• 沒有找到結果。

Speech Perception in Infancy Predicts Language Development in the Second Year of Life: A Longitudinal Study

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Speech Perception in Infancy Predicts Language Development in the Second Year of Life: A Longitudinal Study"

Copied!
18
0
0

加載中.... (立即查看全文)

全文

(1)

Speech Perception in Infancy Predicts Language Development in the Second

Year of Life: A Longitudinal Study

Feng-Ming Tsao, Huei-Mei Liu, and Patricia K. Kuhl

Infants’ early phonetic perception is hypothesized to play an important role in language development. Previous studies have not assessed this potential link in the first 2 years of life. In this study, speech discrimination was measured in 6-month-old infants using a conditioned head-turn task. At 13, 16, and 24 months of age, language development was assessed in these same children using the MacArthur Communicative Development Inven-tory. Results demonstrated significant correlations between speech perception at 6 months of age and later language (word understanding, word production, phrase understanding). The finding that speech perception performance at 6 months predicts language at 2 years supports the idea that phonetic perception may play an important role in language acquisition.

Language development is one of the major achieve-ments of infancy and early childhood. The mile-stones of linguistic achievement have been documented across cultures and suggest that infants follow a set of universal stages both in speech pro-duction and speech perception. Wide consensus now exists among investigators, for example, on the stages in speech production exhibited by young in-fants in American English and other languages; a universal progression from cooing (1 to 4 months), to babbling (5 to 10 months), to meaningful speech (10 to 18 months) is shown (Ferguson, Menn, & Stoel-Gammon, 1992).

Research has also documented stages in speech perception development that have been shown in experimental tests on infants across cultures. Infants begin life with a universal capacity to differentiate the fine-grained acoustic events that differentiate

phonemes across languages, and this ability is ex-hibited until about 6 months of age. For example, in the early months of life, young infants have been shown to discriminate a 20-ms difference in voice onset time (VOT), an acoustic difference that is suf-ficient to distinguish English voiced (/b, d, g/) from voiceless (/p, t, k/) stop consonants (e.g., Eimas, Siqueland, Jusczyk, & Vigorito, 1971). Infants’ sen-sitivity to this subtle acoustic difference also allows them to discriminate Kikuyu (or Spanish) prevoiced phonetic units from voiceless unaspirated phonetic units, an ability that has been demonstrated in young infants both behaviorally (Streeter, 1976) and using event-related potential brain measures (Rivera-Gaxiola, Silva-Pereyra, & Kuhl, in press). Subtle acoustic differences among vowel sounds are also discriminated, allowing infants to distinguish the vowels of many languages early in life (e.g., Ald-ridge, Stillman, & Bower, 2001; Trehub, 1973).

The fine-grained perceptual skills shown early in infancy raise a question for developmental psychol-ogists studying language: How do infants use the exquisite speech perception skills shown in early infancy in language acquisition? What role does early speech perception play in later language learning? There is a lack of developmental language research attempting to bring the fields of early speech perception and later language acquisition together. The literature attests to infants’ early speech perception abilities but fails to explain whether or how infants’ abilities to perceive phonetic differences play a role in language development. Although several researchers have raised this issue, asserting that infants’ early phonetic abilities must be

r2004 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc. All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2004/7504-0007

Feng-Ming Tsao, Institute for Learning and Brain Sciences, University of Washington, and Department of Psychology, Na-tional Taiwan University, Taiwan; Huei-Mei Liu, Department of Special Education, National Taiwan Normal University, Taiwan; Patricia K. Kuhl, Institute for Learning and Brain Sciences, Uni-versity of Washington.

We thank Erica Stevens for assistance with all aspects of the study, Virpi Lahteenmaki for help with participant testing, and Barbara Conboy for helpful comments on the manuscript. During the revision of this manuscript, the first author was supported by the National Taipei Teachers College, Taiwan. Funding for the research was provided by a grant to the third author from the National Institutes of Health (HD37954); the William P. and Ruth Gerberding University Professorship; the University of Washing-ton Institute for Learning and Brain Sciences; and the Talaris Re-search Institute and Apex Foundation, the family foundation of Bruce and Jolene McCaw.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Feng-Ming Tsao, Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan 106. Electronic mail may be sent to tsaosph@mail2000.com.tw.

(2)

important for later language acquisition (Jusczyk, 1997; Kuhl, 2000; Werker & Tees, 1999), few studies have directly assessed the potential link between the two.

The purpose of the present study was to test a specific hypothesis about the connection between infants’ early speech perception abilities and their later abilities to acquire language. We posited a strong, positive association between an infant’s speech perception performance at 6 months of age and that infant’s scores on measures of language acquisition at 2 years. We tested the hypothesis using a well-researched and often-used measure of speech perception ability (conditioned head turn [HT]; see Polka, Jusczyk, & Rvachew, 1995) to test infants at 6 months of age and used subsequent follow-up tests to assess language skill in those same infants at three ages: 13, 16, and 24 months of age. No previous studies have examined the potential connection be-tween speech perception and language acquisition in children under age 3.

The hypothesized association between infants’ early phonetic abilities and their later language skills is supported by three linked arguments: (a) recent studies show that speech discrimination perfor-mance in infancy does not reach adult levels and that individual variability is largeFwe argue that dif-ferential ability in initial phonetic perception could affect language acquisition; (b) infants’ phonetic perception skills are vital to the identification of words in running speech, and children with better phonetic skills may advance more quickly toward word acquisition; and (c) existing studies of children with language impairments often show deficits in children’s phonetic perception skills, suggesting a relation between poor phonetic skills and language problems.

Consider first the performance levels of infants on tests of early speech perception and the variability shown in the infant population. Historically, research on infant speech perception examined group per-formance. Eimas and colleagues (Eimas, 1974, 1975; Eimas et al., 1971), for example, and most other early investigators, used high-amplitude sucking (HAS) as a measure of infant speech discrimination ability. HAS compares performance between an experi-mental and a control group. Discrimination is evi-denced by group differences rather than by performance of individual infants (Kuhl, 1985; Polka et al., 1995). Studies by Werker and colleagues (Werker & Tees, 1984) and Kuhl and colleagues (Kuhl, 1979, 1983) employed the conditioned HT technique, which assesses performance on both change and control trials in individual infants. The use of the HT technique permits the calculation of a

percent correct measure and the calculation of an unbiased estimate of sensitivity, d’. However, these experimenters only reported data on the proportion of infants who met a preestablished criterion thresh-old (e.g., Werker & Tees, 1984) or the average number of trials required to meet such a criterion (Kuhl, 1979, 1983) rather than individual infants’ levels of performance.

