttooffenders

hadaninfluenceondrivingbehavior(R=0.03).Lowdrivinghabitstrengthoffendersaremotivatedto

drivebecauseofbehavioralintention,whereasstrongdrivinghabitstrengthoffendersaremotivated

todrivebecauseofperceivedbehavioralcontrol.Previousdrivinghabitstrengthisamoderatorinthe

intention–behaviorrelationship.Themodelappearedsuccessfulwhenprevioushabitswereweak,but

lesssuccessfulwhenprevioushabitswerestrong.

© 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction

Manydriversgivenasentenceoflicensesuspension/revocation

(S/R)continuetodrive,butatreducedlevels(Hagenetal.,1980;

Ross and Gonzales, 1988). Ingraham and Waller (1971) found

atleast30%ofdriversgivenS/Rfordrunk-drivingcontinuedto

operateavehicleinspiteofthelicensingaction.Williamsetal.

(1984) concluded that 65% of drivers admitted to operating a

vehicle after a license S/R. Ross and Gonzales (1988) reported

that66%ofthedriverswhoselicensingweresuspendedwerestill

driving.DeYoung(1999) estimatedthat as many as75% ofS/R

licenseddriverscontinuetodrive,althoughtheyapparentlydrove

lessoftenandmorecarefully.Malenfantetal.(2002)showedthe

percentageofmotoristsdrivingwhilesuspendedwas57%ofthe

expectedvalue,relativetotheirrepresentationamongalldrivers.

Changetal.(2006)foundthat86%ofoffenderscontinuedtodrive,

butwithsignificantlyreduceddrivingactivitiesandmileage.The

generalapproach ofS/R,a driver-based sanction,was intended

∗ Correspondingauthor.Tel.:+886427011026;fax:+886423588039.

E-mailaddresses:tsengcm168@yahoo.com.tw(C.-M.Tseng),

hlchang@cc.nctu.edu.tw(H.-L.Chang),thwoo@mail.nctu.edu.tw(T.H.Woo).

1 Tel.:+88635731908;fax:+88635712365.

2 Tel.:+88635731998;fax:+88635720844.

tokeep offenders off the road during theirrestrictionperiods.

InthecaseofshorttermS/R,althoughmanyS/Rdriversviolate

drivingrestrictionandcontinuetodrive,mostpriorresearchhas

focusedontheeffectivenessofS/Randsupportedtheviewthat

this is a positive step in reducing subsequentalcohol-involved

drivingbyoffenders(Zadoretal.,1989;HendersonandKedjidjian,

1992; Lund,1993; Sweedlerand Stewart, 1993; NHTSA, 1993).

However,fewstudieshaveexploredmotivationalfactorscausing

offenderstodrivewhile underlicenseS/R.Furthermore,as S/R

is usually awarded for nomore than a few years, few studies

haveexploredS/Rover alongerperiod oftime (Siskind, 1996).

Thus,itseemsthatnostudyhasexploredthemotivationalfactors

underlying the behavior of driving under a long-term S/R. In

thecase of administrative lifetimelicenserevocation (ALLR) in

Taiwan,thebasicgoalistomaintainroadsafetybykeepingsuch

disqualified drivers off theroad allowing them noopportunity

for rehabilitation in the licensing system design. Chang et al.

(2006)exploredtheeffectivenessofALLR;however,nostudyhas

exploredthemotivationalfactorsofdrivingwhileunderalifetime

license revocation. Therefore, the motivational or suppressive

factorsleadingtodriveunderALLRremainunclear.

Caruseisimportantformanyhouseholdactivitiesinwestern

developedsocietiesaswellasdevelopingcountriessuchasTaiwan.

Householdsusecarsfor travelingtovariousactivities(Eriksson

etal.,2008).Inthelastthreedecades,considerableprogresshas

0001-4575/$–seefrontmatter © 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

(2)

beenmadeinexplainingandpredictingtheinitiationofhuman

behaviorsasrevealedbycurrentlypopularattitude-behavior

mod-els(e.g.FishbeinandAjzen,1975;Ajzen,1991).Supportforthe

efficacyofthetheoryofplannedbehavior(TPB)hasbeen

success-fullyfoundinthecontextofcommondrivingbehaviorsrelatingto

roadsafetysuchasseatbeltuse(JonahandDawson,1982;Budd

etal.,1984;ThuenandRise,1994;S¸ims¸eko˘gluandLajunen,2008),

theuseofacarchildrestraintdevice(GodinandKok,1996),

pedes-trianroadcrossingbehavior(EvansandNorman,1998;Moyano

Díaz,2002),speeding(Parkeretal.,1992a;Forward,1997;Elliott etal.,2003; Letirandand Delhomme,2005; DePelsmacker and Janssens,2007;WarnerandÅberg,2008),drunk-driving(Parker etal.,1992a;Åberg,1993;Parkeretal.,1996;Sheehanetal.,1996; GordonandHunt,1998;Marciletal.,2001;Armitageetal.,2002),

aggressivedriving(Parkeretal.,1995,1998;MilesandJohnson,

2003),anddrivingviolations(Parkeretal.,1992b,c,1995;Forward,

2006).TPBhasbeenfoundtobeausefulmodelfororganizingand

understandingpotentialfactorsthatinfluenceintentiontoengage

insafedrivingbehaviorandlawcompliance(Yagil,1998;Gordon

andHunt,1998;Poulteretal.,2008).AnextendedTPBseemsa

valu-ableframeworkfor understandingand changingpeople’ssafety

relatedactionsintraffic(Victoiretal.,2005).

AccordingtoTPB, travelmodechoiceis determinedby

atti-tude,subjectivenorm,perceivedbehavioralcontrol,andbehavioral

intention (Ajzen, 1991; Bamberg and Schmidt, 2001, 2003).

Althoughtheseresearcheffortshaveexplainedmoreofthe

reason-based and deliberate natureof behavior, one important aspect

hasbeenoverlooked inresearch,namely,thefactthat manyof

theaforementionedbehaviorsareexecutedonadaily,repetitive

basis,andthereforemaybecomeroutineorhabitual.Lifeconsists

largelyofdailyroutines(HuffandHanson,1986;Pas,1988;Eagly

andChaiken,1993), andtravelmodechoicemayalsobe

deter-minedby habits(Bambergand Schmidt, 2003;Verplanken and

Orbel,2003).Therefore,itisproposedthatwhenbehavioris

per-formedrepeatedlyandbecomeshabitual,thatbehaviorisguidedby

anautomatedcognitiveprocess,ratherthanbeingprecededbyan

elaboratedecisionmakingprocess(i.e.adecisionbasedonattitudes

andintentions)(Aartsetal.,1998).Habithasbeenperceivedasan

automaticlinkbetweenagoalandaspecificbehavior(Verplanken

andAarts,1999;AartsandDijksterhuis,2000),orasabehavioral

scriptstoredinmemory(SchankandAbelson,1977;Abelson,1981;

Schank,1982;Gärlingetal.,2001;FujiiandGärling,2003).

Habit-ualbehavior,incontrasttomoredeliberatelycontrolledbehavior,

demandsonlya smallamountofattention,andtheindividual’s

control over behavioral intention aswell asthe behavior itself

isminor(Bargh,1994).Habitualcarusehasbeendemonstrated

inseveralstudies(Verplankenetal.,1994,1998;Erikssonetal.,

2008;Heath and Gifford,2002; Klöckneretal., 2003).A strong

habittouseaparticulartravelmodeis,incomparisonwithaweak

habit,characterizedbyseekinglessinformationandaless

elab-oratechoiceoftravelmode(Aartsetal.,1997).Verplankenetal.