Recent tests examined infants’ absolute levels of performance on tests of speech discrimination (e.g., Kuhl, Stevens, et al., 2004; Kuhl, Tsao, & Liu, 2003; Liu, Kuhl, & Tsao, 2003; Polka & Bohn, 1996; Tsao, 2001). These data reveal two things relevant to the present investigation: First, on average, infant per-formance does not approach adult levels, and sec-ond, there is considerable variability in performance across infants. In Kuhl, Stevens, et al. (2003), Amer-ican and Japanese infants were tested on the /ra-la/ phonetic contrast at 6 to 8 and 10 to 12 months. The resulting tests showed that although infant per-formance on consonant discrimination at 6 to 8 months is solidly above chance, it is substantially below adult levels. American 6- to 8-month-olds perform on average at 63.7% correct, with a range extending from 43% to 93% correct. Japanese 6- to 8-month-olds show the equivalent pattern, with a mean score of 64.7% correct and a range of 47% to 87%. By 10 to 12 months of age, American infants improve as a group, performing at 73.8% correct, though variability remains large; individual per-formance ranges from 43% to 100%. American adults perform at 99.2% correct on this native language /r-l/ contrast (Zhang, 2002). These data suggest that: (a) phonetic perception is not uniform in the popu-lation of infants at 6 to 8 months, (b) average per-formance increases for native language consonants between 6 to 8 and 10 to 12 months but remains variable at 10 to 12 months, and (c) performance at neither age, on average, approaches adult levels. The question addressed here is whether the variance observed in infants’ early phonetic perception performance is related to the variance in later lan-guage development.

The variability seen in infants’ perception of speech raises a more fundamental issue: How might early phonetic perception skill contribute to lan-guage development? There is increasing evidence that in the first year of life infants are acquiring de-tailed information about language by listening and analyzing linguistic input (Jusczyk, 1997; Kuhl, 2000; Werker & Tees, 1999). Studies show, for example, that infants’ exposure to ambient language results in rapid learning. By 6 months of age, infants engage in a detailed analysis of the distributional properties of

(3)

sounds contained in the language they hear, and this alters perception to produce more native-like pho-netic processing (Kuhl, Williams, Lacerda, Stevens, & Lindblom, 1992; Maye, Werker, & Gerken, 2002). By 10 to 12 months of age, consonant discrimination shows a steep decline for non-native phonetic units, again reflecting a change that depends on linguistic experience (Werker & Tees, 1984). The ability to discern phonetic differences in language input is essential for the kinds of distributional analyses that infants appear to be performing. Infants whose phonetic skills are advanced may progress sooner toward native language processing. Native language processing would in turn advance infants’ abilities by allowing them to process language using the phonetic cues that are pertinent to their native language.

Resolving phonetic differences is essential to the detection of phonotactic patterns, patterns that de-scribe combinations of phonemes that are legal in the child’s native language and patterns that character-ize words in the child’s native language. Because spoken words are embedded in a continuous acoustic stream, with no clear acoustic boundaries between them, infants rely on statistical relations between phones and syllables, including distribu-tional information (Friederici & Wessels, 1993; Mattys, Jusczyk, Luce, & Morgan, 1999) and transi-tional probabilities (Goodsitt, Morgan & Kuhl, 1993; Saffran, Aslin, & Newport, 1996) to segment words from the speech stream. Between 6 and 9 months of age, infants have been shown to use phonotactic patterns to segment words from running speech (Friederici & Wessels, 1993; Mattys et al., 1999). This requires prior analysis of the phonetic cues con-tained in speech. Individual differences in the ability to detect phonetic cues could therefore potentially modulate the detection of likely word candidates.

Experimental studies that directly assess how infants’ phonetic abilities are put to work in word learning have been conducted by Werker and her colleagues (Stager & Werker, 1997; Werker, Fennell, Corcoran, & Stager, 2002). In these experiments, in-fants attempt to learn new words using phonetically similar syllable pairs that are easily confused. In-fants’ discrimination of the phonetic contrasts is necessary to establish the association between speech sounds and novel objects in such word-learning experiments, and Werker’s experiments illustrate that word-learning tasks challenge infants’ speech perception skills. These experiments indicate that the task of associating sounds and objects is sufficiently difficult that infants at 14 months cannot do so unless the phonetic units being used in the word association task are very different from one

another. When presented with novel objects and novel words, 14-month-old infants fail to associate phonetically similar names (e.g., bih – dih) with un-familiar objects. At 17 months, infants show im-provement, but they do not fully succeed at the task until 24 months. Children thus depend on their abilities to perceive phonetic distinctions to associate a sound pattern with a lexical item. An individual infant with advanced speech perception skills might therefore be expected to show advanced word-learning skills.

A third source of evidence is indirect, but relevant: The phonetic abilities of children diagnosed with reading disorders, learning disabilities, or language impairment in the form of specific language im-pairment (SLI) have often been shown to be defi-cient. In a variety of studies, children with learning disabilities or reading disabilities perform signifi-cantly lower on speech perception tasks. Compared with an age-matched control group, children with reading disabilities were poorer than age-matched controls on the discrimination of consonants in word pairs that differed by only one phoneme (Reed, 1989). Several studies also reported performance differences between children with dyslexia and controls in tests of categorical perception with con-sonant sounds (Godfrey, Syrdal-Lasky, Millay, & Knox, 1981; Manis et al., 1997; Reed, 1989; Werker & Tees, 1987). Similar findings, using both brain and behavioral measures, have been reported for chil-dren with various forms of learning disabilities (Bradlow et al., 1999; Kraus et al., 1996). Not all studies report that speech perception deficits in children with dyslexia reach significance (Adlard & Hazan, 1998; Hurford & Sanders, 1990; Manis et al., 1997), but the direction of the effect is consistent across studies.

Further evidence of a link between deficiencies in speech perception and language acquisition has been shown in school-age children with SLI (Leonard, McGregor, & Allen, 1992; Stark & Heinz, 1996a, 1996b; Sussman, 1993, 2001; Tallal & Piercy, 1974, 1975). Studies have repeatedly shown that SLI chil-dren perform more poorly than age-matched con-trols. Tests on consonant perception indicate that SLI children perform significantly more poorly than controls in the perception of acoustic cues such as formant transition, VOT, and frication noise (Leo-nard et al., 1992; Tallal & Piercy, 1975; Tallal & Stark, 1981). Compared with age-matched controls, tests on vowel perception show deficits for SLI children when vowels are presented as steady-state formants or with formant transitions (Sussman, 2001), when they are of short duration (Stark & Heinz, 1996b),

(4)

and when they are in a multisyllabic context (Leo-nard et al., 1992). To summarize, in studies in which children with a variety of impairments that involve language are compared with age-matched controls, measures of speech perception typically show that children with language-related difficulties also have significant deficits in speech perception.

The foregoing arguments that skill in phonetic analysis is essential for the acquisition of higher or-der language units does not, however, provide the needed data. Is there any evidence to suggest a re-lationship between early speech perception abilities and later language skills? One source of evidence derives from retrospective studies of newborns’ brain-wave patterns in response to speech as pre-dictors of later language delay in children between 3 and 8 years of age. Molfese and colleagues (Molfese, 2000; Molfese & Molfese, 1985, 1997) showed that when 3-, 5-, and 8-year-old children were classified into normal- and low-language performance groups, based on standardized tests, a discriminant function analysis of their brain waves as newborns predicted their classification with about 80% accuracy.

Although previous studies provide some support for the hypothesized connection between early speech perception and later language development, there are no prospective longitudinal studies exam-ining the relation between a specific measure of early speech perception and a specific set of language skills in young children; in fact, no studies have been conducted in children under 3. The purpose of the present study was to test this hypothesis. The spe-cific aim of the study was to determine whether individual differences in a standard behavioral measure of phonetic discrimination in 6-month-old English-learning infants would predict individual differences in language performance at three later time points: 13, 16, and 24 months of age.

The speech perception task used in the current study is a highly sensitive behavioral measure of infant speech perception: HT conditioning, a tech-nique used in many studies of infant speech per-ception (Kuhl, 1985; Polka et al., 1995; Werker, Polka, & Pegg, 1997) and one that allows absolute per-formance measures to be assessed in individual in-fants. Previous studies indicate that the HT task provides a sensitive measure of an individual infant’s speech perception performance (Lalonde & Werker, 1995; Liu et al., 2003). The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories (CDIs), a well-established measure of language development, were used to measure language outcomes at 13, 16, and 24 months of age (Fenson et al., 2000; Fenson et al., 1994).

Our design relies on the correlational approach. A significant correlation between early phonetic per-ception performance and later language develop-ment forges an associative link but does not establish a causal relationship. Of importance to the interpre-tation of an association is the examination of other variables that could mediate the observed link. One feature of the study was the inclusion of measure-ments of parental variables, such as age, education, and work status, to examine the relationship be-tween these factors and infants’ early speech per-ception and later language skills.

The effect of parental socioeconomic status (SES) on infant speech perception has not been studied, and support for the idea that parental SES relates to language development is mixed. For example, ma-ternal SES has been shown to contribute to lexical comprehension and production in 20-month-olds, independent of maternal verbal ability (Bornstein, Haynes, & Painter, 1998). In other studies, SES-associated differences in child language develop-ment have been attributed to the amount of language input provided by mothers, which differed in higher versus lower SES mothers (Hart & Risley, 1995, 1999; Hoff-Ginsberg & Tardif, 1995; Huttenlocher, 1999). However, other studies did not find a relationship between SES factors and child language develop-ment. Using parental report, large-scale language development studies (sample size 4500) did not find a significant association between SES and vo-cabulary development during the second year of life (Fenson et al., 1994; Hamilton, Plunkett, & Schafer, 2000). In the present study, we obtained parental SES data, allowing us to take parental SES factors into account.

A second factor of interest in the study was to examine the stability of language measures for in-dividual children. Linguistic abilities undergo rapid changes during the second year of life. At 13 months most children begin to produce their first words (Benedict, 1979), at 16 months their production vo-cabulary size is more than 50 words (Lucariello, 1987), and by 24 months children are typically combining words (Bates, Dale, & Thal, 1995). Previ-ous studies have shown high variability at each age; one study with a large sample (N 5 217) indicated that individual differences in language comprehen-sion and production were relatively stable during the 6-month period between 13.5 and 20 months, although large variations in developmental rate exist-ed (Fenson et al., 1994). Another study investigatexist-ed language continuity in groups of late- and early-talking toddlers and demonstrated that differences in language development were relatively stable from

(5)

8 to 30 months at the group level (Thal, Bates, Goodman, & Jahn-Samilo, 1997). However, per-formance differences between groups were not in-dicative of individual children. For example, Thal et al. (1997) reported that only 60% of late talkers, classified at 18 to 24 months, had been among the bottom 10% for word production at 16 months. These data suggest that additional longitudinal studies are needed to examine individual stability in language development in the second year of life.

In summary, the major goal of the present study was to test the hypothesis that speech perception in infancy is predictive of subsequent language devel-opment in the second year of life. We argue that phonetic discrimination ability is essential for nor-mally developing young infants’ acquisition of lexi-cal information from the speech stream and therefore that better phonetic perception ability in infancy will be associated with advanced lexical development in the second year of life. A secondary focus was the stability in measures of language development be-tween 13, 16, and 24 months of age.

Method Participants

The participants were 28 full-term infants (14 boys, 14 girls) tested at 6 months of age (M age 5 5.92 months, range 5 5.67 to 6.27 months). Criteria for infant participants included: (a) English as the only language spoken in the household; (b) no known physical, sensory, or mental handicap; (c) gestational age at birth at 40  3 weeks; and (d) birth weight between 5.5 and 10 lb. An additional 6 babies were tested and eliminated for failure to reach criterion in the training stages of the HT procedure (described later). The majority of infants were White from college-educated parents. Infants were recruited through the database of names contained in the Infant Studies Subject Pool at the University of Washington.