(1998)foundthatbothhabitandintentionweresignificant

predic-torsofcaruseamongdriverswhowereencouragedtodeliberately

thinkabouttravelmodechoice.InaccordancewithTPB,researchers

expectedthatattitudestowardschoosingtouseone’scar,together

withsubjectivenormandperceivedbehavioralcontrol,would

pre-dictbehavioralintentions,whichinturnwereexpectedtopredict

futurebehavior.

Thepresentstudyisconcernedwiththemotivationalfactorsof

drivingbehaviorusingcomponentsofTPBaswellastheprevious

habitualnatureofdriversdrivingactivityunderALLRasan

exam-pleofarepetitivebehavior.ALLRmaybeanexampleofarepetitive

behaviorthatismotivatedbybehavioralintentionandprevious

drivinghabit.Thisstudyhastwogoals.Thefirstistoinvestigate

themotivationalfactorsthatcorrelatetoactualdrivingincidence

whileunder ALLR.The secondistoponder therole ofhabitin

attitude-behavior models. For these goals, the researchers

focusedontherelationshipsbetweenbehaviorandreason-based

antecedents(i.e.asdefinedbyTPB)ononehandandbehaviorand

habitstrengthontheotherhand.Thepresentstudyaimedto

exam-inethepsychologicalfactorspredictingtheactualdrivingbehavior

ofoffenderswhohadbeenpunishedbyALLR.Forthefirstgoal,

weadopttheTPBmodel,whichincludeattitude,subjectivenorm,

perceivedbehaviorcontrol,behavioralintention,previousdriving

habitandbehavior.Forthesecondgoal,theresearchersexplored

theTPBmodel’sabilitytopredictbehaviorunderdifferentdriving

habitstrength(i.e.stronghabit,moderatehabit,andweakhabit).

Itisexpectedthatoffenderswithstronghabitwillreporthigher

drivingbehaviorwhileunderALLR.ItisalsoexpectedthattheTPB

modelperformmorepredictabilityforweakhabitthanstronghabit

participants.

2. Method

2.1. Outlineoftheplannedbehaviormodelandprevioushabit

TheTPB,oradaptationsofit,isthemostoftenusedtheoretical

frameworkofmodelsexplainingtrafficsafetybehavior.Inpresent

study,theconceptualmodelisrepresentedinFig.1.TheTPB

con-tendsthatbehavioralintentionstoengageinabehavioristhemain

predictorofactualdrivingbehavior,andthatbehavioralintentions

areinfluencedbyattitudestowardsthatbehavior,subjectivenorms

(i.e.,whetherimportant otherswould approveor disapproveof

thebehavior)andperceivedbehavioralcontrol(Ajzen,1991).

Atti-tudesaregenerallydefinedasconsistingofcognitiveandaffective

componentsorantecedents.Inpresentstudy,offendersmay

ratio-nallyunderstandthatdrivingunderALLRisnotgoodorunsafe,

butatthesametimetheymayliketodrive,becauseitgivesthem

agoodfeelingortheyfeeldeservedtodrive.Subjectivenormis

partoftheTPB,andreflectstheoffenders’perceivedsocial

pres-sure(whatindividualsbelieveotherpeoplethinktheyshoulddo).

AccordingtotheTPB,theperceivedopinionofsignificantothers

(subjectivenorm)caninfluenceintentionsandbehavior.Perceived

behavioralcontrolrepresentsaneffectonintentiontoperforma

behaviorthatisnotmediatedbyattitudeorsubjectivenorm(Ajzen,

1988;AjzenandMadden,1986).Whilesomeresearchershave

sug-gestedthatcarusemaybehabitual(e.g.Verplankenetal.,1994;

BambergandSchmidt,2003),thepresentstudyhypothesisthat

habitformationleadsto‘automatic’thatmaydeliberatelyleadto

actualdrivingbehavior.Therefore,habitmayactasamoderatorof

theintention–behaviorrelationship.Andintentionsandbehavior

arealsodeterminedbyperceivedbehavioralcontrolandbyhabit

formation.

2.2. Dataandparticipants

DatawerecollectedfromapreviousALLRstudy(Changetal.,

2006)conductedinJuly2003,andafollow-upquestionnaires

sur-veyconductedthreemonthsafterthefirstresponses.Theprevious

ALLR studyinvestigated asample of 768volunteerALLR

offen-derswhohadbeeninvolvedineitherahit-and-runoffencecausing

death/orinjury,oradrunkdrivingoffencecausingdeath/orserious

injury from1993to2002in Taiwan.Only16.8%ofALLR

offen-dersgave up drivingentirely aftertheALLR was imposed.The

samplepopulationofthepresentstudyfocusedonthe83.2%of

ALLRoffenderscontinuingtodrive.SincetheseALLRoffenderswere

expelledfromtheDepartmentofMotorVehicles,objectiverecords

ofdrivingbehaviorsarenotavailable.Thefollow-upquestionnaires

weredirectlymailedtothese639stilldrivingALLRoffendersand

theirself-reportdatawerecollected.Afteratwo-wavetrialed

(3)

AT

SN

PBC

BI B

PDH

Fig.1. Theframeworkoftheplannedbehaviormodelandpreviousdrivinghabit.AT:attitude;SN:subjectivenorm;PBC:perceivedbehavioralcontrol;BI:behavioral

intention;PDH:previousdrivinghabit;B:behavior.

questionnaireswithareturnrateof88%.Finally,atotalof544ALLR

offenderseffectively completedthisfollow-upinvestigationand

usedinthisstudy.Theparticipantsconsistedof98.5%malesand

1.5%females.Over80%werenotcollegeeducated,and70%were

underage40.Ofthisgroupofparticipants,23.9%heldprofessional

driver’slicenses(professionallicenseisdefinedasalicensewhich

qualifiesa drivertodedicatedrivingasa job,includingdriving

acar,atruckorabusaccordingtothesedifferentdriving

vehi-cles’requirementsdistinctively)and76.1%heldordinarydriver’s

licenses.

Theself-reportdataonattitudes,subjectivenorms,perceived

behavioral control, behavioral intention, and previous driving

habitswerecollectedfromthepreviousstudy(Changetal.,2006).

Theactualdrivingbehaviorincludingoffenders’drivingfrequency

andannual mileage drivenunder ALLR wasinvestigatedin the

currentfollow-up survey.Allitemswerecodedusing a7-point

scale. Measures of attitudes, subjective norms, and behavioral

intentiontoward driving under ALLR wereconstructed

accord-ingto theprocedures employed byAjzen and Fishbein (1980),

whereasthemeasureofperceivedbehavioralcontrolwasdesigned

accordingtoAjzen’scollaborativework(AjzenandMadden,1986;

Ajzen,1991;BeckandAjzen,1991).Themeasureofactualdriving

behaviorwasself-reportedbytheparticipantsconcerningdriving

frequency.Self-reportdrivingfrequencywasdefinedfrom

very-highfrequencytovery-lowfrequency.