Procedure and Materials

Infants were tested at 6 months of age in the HT task. At that time a family information questionnaire was used to collect data regarding the infant’s gen-eral health and age, and the age, working status, and years of education of the infant’s parents. The working status of each parent was coded on a 7-point scale assessing the percentage of working hours from 1 (full time) to 7 (unemployed). The three parental SES measures (age, working status, and years of educa-tion) were used in the correlational analysis to assess whether parental SES is related to infants’ phonetic

discrimination and later language scores. Language performance in the same children was measured with the CDI, which was completed by the parents when infants were 13, 16, and 24 months of age.

At 6 months of age: Speech discrimination. Infants were tested in the HT task with computer-synthe-sized vowels. The goal of the study was to examine speech perception abilities using a phonetic contrast that would not produce ceiling effects, thereby masking individual differences, and one that was not strongly influenced by individual native language listening experience. The vowel contrast involved two Finnish vowels, the Finnish high-back vowel /u/versus the Finnish high-front /y/. Native English-speaking adults perceive these two vowels as closely related to the English vowels /u/ and English /i/, respectively, and do not perceive them as foreign language vowels. Our pilot test showed that Amer-ican English-speaking adults judge the vowel pro-ductions as nonprototypical instances of the English /u/ and /i/ vowels. We reasoned that the use of nonprototypical vowels would increase the difficulty and thus the individual differences in speech per-ception ability.

The /u/ and /y/ vowels used in our tests fall into the range of vowels heard by American listeners but differed from American English prototypes: The /u/ has a lower second formant (F2 5 630 Hz) than pro-totypical English /u/ (F2 5 997 Hz), and the /y/ has a lower first (F1 5 280 Hz) and second formant (F2 5 1850 Hz) than English /i/ (F1 5 342 Hz, F2 5 2322 Hz; Aaltonen & Suonpa¨a¨, 1983). The major acoustic difference between the two synthesized vowel sounds is the higher second formant in /y/; the first and second formants were 255 and 1947 Hz, respectively, for /y/, and 289 and 630 Hz, respec-tively, for /u/. The duration of both vowels was 271 ms and the fundamental frequency was 120 Hz.

During the HT procedure, the infant sat on his or her parent’s lap in a sound-attenuated booth. An assistant played with toys silently on the infant’s right to keep the infant’s attention. On the infant’s left, speech sounds were presented continuously from a studio-quality loudspeaker (Electro-Voice SP 12) at a comfortable listening level of 68 dB SPL. The presentation level of the speech stimuli was cali-brated each day before the test with a sound-level meter (Bruel & Kjaer Model 2203) placed in the ap-proximate position of the infant’s head. Above the loudspeaker was a Plexiglass box containing a me-chanical toy that could be activated and lit; it served as the reinforcer for the infant. An experimenter observed infants’ responses from the control room via a video monitor and judged HT responses, which

(6)

were recorded by a computer (IBM 286 compatible PC). The parent and the assistant wore headphones and listened to music that masked the speech sounds so they could not influence infants’ responses. The experimenter heard masking music and could barely hear the background sound, but during trials, the experimenter’s headphones were totally deactivated as a further control for bias. The sound presented during trials could not be heard and the experi-menter could therefore not influence performance.

Two kinds of trials, change and control, were conducted during various phases of the experiment. During change trials, the background sound, which was repeatedly played every 2 s during the experi-ment, was changed to the target sound for a 5-s period. During that period, HT responses were reinforced with a 5-s presentation of the mechanical toy (a bear pounding a drum or a monkey playing cymbals). During control trials, no sound change occurred, but infants’ responses were monitored. Four outcomes are possible in the two types of trials. During change trials, an HT is correct and is scored as a hit; failure to turn is scored as a miss. During control trials, a HT response is scored as a false alarm; failure to turn is scored as a correct rejection. The background sound was /u/ and the target sound was /y/. Only one direction of stimulus change was tested because directional effects have been observed in studies of infant speech perception (e.g., Kuhl, Stevens, et al., 2004; Polka & Bohn, 1996); differences in performance due to directional effects could therefore have diluted the power of our as-sessment of individual phonetic perception.

The HT procedure consisted of three phases: con-ditioning, criterion, and test. In the conditioning phase, only change trials were conducted so that infants could learn the association between a change in the sound and the reinforcer. During conditioning, the target sound was presented at a louder level than the background sound (14 dB SPL) to alert infants to the sound change. During the first conditioning tri-als, the reinforcer was automatically activated after two target sounds were presented so that the infant learned to pair a change in the sound with the reinforcer. After the infant made two consecutive HTs that occurred before the end of the first two presentations of the target sounds (i.e., anticipatory HTs), the intensity cue was removed. After three consecutive anticipatory HTs with no intensity cue, infants moved to the criterion phase. If the infant did not meet the conditioning criteria within 60 trials, the infant was excluded from participation in the study. The use of 60 trials ensured that as many infants as possible would be included in the study.

In the criterion phase, both change and control trials were conducted. The probability of the two types of trials was .5 with the restriction that no more than three consecutive trials of one type could occur. The criterion phase continued until 7 of 8 consecu-tive correct responses occurred, with a maximum of 30 trials. After meeting criterion, the test phase be-gan. During the test phase, 30 trials, both change and control at .5 probability, were conducted. Typically, infants required two visits, approximately 20 min each, to complete the entire procedure, which were scheduled within 1 week. The infants typically completed the conditioning and criterion phases in the first visit and the test phase in the second visit.

At 13, 16, and 24 months of age: Language compre-hension and production. The language performance of children in this longitudinal study was collected through the CDI, which has been shown to be a re-liable and valid tool for assessing language and communication development from 8 to 30 months of age (Fenson et al., 2000; Fenson, et al., 1994). Two forms of the CDIFInfant and ToddlerFwere used in this study. The Infant form (CDI: Words and Gestures) provides norms for vocabulary compre-hension, vocabulary production, and gesture pro-duction in children from 8 to 16 months of age. The Infant form consists of two parts. Part I is a 396-item vocabulary checklist divided into 19 semantic categories. Ten of those categories are composed of nouns (animals, vehicles, toys, food and drink, clothing, body parts, furniture and rooms, small household items, outside things and places to go, and people). The remaining nine categories include sound effects and animal sounds, games and rou-tines, action words, words about time, descriptive words, pronouns, question words, prepositions and locations, and quantifiers. Parents respond to each item, answering whether their child understands (and produces) each word. Part II lists gestures that are examples of nonverbal communication skills. The gestures are categorized as: early communica-tive gestures (intentional communication behaviors, such as giving, showing, and pointing), games and routines (social interactive gestures, such as pat-a-cake), actions with objects, pretending to be a parent, and imitating other adult actions. In the current study, the CDI Infant form was used to assess language development when children reached 13 and 16 months of age.