Table1outlinestheobservedvariablesandquestionitemsofthe

presentstudy.Themeasureofattitudeswerebasedonfour

seman-ticdifferentials(e.g.,goodor bad;safeorunsafe),and assessed

therespondent’spositiveornegativeevaluationofdrivingunder

ALLR.Thefouritemswereaveragedtoobtainthescalescore.The

internalconsistencyofthescalewas0.88.Thesubjectivenorms

weremeasuredbysixquestionsassessingtherespondent’s

eval-uationofhisreferentopinionsregardingactualdrivingbehavior

in the described situation. The averageof the six items

corre-spondstothescoreofthisscale.Cronbach’salphawas0.82.The

perceivedbehavioral controlconstructincludedfouritems that

wereaveragedtoobtainthescalescore. Thismeasureassessed

therespondent’sperceptionofcontroloverdrivingafterALLR.The

internalconsistencyofthisscaleaccordingtoCronbach’salphawas

0.93.Themeasureofbehavioralintentionincludedtwoitems

mea-suringthestrengthoftherespondent’sintentiontodriveafterALLR.

Responsestothetwoitemswereaveragedtoobtainthescoreon

thescale.InternalconsistencyasmeasuredbyCronbach’salpha

was0.92. Theresearchersoperationalizedthemeasurement for

habitstrengthsothat themeasurement would matchthe

con-ceptofageneralizedtypeofhabit.Thus,theresponse-frequency

(RF)measureofgeneralhabitstrengthwhichwasusedand

vali-datedinanumberofearlierstudies(Aarts,1996;Aartsetal.,1997;

Verplankenetal.,1998)wasadopted.WhileVerplankenandAarts

(1999)suggestedthatanimportantfeatureofhabitualbehavior

isautomaticityofresponding,presentparticipantsrespondedto

10itemsthatvariedwidelyintravelpurposes(e.g.goingtothe

market,visitingfriends/orrelatives).Theresearchersassumedthat

themoreinvariantparticipants’responseswerethestrongerthe

habitstrengthwasthusindexedbythemeanofthe10items.The

measureof habitstrength hadhighinternal consistency

(Cron-bach’salpha=0.94)whichrevealedthereliabilityisgood.Inorder

tojustifythe variationis a good representationof thehabitual

behavior.Theresearchersfurthertoconfirmweatherthevalidity

isacceptableornot.Afterconductingthe“Principalcomponents

analyses”,atwo-dimensionalstructureappeared.Thereweretwo

eigenvaluesgreaterthan1(6.601and1.070).Thefirstcomponent

includinghabitmeasureitemsfrom1to8accountedfor55.803%

ofthevarianceandthesecondcomponentincludinghabit

mea-sureitemsfrom9to10accountedfor 20.907%ofthevariance,

totallyaccountedfor76.710%ofthevariance.Finally,itisconcluded

thatthisvariationinpresentstudyisagoodrepresentationofthe

habitualbehavior.

3. Results

3.1. CorrelationsofmotivationaldrivingfactorsunderALLR–

combinetheplannedbehaviortheoryandhabit

Correlationswerecalculated betweendriving attitudes,

sub-jectivenorms,perceivedbehavioralcontrol,behavioralintention,

previousdrivinghabit,andactualdrivingbehaviormeasures.These

correlationsareshowninTable2.Theresultsindicatethatactual

drivingbehaviorwascorrelatedwithattitude(R=0.41, p<0.01),

perceivedbehavioralcontrol(R=0.61,p<0.01),previousdriving

habit(R=0.44,p<0.01),andbehavioralintention(R=0.60,p<0.01).

Amongthesesignificantcorrelativevariables,perceivedbehavioral

controlandbehavioralintentionwerethetwomostimportant

fac-torscorrelatedwithactualdrivingbehavior.Theresultsindicate

thattherespondentsthinkdrivingacarisaneed,andwhenever

needingtodrive,respondentswilldriveevenifalicensehadbeen

revoked.Also,mostrespondentsthinkdrivingunderALLRiseasy

andpossible.Attitudeandpreviousdrivinghabitwerealso

signifi-cantlycorrelatedwithactualdrivingbehavior.Theresultsindicated

thatbehavioralintentionwassignificantlycorrelatedwithattitude

(R=0.36,p<0.01),perceivedbehavioralcontrol(R=0.65,p<0.01),

andpreviousdrivinghabit(R=0.47,p<0.01)suchthatthe

respon-dentswhoreportedexperiencingamorepositiveaffect,moreneed

todrive,andhigherpriordrivinghabitstrengthoverdrivingunder

ALLR,alsoreportedstrongerintentions.However,theresults

indi-catethatsubjectivenormsarenotrelatedtobehavioralintention

(R=0.07)aswellasactualdrivingbehavior(R=0.03).Nomatter

(4)

Table1

Observedvariablesandquestionitems(N=544).

Factors Item Cronbach’s

alpha

Questions Scoring

Behavior 1 – Althoughyourlicensewasrevoked,youdrovewith frequencyafterALLR. 7=veryhigh,1=verylow Behavioralintention 2 0.92 Althoughyourlicensewasrevoked,you havetodriveinthefuture. 7=extremelylikely,

1=extremelyunlikely Althoughyoulicensewasrevoked,you willdriveinthefuture.

Attitude 4 0.88 YouthinktodriveunderALLRis good. 7=extremelylikely,

1=extremelyunlikely YouthinktodriveunderALLRis convenient.

YouthinktodriveunderALLRis deserved. YouthinktodriveunderALLRis safe.

Subjectivenorm 6 0.82 Mostpeopleimportanttoyouwould agreeyoutodrive. 7=extremelylikely, 1=extremelyunlikely Thosewhoimportanttoyou,theiropinionhave theinfluenceonyou.

Yoursocieties agreeyoutodriveunderALLR.

Yoursocietieshave influenceonyoufordrivingunderALLR. Regulations agreeyoutodrive.

Regulations influenceyoutodrive.

Perceivedbehavioralcontrol 3 0.93 DrivingunderALLRis easyforyou. 7=extremelylikely, 1=extremelyunlikely DrivingunderALLRis possibleforyou.

Wheneveryouhavetogosomewhere,youwill choosedrivingacar.

Previousdrivinghabita 10 0.94 You driveacartogoaroundsomewheremarket. 7=extremelylikely,

1=extremelyunlikely You driveacartovisityourfriends/orrelatives.

You driveacarforshopping.

You driveacartohaveyourdinner.

You driveacartotransportyourkidsorfamilies.

You driveacartogoaroundsomewheredowntown.

You driveacarforleisureactivitiesonweekdays. You driveacarforleisureactivitiesonweekends.

You driveacarforcommutingtowork.

You driveacarforbusiness.

aParticipantswererequestedtoresponseassoonaspossible,andnotthinktoomuchortaketoomuchtimetorespond.

theseperceptionshadnoinfluenceonbothbehavioralintention

andactualdrivingbehavior.

3.2. MotivationaldrivingfactorsunderALLR

According tothe planned behavior theory, driving behavior

under ALLR should be predicted by motivational components.

Giventhatthedependentvariable“drivingfrequencyunderALLR”

hasanordinalnature,withsevenpossibleoutcomes(from1=very

lowto7=veryhigh).Ordinaryleastsquaresareseldomappropriate

forsuchdatasinceitrequiresacontinuousdependentvariable.An

ordered-responsemodelappearsthemostappropriateapproach.