Language skills were assessed in 24-month-olds using the CDI Toddler form (CDI: Words and Sen-tences). This form is designed to measure language production in children from 16 to 30 months of age.

(7)

This form divides language production into two major parts. Part I contains a 680-word vocabulary production checklist composed of 22 semantic cate-gories. Part II includes five sections designed to as-sess morphological and syntactic development. Two of these sections, irregular words (noun and verb) and grammatical complexity, were used in this study. The irregular word section contains a checklist of 25 irregular nouns and verbs (e.g., teeth, got). The grammatical complexity section includes 37 sentence pairs in which one exemplifies typical immature grammatical structures and the other represents more advanced structures. Three levels of gram-matical ability are evaluated in this section: bound morphemes (e.g., two shoe vs. two shoes), functional words (e.g., doggie table vs. doggie on table), and early emerging complex sentences forms (e.g., turn on light vs. turn on light so I can see you). In each case, parents choose the option that typifies their child’s language at a particular age.

Just before the time the child reached 13 and 16 months, parents were sent the McArthur CDI Infant form (CDI: Words and Gestures). Language com-prehension was assessed in two subscales of the CDI: words understood and phrases understood. Lan-guage production was measured using the number of words produced. Nonlinguistic communicative development was quantified by the use of early gestures (intentional communication), late gestures (representational skills), and total ( 5 early1late) gestures. Parents typically filled out the CDI on the day their infant turned the target age and returned the form within 1 week. Each parent received $10 for mailing back a completed CDI.

Just before the children reached 24 months of age, the McArthur Toddler form (CDI: Words and Sen-tences) was sent to parents. Expressive language development was quantified in three subscales: word production, irregular words, and grammatical complexity. Therefore, these subscales were included in the data analysis. Each parent received $15 for mailing back a completed CDI.

Results

At 6 Months of Age: Speech Discrimination Ability Two quantitative measures obtained from the HT procedure were used: (a) the number of trials needed to pass the criterion phase, and (b) the overall per-cent correct in the test phase. Reaching criterion within a set number of trials has been used in pre-vious HT studies to examine infant speech percep-tion development (Werker et al., 1997; Werker &

Tees, 1984) and has been shown to be a sensitive index of individual differences in speech perception (Lalonde & Werker, 1995; Liu et al., 2003). In the present study, we assumed that infants who reached criterion in fewer trials were able to process speech information more efficiently than infants who need-ed additional trials to reach the criterion. The overall percent correct measure assessed infants’ speech perception ability over the entire test period and has been frequently used in previous studies as a measure of speech discrimination performance (Goodsitt, Morse, & ver Hoeve, 1984; Kuhl et al., 1992). The two speech perception measures were calculated for each 6-month-old infant and are shown in Table 1.

The mean percent correct (73.61%) for all infants was well above the 50% chance level, t(27) 5 11.381, po.0001, indicating that American 6-month-old in-fants do not experience great difficulty discriminat-ing these vowels. No gender differences on phonetic discrimination were found; boys (M 5 74.92%, SD 5 12.64%) and girls (M 5 72.29%, SD 5 9.31%) performed equally well, t(26)o1. Table 1 shows that individual infants’ performance was widely distrib-uted for both of the two perceptual measures ob-tained at this age.

The first step in data analysis was to test whether infants’ speech perception scores at 6 months were predicted by parental factors. The range in SES var-iables reflected our relatively homogeneous sample: maternal age (M 5 32.07 years, SD 5 4.76; range 5 21 to 40), paternal age (M 5 35.48 years, SD 5 6.62; range 5 22 to 52), maternal education (M 5 17.1 years, SD 5 1.58; range 5 14 to 21), paternal educa-tion (M 5 16.1 years, SD 5 2.28; range 5 12 to 21), maternal work status (scale 5 1 to 7, full time 5 1; M 5 4.65. SD 5 1.42; range 5 3 to 7), and paternal work status (M 5 1.26, SD 5 1.14; range 5 1 to 6). No significant correlations were found between parental age and percent correct performance (mother’s age, r 5.050; father’s age, r 5.349), years of education (mother’s education, r 5 – .087; father’s education, r 5 – .306), or working status (mother, r 5.066; father, r 5.296). Similar data were obtained for the trials-to-criterion measure. In this group of middle-class

Table 1

Speech Perception Measures on 6-Month-Old Infants

Perceptual measures M SD Range

Number of criterion trials 15.18 7.08 7 – 29

(8)

families, parental SES was not associated with 6-month-old infants’ phonetic discrimination abilities.

Relation Between Speech Discrimination at 6 Months and Language at 13 Months

Results for the 13-month-old infants are based on analysis of CDIs returned by the parents of 20 infants (boys 5 10, girls 5 10). Table 2 shows the item and percentile scores on the CDI subscales. Item scores are directly calculated from the number of items checked in the CDI. Percentile scores are the relative position measures of item scores based on CDI norms for boys and girls. The range of percentile scores was large (from 5% to 93%), though the av-erage percentile scores of this group of children on the CDI subscales were below the normed averages for this age. The skewed percentile scores of indi-vidual infants reduce the performance differences between individuals and might reduce the

proba-bility of obtaining statistically significant correlations between early speech perception and subsequent language development. Therefore, to examine fully the longitudinal relation between speech perception and language development, both item scores and percentiles were used in calculating the correlations between speech perception and language.

The relation between early speech perception and later language development was assessed using the Spearman rank order correlation analysis. Because of the relatively small sample size at each age, and the fact that the speech perception scores obtained in the HT procedure constituted relative scores along which the 20 infants were ranked, a nonparametric correlation coefficient (rs) was considered the

statis-tically appropriate tool to assess these relations. Ta-ble 3 displays the correlation coefficients obtained for CDI item and percentile scores relating speech discrimination at 6 months and language perfor-mance at 13 months. Significant coefficients in Table 3 indicate the direction and strength of the relation between phonetic discrimination and CDI language subscores.