Theordered-discretechoicemodels(i.e.,theorderedlogitmodes)

havebeenemployedbyseveralresearchers(see,forexample,Pai

and Saleh, 2008; Neyens and Boyle,2008)for modeling injury

severities.Therefore,theorderedlogitmodelwasadoptedto

esti-matethemotivationalfactorsofdrivingbehaviorunderALLR.The

theoreticalframeworkoftheorderedlogitmodelandmethodof

evaluationhasthoroughlydiscussedinseveralstudies(e.g.,Long,

1997; Borooah, 2001). The actual driving behavior under ALLR

was modeled as a function of the attitudes, subjective norms,

perceivedbehavioral control, behavioralintention and previous

driving habits, wascreated and verified the motivational

driv-ingfactorsunderALLR.Theresearchersexaminedwhetheractual

drivingbehaviorformedasa resultofthevariables ofTPB.The

results presented in Table 3 indicate that attitudes, perceived

behaviorcontrol,behavioralintentionandpreviousdrivinghabits

accountforaround49%(PseudoR2=0.487)ofthetotalvariancein

actualdrivingbehaviors.Acloserlookattheseresultsindicatedthat

behavioralintention(ˇ=0.718,p=0.000)andperceivedbehavioral

control(ˇ=0.651,p=0.000)arethetwomostimportantfactors

inthedeterminationofactualdrivingbehavior.Previousdriving

habits(ˇ=0.141,p=0.049)andattitudes(ˇ=0.266,p=0.004)also

significantlyinfluenceactualdrivingbehavior.Theresultsindicate

thatsubjectivenorms(ˇ=−0.022,p=0.841)arenotsignificantly

associatedwithactualdrivingbehavior.

3.3. MotivationaldrivingfactorsunderALLR– TPBmodelwith

differentdrivinghabitstrengths

Inthissection,theeffectsoftheTPBmodelwhileunder

vari-ousdrivinghabitstrengthsareexplored.Previousstudiesclassified

participant’shabitstrengthintodifferentlevelsmainlybyuseof

theparticipants’standarddeviation(i.e.atonestandarddeviation

belowthemean:weakornohabit;atthemean:moderatehabit;

andatonestandarddeviationabovethemean:stronghabit;Aiken

andWest,1991;Gardner,2009).Inthepresentstudy,the

respon-dent’shabitstrengthwasdividedbysequenceintothreegroups

Table2

Correlationamongtheoryofplannedbehaviorvariables(N=544).

Studyvariable Mean S.D. 1 2 3 4 5 6

1.Behavior 4.7 1.9 –

2.Behavioralintention 6.1 1.2 0.60**

3.Attitudes 3.8 1.0 0.41** 0.36**

4.Subjectivenorms 3.9 0.7 0.03 0.07 −0.04 –

5.Perceivedbehavioralcontrol 5.7 1.4 0.61** 0.65** 0.43** 0.002

6.Previousdrivinghabits 4.8 1.4 0.44** 0.47** 0.49** −0.023 0.47**

(5)

Table3

Orderedlogitmodelestimationresults:predictionofbehaviorbyintension,attitudes,subjectivenorms,PBCandpreviousdrivinghabits–allparticipants,N=544,average

annualmileage=14,370km.

Variables ˇ S.E. Wald Significance(P) 95%C.I.

Intension 0.718*** 0.098 53.782 0.000 0.526to0.910

Attitudes 0.266*** 0.093 8.126 0.004 0.083to0.448

Subjectivenorms −0.022 0.110 0.040 0.841 −0.237to0.193

PBC 0.651*** 0.084 60.730 0.000 0.487to0.815

Previousdrivinghabits 0.141* 0.072 3.851 0.049 0.000to0.282

NagelkerkePseudoR2=0.487;Cox&SnellPseudoR2=0.475;Log-likelihood:1674.296;ˇ:regressioncoefficients;S.E.:standarderrorofˇ. * p<0.05.

**p<0.01. ***p<0.001.

Table4

Orderedlogitmodelestimationresults: prediction ofbehavior byintension, attitudes,subjectivenorms andPBC– highhabitstrength, n=180,averageannual

mileage=16,945km.

Variables ˇ S.E. Wald Significance(P) 95%C.I.

Intension 0.439* 0.205 4.571 0.033 0.037to0.841

Attitudes 0.136 0.150 0.820 0.365 −0.158to0.431

Subjectivenorms −0.038 0.166 0.052 0.819 −0.364to0.288

PBC 0.563*** 0.145 15.067 0.000 0.279to0.847

NagelkerkePseudoR2=0.241;Cox&SnellPseudoR2=0.231;Log-likelihood:439.195;ˇ:regressioncoefficients;S.E.:standarderrorofˇ. * p<0.05.

**p<0.01. ***p<0.001.

Table5

Orderedlogitmodelestimationresults:predictionofbehaviorbyintension,attitudes,subjectivenormsandPBC–moderatehabitstrength,n=184,averageannual

mileage=13,258km.

Variables ˇ S.E. Wald Significance(P) 95%C.I.

Intension 0.782*** 0.168 21.713 0.000 0.453to1.112

Attitudes 0.579** 0.184 9.917 0.002 0.219to0.940

Subjectivenorms −0.087 0.181 0.230 0.631 −0.442to0.268

PBC 1.046*** 0.177 35.076 0.000 0.700to1.392

NagelkerkePseudoR2=0.420;Cox&SnellPseudoR2=0.407;Log-likelihood:614.850;ˇ:regressioncoefficients;S.E.:standarderrorofˇ. *p<0.05.

** p<0.01. ***p<0.001.

witheachgroupcontainingasimilarnumberofrespondents.The

reasonfor this isthat thegroupingwillreduce thevariance of

therespondents’habitstrength.Iftheseresultsrepresenthabit

strengthinfluenceonbehavior,thentherelationshipofhabitand

behavior can be more easily conceived. Therefore, all

respon-dentswereclassifiedintothreehabitstrengthgroupsincluding

a high driving habit strength group, a moderate driving habit

strengthgroup,andaweakdrivinghabitstrengthgroup.Tables4–6

revealtheactualdrivingbehaviorsregressedonattitude,perceived

behavioralcontrol,andbehavioralintentionforthethreedriving

habitstrengthgroups.Forthehighhabitstrengthgroup,perceived

behavioralcontrol(ˇ=0.563,p=0.000)isthemostimportant

fac-torthatinfluencedactualdrivingbehavior.However,forthelow

habitstrengthgroup,behavioralintention(ˇ=0.822,p=0.000)is

themostdeterminantfactoronactualdrivingbehavior.Attitudes

aresignificantly associatedwiththeparticipants’actualdriving

behaviorformoderatehabitstrength(ˇ=0.579,p=0.002)andlow

habitstrength(ˇ=0.401,p=0.009)groups.Therewasnoevidence

thatsubjectivenormshaveaninfluenceontherespondents’

driv-ingbehaviornomatterwhetherdrivinghabitsarestrongornot

(ˇ=−0.038,p=0.819;ˇ=−0.087,p=0.631;ˇ=212,p=0.412for

strong,moderateandlowhabitstrength,respectively).

The strength of the associations with actual driving

behav-ior was correlated with perceived behavior control, behavioral

intention and attitude, but not the norms in all three habit

strengthgroups.Theassociationbetweenbehavioralintentionand

actualdrivingbehaviorshouldbecomeweakerwhiledrivinghabit

strengthwashigh.Theassociationbetweenbehavioralintention

Table6

Orderedlogitmodelestimation results: prediction ofbehavior by intension,attitudes, subjectivenorms and PBC– low habitstrength,n=180,averageannual

mileage=4,349km.

Variables ˇ S.E. Wald Significance(P) 95%C.I.

Intension 0.822*** 0.153 29.013 0.000 0.523to1.121

Attitudes 0.401** 0.154 6.786 0.009 0.099to0.703

Subjectivenorms 0.212 0.258 0.674 0.412 −0.294to0.718

PBC 0.469*** 0.128 13.466 0.000 0.218to0.719

NagelkerkePseudoR2=0.593;Cox&SnellPseudoR2=0.582;Log-likelihood:566.727;ˇ:regressioncoefficients;S.E.:standarderrorofˇ. *p<0.05.

** p<0.01. ***p<0.001.

(6)

andactualdrivingbehaviorbecomestrongerwhilehabitstrength

wasweak.WhiletakingacloserlookatthePseudoR2ofthethree

models,0.241,0.420and0.593forhigh,moderateandlowhabit

strengthgroup,respectively,theTPBmodelaccountsforahigher

percentageofthetotalvarianceoftheactualdrivingbehaviorwhile

previoushabitstrengthwasweak,andviceversa.

4. Discussion

Whilefacingaserioustrafficviolationproblem,traffic

authori-tiescommonlyadoptadeterrentofincreasingthepenaltyforsuch

serioustrafficoffence.Thepenaltiesincludecar-basedsanctions

and driver-basedsanctions.In driver-basedsanctions,themost

seriouspenaltyforanoffenderisalifetimedriver’slicense

revo-cation.Suchadeterrenthastheaimofmaintainingroadsafetyby

suspendingserioustrafficoffenders,andofferingnoopportunity

fortheoffendertore-enterthelicensingsystemevenwhenthey

demonstrateanabilityandwillingnesstofollowtheregulations

ofroadandsociety.However,inamodernsocietymanyeconomic

andsocialactivitiesrelyondrivingavehicletofulfillliving

pur-poses.Manypreviousstudies,basedonashort-termlicenseS/R,

havefoundthatdriverscontinuedtodrivebutatreducedlevels

(Hagenetal.,1980;RossandGonzales,1988;SmithandMaisey, 1990;Voasand DeYoung,2002).In thecaseoflifetimedriver’s

licenserevocation,driversareinthesamesituationasashort-term

licenseS/R,andhavetoseekanalternativeefficientwayofgoing

towork,shopping,visitingrelatives/friends,andforleisure

pur-poses.Suchanalternativesystemoftransportationmustcontinue

forarelativelylongertime,insomecases,therestoflife.Toendure

suchalongalternativesystemoftransportationismoredifficult

thanthatofashortersuspensionperiod.Therefore,thehypothesis

thattheratiooflifetimelicenserevocationoffenderscontinuingto

driveisgreaterthantheratioforashort-termlicenserevocationis

alogicalassumption.Changetal.(2006)foundthatlifetimedriver’s

licenserevocationoffenderswhocontinuedtodrive(83.2%)were

higherthanthatinpreviousfindingsallowingtheconclusionthat

complyingwithashort-termlicenseS/Rmayberelativelyeasy

formostpeoplewhilealifetimesuspensionofdrivingprivileges

maybetoomuchfordriverstoendure.However,thereisalmost

nostudyexploredthemotivationalfactorinfluenceonoffender’s

driving,nomatterunderashort-termS/R,along-termlicenseS/R

orlifetimelicenserevocation.PresentstudyappliedTPBmodelas

wellashabitstrengthtospecificinstancesofaberrantbehaviorand

successfullyexploredthemotivationalfactors,whichinfluence

off-enderstodriveavehicleaftertheirlicenseshadbeenrevokedfor

lifelong.

Thepresent studyresultsrevealedthatthesignificant

moti-vational factors correlated to driving afterALLR are behavioral

intention(R=0.61), perceivedbehavioral control (R=0.60),

pre-viousdrivinghabit(R=0.44),andattitude(R=0.41); theformer

two factorshavingthehighest andthe secondhighest

influen-tialroles.ThisfindingisconsistentwithwhattheTPBcontends

inthatintentions toengageinabehavioristhemainpredictor

of actualbehavior, and behavioral intentions are influenced by

attitudestowardsthatbehavior, subjectivenorms(i.e.,whether

importantotherswouldapproveordisapproveofthebehavior),

and perceived behavioral control (Ajzen, 1991). Empiricaltests

ofintention–behavior correlativestudiesshowthat intentionis

reliablyassociatedwithbehavior(i.e.,WebbandSheeran,2006).

Armitageand Conner (2001),in a meta-analysisof 185studies

thathaveusedtheTPB,alsofoundthatthesample-weighted

aver-agecorrelationbetweenmeasuresofintentionandbehaviorwas

0.47.Theintention–behaviorcorrelationofthepresentALLRcaseis

higherthantheaverageofpreviousfindings.Moreover,thepresent

studyisbasedonoffendersofserioustrafficoffences.

Perceivedbehavioralcontrolisthesecondstrongestinfluential

factormotivatingactualdrivingbehaviorafterALLR.Presentresults

indicatedthatrespondentsdeemdrivingafterALLRisaneed,and

whendriversperceiveaneedtodriveordesiretoaccomplish

liv-ingactivitiesviadrivingavehicle,respondentswilldrive.Attitudes

werealsoasignificantfactorinfluencingactualdrivingbehavior.

ManyrespondentsthinkdrivingunderALLRissafeandconvenient

andcontinuetodrive.Attitudeusuallyplaysanimportantrolein

caruseandtravelmodechoiceasthefindingsofthisstudyindicate

andareconsistentwithpreviousfindings(Ajzen,1991;Tertoolen

etal.,1998).Thepresentstudyisalsoconsistentwiththefindings

ofpreviousstudiesinthattherateoftrafficlawviolationsis

influ-encedbytheleveloflawenforcement,andaslongastheperceived

threatofdetection remainslow, trafficviolatorsareunlikely to

changebehavior(Harper,1991;Yagil,1998).Subjectivenormsare

notstronglycorrelatedwithactualdrivingbehavior(R=0.03)and

behavioralintention(R=0.07).Roadregulations,socialethics,and

importantpeopletooffendershavealmostnoinfluenceon

choos-ingtooperateavehicle.Thisfindingisdifferentfromotherprevious

trafficsafetystudies,e.g.LajunenandRäsänen(2004),whofound

thatthesubjectivenormwasthestrongestpredictorofthe

inten-tionto usea bicycle helmet.This findingisalsodifferent from

Manstead’s(2000)reviewof20studiesfindingonlythree

repor-tingnoempiricalsupportfortheindependentpredictiveutility

ofpersonalnorm.Thepresentmodelbetterpredictstheseresults

thanpreviousstudies,whichpredictbetween23%and47%ofthe

variance(Parkeretal.,1992a,1995).

Specifically,for thelow drivinghabitgroup,themost

moti-vational factor influencing drivers to drive after an ALLR was

behavioralintention,whereasforthestrongdrivinghabitgroup,

the most influential factor influencing drivers to drive was

perceivedbehavioralcontrol.Behavioralintentioninfluencedthe

lowdrivinghabitgroupmorethanthehighdrivinghabitgroup.

Incontrast,perceivedbehavioralcontrolinfluencedthehighand

moderatedrivinghabitgroupsmorethan thelow drivinghabit

group.TheresultsofthisstudyareconsistentwiththeTPB

empha-sisofthereason-basedantecedentsofbehavior.However,when

behavior is repeatedlyand satisfactorily executedand becomes

habitual,itmayloseitsreasonedcharacter.Behaviormaythenbe

moreguidedbytheautomaticityofstimulus–responserelations

andlessbyintentions(Verplankenetal.,1998),therefore,

inten-tionsareassumedtopredictbehaviortotheextentthatthehabit

componentisweak,andnot,ortoalesserdegree,whenhabitis

strong(Triandis,1977).