The results show that infants who require fewer trials to pass the criterion phase and achieve higher percent correct scores in the test phase have devel-oped better lexical abilities by 13 months. The number of trials to reach criterion was highly corre-lated with the language comprehension subscales: phrases understood, rs(item) 5 – .662, po.01 and

rs(percentile) 5 – .606, po.01; words understood,

rs(item) 5 – .700, po.001 and rs(percentile) 5 – .643,

po.01. The percent correct measure was significantly correlated with language production: words pro-duced, rs(item) 5 .477, po.05 and rs(percentile) 5

.490, po.05. There were no significant correlations between parental factors and the CDI language measures at 13 months. Moreover, no significant

Table 2

Item and Percentile Scores of Infants on the CDI Subscales at 13 Months of Age Phrases understood Words understood Words produced Early gesture Late gesture Item scores M 12.70 93.45 8.10 10.32 17.58 Mdn 13.50 80.50 5.50 11.00 18.00 SD 6.03 62.67 7.23 2.47 7.34 Range 4 – 25 13 – 235 0 – 23 5 – 14 5 – 29 Percentile scores M 30.25 39.25 35.90 34.47 47.63 Mdn 26.50 38.00 26.50 35.00 50.00 SD 20.45 22.35 21.98 20.55 25.65 Range 5 – 90 5 – 93 5 – 85 5 – 85 5 – 90

Note. CDI 5 MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory.

Table 3

Correlation Coefficients (rs) Relating Phonetic Discrimination at 6 Months and CDI Scores at 13 Months

Language scores Nonlinguistic communication

Phonetic discrimination measures Phrases understood Words understood Words produced Early gesture Late gesture Item score Criterion trials .662 .700 .220 .339 .072 Percent correct .103 .047 .477 .116 .005 Percentile score Criterion trials .606 .643 .199 .242 .146 Percent correct .135 .072 .490 .059 .046

Note. CDI 5 MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory. po.05. po.01. po.001.

(9)

correlations were found among speech discrimina-tion measures, parental SES, and the CDI nonlin-guistic communication subscales measured at 13 months, including both early and late gestures.

A regression analysis was used to assess the rel-ative contribution of early speech perception and parental factors on later language development. A regression model with the stepwise selection method including both speech perception measures and pa-rental SES as predictors (inclusion criterion, p 5 .05) revealed that the number of criterion trials contrib-utes most to later language ability. Performance on criterion trials accounts for 44%, F(1, 19) 5 14.12, p 5 .001, and 33%, F(1, 19) 5 8.91, p 5 .008, of the variance in phrases understood and words under-stood, respectively, when item scores are used for dependent variables. No other predictors were sig-nificant independent variables in these regression models. Similar results were found when percentile scores were used in regression models. The number of criterion trials accounts for 30%, F(1, 19) 5 7.71, p 5 .012, and 33%, F(1, 19) 5 8.98, p 5 .008, of the variance in phrases understood and words under-stood, respectively. The regression models for word production did not reach statistical significance. Figures 1, 2, and 3 display the significant data re-lating measures of phonetic discrimination and subscales of language development at 13 months: phrases understood, words understood, and words produced, respectively.

Relation Between Speech Discrimination at 6 Months and Language at 16 Months

Results at 16 months are based on CDI forms completed by the parents of 16 infants (boys 5 8, girls 5 8). Table 4 shows the item and percentile

scores of the infants on the CDI subscales. The range of percentile scores remains large (5% to 97%). As at 13 months of age, both item and percentile scores of the CDI subscales were employed to calculate the correlation coefficients between the perceptual measures at 6 months and language development at 16 months.

Table 5 shows the correlation coefficients (rs)

in-dicating the direction and strength of the relation between phonetic discrimination at 6 months and CDI language subscales at 16 months. Figures 4 and 5 display the significant factors for data relating measures of phonetic discrimination and language development subscales at 16 months: words under-stood and words produced. Considering item scores of the CDI, Table 5 shows that the number of trials required to reach criterion is significantly correlated with words understood (rs5– .470, po.05) and the

percent correct is significantly correlated with words produced (rs5.470, po.05). As shown in Table 5, no Figure 1. Relation between number of criterion trials at 6 months

and number of phrases understood at 13 months (rs5– .662,

po.01). The trend line is the least squares regression line.

Figure 2. Relation between number of criterion trials at 6 months and number of words understood at 13 months (rs5– .700,

po.001). The trend line is the least squares regression line.

Figure 3. Relation between percent correct at 6 months and number of words produced at 13 months (rs5.477, po.05). The trend line

(10)

significant correlations were obtained between speech perception and language development when the percentile scores of CDI subscales were used. As at 13 months of age, there was no significant asso-ciation at 16 months between parental SES and CDI measures. In addition, no significant correlations were obtained at 16 months between speech dis-crimination and nonlinguistic measures of commu-nication.

A regression analysis with stepwise selection method and a predictor inclusion criterion of p 5 .05 was used to assess the relative contribution of speech perception and parental factors at 6 months, and CDI language measures at 13 months, to language per-formance at 16 months. Phrases understood (R25.33, p 5 .021) and words understood (R25.52, p 5 .002) at 13 months are the most powerful pre-dictors for phrases understood and words under-stood at 16 months, respectively. No other predictors met the criteria for inclusion in these regression models.

Relation Between Speech Discrimination at 6 Months and Language at 24 Months

At 24 months, data were obtained from the par-ents of 13 infants (boys 5 7, girls 5 6) who completed the CDI Toddler form (CDI: Words and Sentences). Table 6 shows the item and percentile scores of the CDI subscales at this age. Substantial variation was again observed in percentile scores on the CDI sub-scales (10% to 97%).

The correlation coefficients (rs) shown in Table 7

indicate the direction and strength of the relation be-tween phonetic discrimination at 6 months and CDI language subscores at 24 months. The number of criterion trials was significantly correlated with word production, rs(item) 5 – .480, po.05 and rs

(percen-tile) 5 – .523, po.05; production of irregular words, rs(item) 5 – .595, po.05 and rs(percentile) 5 – .592, p

o.05; and grammatical complexity, rs(item) 5 – .609, p

o.05 and rs(percentile) 5 – .570, po.05. Figures 6, 7,

and 8 illustrate the significant correlations.