AccordingtoWoodandcolleagues(OuelletteandWood,1998;

Woodetal.,2002;WoodandQuinn,2005),behaviorsthatare

per-formedfrequentlyinstablecontextssupportthedevelopmentof

habits,and thus theimpactofintention onbehavioris

attenu-ated.Ameta-analysisbyOuelletteandWood(1998)showedthat

when behavior is practiced repeatedly and thecontext of

per-formance isstable,past behavioris a betterpredictor offuture

behavior than is intentionwhereas the reversewas truewhen

behaviorswereperformedinfrequentlyinunstablecontexts.

Sim-ilarly,Verplankenetal.(1998)foundaninteractionbetweenhabit

andintentionsuchthatintentionswereonlysignificantlyrelated

tobehaviorwhenhabitstrengthwasweak.Whenparticipants

pos-sessedstronghabits,intentionshadlessinfluenceonsubsequent

behavior(seealsoFergusonandBibby,2002;Klöckneretal.,2003);

seeAjzen(2002)foradifferentview.Thus,whetherbehaviorshave

thepotentialtobecontrolledbyhabitcouldbeanimportant

mod-eratorofintention–behaviorrelations.Thepresentstudyalsofound

drivinghabitstrengthwasamoderatorintheintention–behavior

relation,moreover, themodel appeared relatively successfulin

modelingdrivingbehaviorunderALLRwhenprevioushabitswere

weak,whereas lesssuccessfulinmodelingwhenprevioushabit

(7)

5. Conclusions

PreviousresearchfoundthattheALLRpolicyeffectively

influ-enced offenders to drive less frequently, fewer miles, more

carefully,moredefensively(Changetal.,2006)andlesscrashrisk

(Changetal.,2011).Thepresentstudyidentifiedthemotivational

factorsleadingoffenderstodrivewitharevokedlicensed.However,

theprivilegetodriveisvalued, andwithdrawaloftheprivilege

feared(DeYoungand Gebers,2004).Evenfor a short-termS/R,

one-fifthoftheUSstatesrejectedtheadoptionofadministrative

S/Rbecauseitcouldleadtolossofemployment,inturn

impact-ingtheoffender’sdependentsandsubsequentsocialwelfarecosts

(KnoebelandRoss,1997;VoasandDeYoung,2002).ALLRmaynot

beimplementedindevelopedcountries;however,itmaybe

imple-mentedindevelopingcountries.FuturestudyemploysTPBmodel

orothermeasuringmethodsmayconfirmpresentresults.In2001,

theTaiwanConstitutionalCourtaskedthetransportationauthority

toreconsiderwhetherALLRoffendersshouldbeallowedtore-enter

thelicensingsystemiftheycandemonstrateanabilityand

willing-nesstofollowtheregulationsoftheroadandsociety.In2006,the

ALLRpolicywasrevisedbyalicenserevocationfor8,10,or 12

yearsaccordingtothesameoffencecausinginjury,seriousinjury,

ordeath,respectively.

Acknowledgements

Thisstudy was approved by the Ministryof Transportation

andCommunication,Taiwanandconductedwiththeassistanceof

DepartmentofMotorVehicles;participantswerevolunteersand

withoutpayment; financewassupportedbytheDepartment of

TransportationTechnologyandManagement,NationalChiaoTung

University.

References

Aarts,H.,1996.Habitanddecisionmaking:thecaseoftravelmodechoice. Unpub-lishedDoctoralDissertation,UniversityofNijmegen,TheNetherlands. Aarts,H.,Verplanken,B.,VanKnippenberg,A.,1997.Habitandinformationusein

travelmodechoices.ActaPsychologica96,1–14.

Aarts,H.,Verplanken,B.,VanKnippenberg,A.,1998.Predictingbehaviorfrom

actionsinthepast:repeateddecisionmakingoramatterofhabit?Journalof

AppliedSocialPsychology28(15),1355–1374.

Aarts,H.,Dijksterhuis,A.,2000.Habitsasknowledgestructures:automaticityin goal-directedbehavior.JournalofPersonalityandSocialPsychology78,53–63. Abelson,R.P.,1981.Psychologicalstatusofthescriptconcept.AmericanPsychologist

36,715–729.

Åberg,L.,1993.Drinkinganddriving:intentions,attitudes,andsocialnormsof

Swedishmaledrivers.AccidentAnalysisandPrevention25,289–296.

Aiken,L.S.,West,S.G.,1991.Multipleregression:testingandinterpreting interac-tions.SagePublications,NewburyPark.

Ajzen,I.,1988.Attitudes,personalityandbehavior.OpenUniversityPress,Milton

Keynes,UK.

Ajzen,I.,1991.Thetheoryofplannedbehaviour.OrganizationalBehaviourand

HumanDecisionProcesses50,179–211.

Ajzen,I.,2002.Residualeffectsofpastonlaterbehavior:habituationandreasoned actionperspectives.PersonalityandSocialPsychologyReview6,107–122. Ajzen,I.,Fishbein,M.,1980.UnderstandingAttitudesandPredictingSocialBehavior.

Prentice-Hall,NewJersey.

Ajzen,I., Madden,T.J., 1986. Predictionof goal-directedbehaviour: attitudes,

intentions,andperceivedbehaviouralcontrol.JournalofExperimentalSocial

Psychology22,453–474.

Armitage,C.J.,Conner,M.,2001.Efficacyofthetheoryofplannedbehaviour:a meta-analyticreview.BritishJournalofSocialPsychology40,471–499.

Armitage,C.J.,Norman,P.,Conner,M.,2002.Canthetheoryofplannedbehaviour mediatetheeffectsofage,genderandmultidimensionalhealthlocusofcontrol? BritishJournalofHealthPsychology7,299–316.

Bamberg,S.,Schmidt,P.,2001.Theory-drivensubgroup-specificevaluationofan

interventiontoreduceprivatecaruse.JournalofAppliedSocialPsychology31, 1300–1329.

Bamberg,S.,Schmidt,P.,2003.Incentives,moralityorhabit?Predictingstudents’ caruseforuniversityrouteswiththemodelsofAjzen,SchwartzandTriandis.

EnvironmentandBehavior35,264–285.

Bargh,J.A.,1994.Environmentalcontrolofgoal-directedaction:automaticand

strategiccontingenciesbetweensituationsandbehavior.In:Nebraska

Sympo-siumonMotivation,vol.41.NewYorkUniversity,USA,pp.71–124.

Beck,L.,Ajzen,I.,1991.Predictingdishonestactionsusingthetheoryofplanned behaviour.JournalofResearchinPersonality25,285–301.

Borooah,V.,2001.Logitandprobit:orderedandmultinomialmodels.In:Sage Uni-versityPapersSeriesonQuantitativeApplicationintheSocialSciences07-138.

SagePublications,ThousandOaks,CA.

Budd,R.J.,North,D.,Spencer,C.,1984.Understandingseat-beltuse:atestofBentler andSpeckart’sextensionofthetheoryofreasonedaction.EuropeanJournalof SocialPsychology14,69–78.

Chang,H.L.,Woo,T.H.,Tseng,C.M.,2006.Isrigorouspunishmenteffective?Acase studyoflifetimelicenserevocationinTaiwan.AccidentAnalysisandPrevention

38,269–276.

Chang,H.L.,Woo,T.H.,Tseng,C.M.,Tseng,I.Y.,2011.Drivingbehaviorsand acci-dentriskunderlifetimelicenserevocation.AccidentAnalysisandPrevention

43,1385–1391.

DePelsmacker,P.,Janssens,W.,2007.Theeffectsofnormsattitudesandhabits

onspeedingbehavior:scaledevelopmentandmodelbuildingandestimation.

AccidentAnalysisandPrevention39,6–15.