Table 4

Item and Percentile Scores of CDI Subscales at 16 Months of Age Phrases understood Words understood Words produced Early gesture Late gesture Item score M 21.50 225.31 47.94 14.00 26.57 Mdn 22.50 234.50 42.00 14.00 27.00 SD 4.15 64.45 44.98 1.88 8.64 Range 13 – 28 103 – 324 1 – 182 11 – 16 10 – 41 Percentile score M 41.36 63.29 45.06 38.14 37.92 Mdn 41.00 65.00 46.00 27.50 20.00 SD 25.82 21.62 28.29 29.07 34.11 Range 5 – 90 25 – 93 5 – 97 5 – 85 5 – 95

Note. CDI 5 MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory.

Table 5

Correlation Coefficients (rs) Relating Phonetic Discrimination at 6 Months and CDI Scores at 16 Months

Language scores Nonlinguistic communication

Phonetic discrimination measures Phrases understood Words understood Words produced Early gesture Late gesture Item score Criterion trials .237 .470 .096 .009 .220 Percent correct .129 .170 .470



.093 .197 Percentile score Criterion trials .088 .030 .226 .272 .381 Percent correct .080 .162 .409 .019 .072

Note. CDI 5 MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory. po.05.

Figure 4. Relation between number of criterion trials at 6 months and number of words understood at 16 months (rs5– .470,

(11)

The regression model with stepwise selection method including the speech perception measures, percentile scores of CDI language measures at 13 and 16 months of age, and parental SES factors as inde-pendent variables demonstrates that the speech perception measure, the number of criterion trials, is the most important predictor for lexical production at 24 months. It accounts for 31%, F(1, 12) 5 4.85, p 5 .050, of the percentile score variances in word production. Using the same predictors in the regres-sion analysis, the number of criterion trials is also the most powerful predictor (R25.43, p 5 .009) in a re-gression model that accounts for 61% of the percen-tile score variances in irregular words produced, F(2, 12) 5 7.91, p 5 .009. The mother’s working status (R25.18, p 5 .056) reached marginal significance at 24 months. When using item scores, a regression model using the same predictors accounts for 66%, F(2, 12) 5 9.50, p 5 .005, of the variance in the

pro-duction of irregular words. The number of criterion trials (R25.35, p 5 .012) is again the most powerful predictor; mother’s working status (R25.31, p 5 .014) is the second most important predictor. No other predictors met the criteria for inclusion in the regression models for grammatical complexity. The results of various regression models consistently demonstrate that early speech perception is an im-portant predictor of later language development.

Relations Among CDI Subscales

Analyses were also conducted to examine the relations among the various subscales of the CDI. Tables 8 and 9 show correlations (rs) among 13-, 16-,

and 24-month CDI subscales with item and percen-tile scores, respectively. High internal consistency was found for the two subscales of the CDI on lan-guage comprehension, phrases and words under-stood. The two subscales were strongly correlated with each other at 13 months, rs(item) 5 .833, po.001

and rs(percentile) 5 .822, po.001, and 16 months,

rs(item) 5 .716, po.001 and rs(percentile) 5 .686,

po.01. At 24 months, similar consistency was shown for language production among CDI subscales. Words produced was highly associated with pro-duction of irregular words, rs(item) 5 .844, po.001

and rs(percentile) 5 .899, po.001, and grammatical

complexity, rs(item) 5 .789, po.001 and rs

(percen-tile) 5 .720, po.01. Irregular word production was significantly correlated with grammatical complexi-ty, rs(item) 5 .652, po.01 and rs(percentile) 5 .670,

po.01. Beyond the domain of language, significant correlations also indicate high internal consistency between two nonverbal communication subscales, early and late gestures, at 13 months, rs(item) 5 .470,

po.05 and rs(percentile) 5 .554, po.01, and 16

months, rs(item) 5 .618, po.01 and rs(percentile) 5

.664, po.01.

Although high internal consistency is repeatedly shown within various CDI subscales when measur-ing the same communication dimension at different ages, few significant correlations are shown among subscales measuring different communication as-pects. At both 13 and 16 months, no significant cor-relation was found between the comprehension and production subscales of the CDI except when the percentile scores were used to examine this relation at 13 months. It is interesting that the early gesture subscale score is significantly correlated with two subscales of language comprehension, words un-derstood and phrases unun-derstood, at both 13 months: phrases understood, rs(item) 5 .564, po.01

and rs(percentile) 5 .473, po.05; words understood, Table 6

Item and Percentile Scores of CDI Subscales at 24 Months of Age Words produced Production of irregular words Grammatical complexity Item score M 374.38 5.92 12.46 Mdn 391.00 4.00 10.00 SD 128.98 4.94 10.45 Range 125 – 593 1 – 20 1 – 29 Percentile score M 60.31 54.92 63.00 Mdn 60.00 57.50 67.00 SD 17.96 22.36 24.36 Range 25 – 93 25 – 97 10 – 93

Note. CDI 5 MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory. Figure 5. Relation between percent correct at 6 months and number of words produced at 16 months (rs5.470, po.05). The trend line

(12)

rs(item) 5 .611, po.01 and rs(percentile) 5 .550,

po.01; and at 16 months: phrases understood, rs

(i-tem) 5 .714, po.01 and rs(percentile) 5 .636, po.01;

words understood, rs(item) 5 .620, po.01 and

rs(percentile) 5 .749, po.001. However, the late

ges-ture scale is not significantly correlated with lan-guage comprehension subscales except for phrases understood at 16 months, rs(item) 5 .499, po.05 and

rs(percentile) 5 .467, po.05.

Language Stability Over Time: Relations Among 13-, 16-, and 24-Month Data

One advantage of using a psychometrically equivalent tool, such as the CDI, is that the results allow one to explore an individual child’s language stability over time. Developmental stability can be assessed in 13- and 16-month-old children for all

subscales of the CDI. Because a different form of the CDI was used at 24 months, only the words pro-duced subscale can be used at that age to assess stability.