DeYoung,D.J.,1999. Anevaluationofthespecific deterrenteffectsof vehicle

impoundmentonsuspended,revoked,andunlicenseddriversinCalifornia. Acci-dentAnalysisandPrevention31,45–53.

DeYoung,D.J.,Gebers,M.A.,2004.Anexaminationofthecharacteristicsand traf-ficrisksofdriverssuspended/revokedfordifferentreasons.JournalofSafety

Research35,287–295.

Eagly,A.H.,Chaiken,S.,1993.ThePsycologyofAttitudes.HarcourtBraceJovanovich, FortWorth,TX.

Elliott,M.A.,Armitage,C.J.,Baughan,C.J.,2003.Drivers’compliancewithspeed

limits:anapplicationofthetheoryofplannedbehavior.JournalofApplied

Psychology88,964–972.

Eriksson,L.,Garvill,J.,Nordlund,A.M.,2008.Interruptinghabitualcaruse:the impor-tanceofcarhabitstrengthandmoralmotivationforpersonalcarusereduction. TransportationResearchPartF11,10–23.

Evans,D.,Norman,P.,1998.Understandingpedestrians’roadcrossingdecisions:an applicationofthetheoryofplannedbehaviour.HealthEducationResearch13, 481–489.

Ferguson,E.,Bibby,P.A.,2002.Predictingfutureblooddonorreturns:pastbehavior, intentions,andobservereffects.HealthPsychology21,513–518.

Fishbein,M.,Ajzen,I.,1975.Belief,Attitude,Intention,andBehavior:AnIntroduction

toTheoryandResearch.Addison-Wesley,Reading.

Forward,S.E.,1997.Measuringattitudesandbehaviourusingthetheoryofplanned behaviour.In:Rothengattter,T.,Vaya,E.C.(Eds.),Traffic&TransportPsychology.

Pergamon,NY,pp.353–365.

Forward,S.E.,2006.Theintentiontocommitdrivingviolations-Aqualitativestudy. TransportationResearchPartF9,412–426.

Fujii,S.,Gärling,T.,2003.Developmentofscript-basedtravelmodechoiceafter forcedchange.TransportationResearchPartF6,117–124.

Gardner,B.,2009.Modellingmotivationandhabitinstabletravelmodecontexts. TransportationResearchPartF12,68–76.

Gärling,T.,Fijii,S.,Boe,O.,2001.Empiricaltestsofamodelofdeterminantsof script-baseddrivingchoice.TransportationResearchPartF4,89–102.

Godin,G.,Kok,G.,1996.Thetheoryofplannedbehavior:areviewofits

applica-tionstohealth-relatedbehaviors.AmericanJournalofHealthPromotion11,

87–98.

Gordon,C.,Hunt,M.,1998.Thetheoryofplannedbehaviorappliedtospeeding,

drink-drivingandseatbeltwearing.In:RoadSafetyConferenceProceedings. Hagen,R.E.,McConnell,E.J.,Williams,R.L.,1980.Suspensionandrevocationeffects

ontheDUIoffender.CaliforniaDepartmentofMotorVehicles,Sacramento,CA. Harper,J.G.,1991.Trafficviolationdetectionanddeterrence:implicationsfor

auto-maticpolicing.AppliedErgonomics22,189–197.

Heath,Y.,Gifford,R.,2002.Extendingthetheoryofplannedbehavior:

predic-tiontheuseofpublictransportation.JournalofAppliedSocialPsychology32, 2154–2189.

Henderson,A.,Kedjidjian,C.,1992.Administrativelicenserevocation–arewe driv-ingdrunksofftheroad?TrafficSafety,6–8.

Huff,J.O.,Hanson,S.,1986.Repetitionandvariabilityinurbantravel.Geographical Analysis22,70–93.

Ingraham,W.S.,Waller,J.A.,1971.Alcohol-impaireddriving,licensesuspensions,

andtransportationneedsduringintoxicationorsuspensionamongalcoholics.

CrashReportNo.IV-1,DepartmentofMentalHealth,Waterbury.

Jonah,B.A.,Dawson,N.E.,1982.Predictingseatbeltusefromattitudinaland

nor-mativefactors.AccidentAnalysisandPrevention14,305–310.

Klöckner,C.A.,Matthies,E.,Hunecke,M.,2003.Problemsofoperationalizinghabits andintegratinghabitsinnormativedecision-makingmodels.JournalofApplied

SocialPsychology33,396–417.

Knoebel,K.Y.,Ross,H.L.,1997. Effectsof administrativelicenserevocationon

employment.AccidentAnalysisandPrevention29(5),595–611.

Lajunen,T.,Räsänen,M.,2004.Cansocialpsychologicalmodelsbeusedtopromote

bicyclehelmetuseamongteenagers?AcomparisonoftheHealthBeliefModel,

TheoryofPlannedBehaviorandtheLocusofControl.JournalofSafetyResearch

35,115–123.

Letirand,F.,Delhomme,P.,2005.Speedbehaviourasachoicebetweenobserving

andexceedingthespeedlimit.TransportationResearchPartF8,481–492. Long,J.S.,1997.RegressionModelsforCategoricalandLimitedDependentVariables.

SagePublications,ThousandOaks,CA.

Lund,A.K.,1993. Effectivenessofadministrativelicenserevocation(ALR)laws.

TransportationResearchBoardCircular,No.413TransportationResearchBoard,

(8)

Malenfant,J.E.L.,VanHouten,R.V.,Jonah,B.,2002.Astudytomeasuretheincidence ofdrivingundersuspensionintheGreaterMonctonarea.AccidentAnalysisand

Prevention34,439–447.

Manstead,A.S.R.,2000.Theroleofmoralnormintheattitude–behaviorrelation.In: Terry,D.J.,Hogg,M.A.(Eds.),Attitudes,behavior,andsocialcontext.Lawrence

ErlbaumAssociates,Mahwah,NJ,pp.11–30.

Marcil,I.,Bergeron,J.,Audet,T.,2001.Motivationalfactorsunderlyingtheintention todrinkanddriveinyoungmaledrivers.JournalofSafetyResearch32,363–376. Miles,D.E.,Johnson,G.L.,2003.Aggressivedrivingbehaviors:aretherepsychological

andattitudinalpredictors?TransportationResearchPartF6,147–161.

MoyanoDíaz,E.,2002.Theoryofplannedbehaviorandpedestrians’intentionsto

violatetrafficregulations.TransportationResearchPartF5,169–175. Neyens,D.M.,Boyle,L.N.,2008.Theinfluenceofdriverdistractionontheseverity

ofinjuries.sustainedbyteenagedriversandtheirpassengers.AccidentAnalysis

andPrevention40,254–259.

NHTSA,1993.Policetimeandcostsassociatedwithadministrativelicense

revo-cation.ReportNo.HS-808064,U.S. Departmentof Transportation/National

HighwayTrafficSafetyAdministration,Washington,DC.

Ouellette,J.A.,Wood,W.,1998.Habitandintentionineverydaylife:themultiple pro-cessesbywhichpastbehaviorpredictsfuturebehavior.PsychologicalBulletin 124(1),54–74.

Pai,C.W.,Saleh,W.,2008.Modellingmotorcyclistinjuryseveritybyvariouscrash typesatT-junctionsintheUK.SafetyScience46,1234–1247.

Parker,D.,Manstead,A.S.R.,Stradling,S.G.,Reason,J.T.,Baxter,J.S.,1992a.Intention tocommitdrivingviolations:anapplicationofthetheoryofplannedbehavior. JournalofAppliedPsychology77(1),94–101.

Parker,D.,Manstead,A.S.R.,Stradling,S.G.,Reason,J.T.,Baxter,J.S.,1992b.

Deter-minantsof intentionto commit drivingviolations. AccidentAnalysis and

Prevention24,117–131.

Parker,D.,Manstead,A.S.R.,Stradling,S.G.,Reason,J.T.,Baxter,J.S.,1992c. Inten-tiontocommitdrivingviolations:anapplicationoftheoryofplannedbehavior. JournalofAppliedPsychology77,94–101.

Parker,D.,Manstead,A.S.R.,Stradling,S.G.,1995.Extendingthetheoryofplanned

behaviour:theroleofpersonal norm.TheBritishPsychologicalSociety34,

127–137.

Parker,D.,Stradling,S.G.,Manstead,A.S.R.,1996.Modifyingbeliefsandattitudesto exceedingthespeedlimit:aninterventionstudybasedonthetheoryofplanned behaviour.JournalofAppliedSocialPsychology26,1–19.

Parker,D.,Lajunen,T.,Stradling,S.,1998.Attitudinalpredictorsofinterpersonally aggressiveviolationsontheroad.TransportationResearchPartF1,11–24. Pas,E.,1988.Weeklytravel-activity.Transportation15,89–119.

Poulter,D.R.,Chapman,P.,Bibby,P.A.,Clarke,D.D.,Crundall,D.,2008.Anapplication

ofthetheoryofplannedbehaviourtotruckdrivingbehaviourandcompliance

withregulations.AccidentAnalysisandPrevention40,2058–2064.

Ross,H.L.,Gonzales,P.,1988.Effectsoflicenserevocationondrunk-driving

offen-ders.AccidentAnalysisandPrevention20,291–379.

Schank,R.C.,Abelson,R.P.,1977.Scripts,plans,goals,andunderstanding.Erlbaum, Hillsdale,NJ.

Schank,R.C.,1982.Dynamicmemory.CambridgeUniversityPress,NewYork.

Sheehan,M.,Schonfeld,C.,Ballard,R.,Schoeld,F.,Najman,J.,Siskind,V.,1996.Athree

yearoutcomeevaluationofatheorybaseddrinkdrivingeducationprogram.

JournalofDrugEducation26,295–312.

S¸ims¸eko˘glu,Ö.,Lajunen,T.,2008.Socialpsychologyofseatbeltuse:acomparison oftheoryofplannedbehaviorandhealthbeliefmodel.TransportationResearch

PartF:Psychology11,181–191.

Siskind,V.,1996.Doeslicensedisqualificationreducereoffencerates?Accident

AnalysisandPrevention28(4),519–534.

Smith,D.I.,Maisey,G.,1990.Surveyofdrivingbydisqualifiedandsuspendeddrivers

inWesternAustralia.RoadSafetyTrends,3–4.WesternAustraliaDepartment

ofTransport,Perth,WA.

Sweedler,B.M.,Stewart,K.,1993.Reducingdrinkinganddrivingthrough

adminis-trativelicenserevocation.In:Proceedingofthe12thInternationalConference onAlcohol,DrugsandTrafficSafety,Cologne.

Tertoolen,G.,VanKreveld,D.,Verstraten,B.,1998.Psychologicalresistanceagainst

attemptstoreduceprivatecaruse.TransportationResearchPartA32(3),

171–181.

Thuen,F.,Rise,J.,1994.Youngadolescents’intentiontouseseatbelts:theroleof attitudinalnormativebeliefs.HealthEducationResearch9,215–223. Triandis,H.C.,1977.InterpersonalBehavior.Brools/Cole,Monterey,CA.

Verplanken,B.,Aarts,H.,VanKnippenberg,A.,VanKnippenberg,C.,1994.Attitude

versusgeneralhabit:antecedentsoftravelmodechoice.JournalofApplied

SocialPsychology24,285–300.

Verplanken,B.,Aarts,H.,VanKnippenberg,A.,Moonen,A.,1998.Habitversus

plannedbehaviour:afieldexperiment.BritishJournalofSocialPsychology37, 111–128.

Verplanken,B.,Aarts,H.,1999.Habit,attitude,andplannedbehaviour:ishabitan

emptyconstructoraninterestingcaseofgoal-directedautomatic?European

ReviewofSocialPsychology10,101–134.

Verplanken,B.,Orbel,S.,2003.Reflectionsonpastbehaviour:aself-reportindexof habitstrength.JournalofAppliedSocialPsychology33,1313–1330.

Victoir,A.,Eertmans,A.,VanDenBergh,O.,VanDenBroucke,S.,2005.Learningto drivesafely:social-cognitiveresponsesarepredictiveofperformanceratedby novicedriversandtheirinstructors.TransportationResearchPartF8,59–74. Voas,R.B.,DeYoung,D.J.,2002.Vehicleaction:effectivepolicyforcontrollingdrunk

andotherhigh-riskdrivers?AccidentAnalysisandPrevention34,263–270.

Warner,H.W.,Åberg,L.,2008.Drivers’beliefsaboutexceedingthespeedlimits.

TransportationResearchPartF11,376–389.

Webb,T.L.,Sheeran,P.,2006.DoesChangingBehavioralIntentionsEngender

Behav-iorChange? AMeta-Analysis of theExperimental Evidence. Psychological

Bulletin132(2),249–268.

Williams,R.L.,Hagen,R.E.,McConnell,E.J.,1984.Asurveyofsuspensionand revo-cationeffectsonthedrink-drivingoffender.AccidentAnalysisandPrevention

16(5–6),339–350.

Wood,W.,Quinn,J.M.,Kashy,D.,2002.Habitsineverydaylife:thought,emotion, andaction.JournalofPersonalityandSocialPsychology83,1281–1297. Wood,W.,Quinn,J.M.,2005.Habitsandthestructureofmotivationineverydaylife.

In:Williams,K.D.,Forgas,J.P.(Eds.),SocialMotivation:Consciousand Uncon-sciousProcesses.CambridgeUniversityPress,NewYork,pp.55–70.

Yagil,D.,1998.Genderandage-relateddifferencesinattitudestowardtrafficlaws

andtrafficviolations.TransportationResearchPartF:TrafficPsychologyand

Behaviour1(2),123–135.

Zador,P.,Lund,A.,Fields,M.,Weinberg,K.,1989.Fatalcrashinvolvementandlaws againstalcohol-impaireddriving.JournalofPublicHealthPolicy10,467–485.

數據

Fig. 1. The framework of the planned behavior model and previous driving habit. AT: attitude; SN: subjective norm; PBC: perceived behavioral control; BI: behavioral

參考文獻

相關文件

We examine how past experiences, perceived behavioral controls, subjective norms, attitudes, and economic pressures affect the behavioral intentions pertaining to

Subsequently, the relationship study about quality management culture, quality consciousness, service behavior and two type performances (subjective performance and relative

Reyes (2002), “Driver Distraction, Warning Algorithm Parameters, and Driver Response to Imminent Rear-end Collisions in a High-Fidelity Driving Simulator,” National

Measures of driver behavior and cognitive workload in a driving simulator and in real traffic environment - Experiences from two experimental studies in sweden, Poster

Therefore, a new method, which is based on data mining technique, is proposed to classify driving behavior in multiclass user traffic flow.. In this study, driving behaviors

However, if we do not analyze the bus driving behavior and driving safety factors (e.g., car following gap or passing sight distance) while designing the control and safety systems

In the processing following action recognition, this paper proposes a human behavior description model to describe events occurring to human and exerts as decision and

A model of service quality perceptions and health care consumer behavior. Measurement and evaluation of satisfaction processes in