Significant correlations were obtained for all CDI subscales between 13 and 16 months, as shown in Tables 8 and 9. For example, performance on phrases understood, rs(item) 5 .591, po.01 and rs

(percen-tile) 5 .523, po.05, and words produced, rs(item) 5

.566, po.05 and rs(percentile) 5 .480, po.05, are

sig-nificantly related at these two ages. Associations between 13 and 16 months are stronger when item scores in CDI subscales, rather than percentile scores, are used to calculate the coefficients. In contrast, no significant correlation was obtained between 13- or 16-month performance and 24-month performance on word production. These results indicate that in-dividual differences in language development are stable from 13 to 16 months in both expressive and receptive aspects of language. Results showing

con-Table 7

Correlation Coefficients (rs) of Phonetic Discrimination Measures at 6

Months and CDI Language Scores at 24 Months Language scores Phonetic discrimination measures Word produced Production of irregular words Grammatical complexity Item score Criterion trials .480 .595 .609



Percent correct .050 .210 .240 Percentile score Criterion trials .523 .592 .570 Percent correct .090 .203 .246

Note. CDI 5 MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory. po.05.

Figure 6. Relation between number of criterion trials at 6 months and percentile of words produced at 24 months (rs5– .523,

po.05). The trend line is the least squares regression line.

Figure 7. Relation between number of criterion trials at 6 months and percentile of irregular word production at 24 months (rs5

– .592, po.05). The trend line is the least squares regression line.

Figure 8. Relation between number of criterion trials at 6 months and percentile of grammatical complexity at 24 months (rs5

(13)

tinuity over a 6-month period in language ability have been reported in other studies using the CDI (Fenson et al., 1994; Thal et al., 1997). Fenson et al. (1994) used a larger group (N 5 217) to examine language stability between 13.5 and 20 months and obtained a correlation coefficient of r 5.69 on word production. The examined periods in the present study, 8 and 11 months, are considerably longer than those used by Fenson et al. Thal et al. (1997) reported great developmental variability between 16 and 24

months, consistent with the present findings, indi-cating that individual differences in word produc-tion development do not remain stable during the second year of life.

Discussion

The present study was designed to test whether infants’ performance on a standard measure of speech perception in the first year of life significantly

Table 8

Correlation Coefficient (Spearman’s rs) Matrix for Item Scores Among CDI Subscales at 13, 16, and 24 Months

CDI subscales (Percentile score) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13

1. Phrases understoodF13 months

2. Words understoodF13 months .833

3. Words producedF13 months .307 .373

4. Early gestureF13 months .564 .611 .171

5. Late gestureF13 months .155 .049 .058 .470

6. Phrases understoodF16 months .591 .547 .125 .470 .255

7. Words understoodF16 months .619 .789 .099 .432 .066 .716

8. Words producedF16 months .118 .053 .566 .007 .081 .039 .218

9. Early gestureF16 months .227 .272 .056 .642 .380 .714 .620 .329

10. Late gestureF16 months .020 .064 .259 .488 .851 .499 .121 .166 .618

11. Words producedF24 months .066 .135 .063 .014 .247 .229 .482 .436 .607 .101

12. Irregular words produced F24 months

.126 .246 .235 .055 .190 .065 .299 .520 .365 .093 .844

13. Grammatical complexity F24 months

.197 .211 .051 .079 .163 .171 .384 .393 .639 .014 .789 .652

Note. CDI 5 MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory. po.05. po.01. po.001.

Table 9

Correlation Coefficient (Spearman’s rs) Matrix for Percentile Scores Among CDI Subscales at 13, 16, and 24 Months

CDI subscales (Percentile score) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13

1. Phrases understoodF13 months

2. Words understoodF13 months .822

3. Words producedF13 months .346 .381

4. Early gestureF13 months .473 .550 .183

5. Late gestureF13 months .166 .014 .067 .554

6. Phrases understoodF16 months .523 .440 .164 .437 .383

7. Words understoodF16 months .340 .518 .129 .398 .254 .686

8. Words producedF16 months .222 .117 .480 .040 .256 .043 .310

9. Early gestureF16 months .063 .037 .025 .497 .474 .636 .749 .372

10. Late gestureF16 months .065 .196 .186 .475 .924 .467 .420 .424 .664

11. Words producedF24 months .000 .069 .081 .101 .312 .169 .364 .196 .324 .009

12. Irregular words produced F24 months

.088 .202 .213 .045 .307 .057 .217 .233 .200 .095 .899

13. Grammatical complexity F24 months

.122 .113 .008 .073 .322 .018 .229 .215 .452 .042 .720 .670

Note. CDI 5 MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory. po.05. po.01. po.001.

(14)

predicted their language abilities at three later points in development. The hypothesis was confirmed. The results of the study demonstrated significant corre-lations between individual infants’ speech percep-tion skills at 6 months and their language abilities at 13, 16, and 24 months. The results demonstrated, for the first time, that a speech perception measure at 6 months of ageFthe ability to discriminate two vowels as measured by the HT conditioning tech-niqueFsignificantly predicts language outcomes at three ages over the next 18 months.

Previous retrospective studies suggested a po-tential connection between brain responses to speech syllables in newborns and the classification of those children at 3, 5, and 8 years into groups having high versus low language skills (Molfese, 2000; Molfese & Molfese, 1985, 1997). Taken together, the results suggest that phonetic perception may play an im-portant role in language acquisition.

A secondary goal was to examine the continuity in measures of language development over time. The results suggest that individual differences in lan-guage development are stable from 13 to 16 months in both expressive and receptive aspects of language. However, between either 13 or 16 months and 24 months, the results on word production show a lack of stability. The lack of developmental stability be-tween 13 or 16 months and 24 months shown in this study is similar to previous findings for this period (Feldman et al., 2000; Thal et al., 1997), suggesting increasing variation in individual lexical develop-ment between the first and second birthdays.

Explaining the Link Between Phonetic Discrimination and Language Development

The present results demonstrate a predictive re-lation between early speech perception performance and later language. The caveat, of course, is that a causal relation between early speech perception and later language performance cannot be established with the correlation approach used in the present experiment. The question is: Is it speech perception, or some other factor, that links early speech percep-tion performance and later language?

There are two obvious mediating factor accounts. Each one argues that it is not infants’ phonetic skills per se that explain the observed association between early speech and later language. Considering the first, a historical view might argue that a unified language faculty, one that varies genetically in the population (Fodor, 1983; Pinker, 1994), explains the results. A unified language faculty could exhibit it-self both in variation in 6-month-old infants’

pho-netic perception and in that child’s more complex later language skills. If a unified language faculty exists, one could not argue that phonetic perception is associated with lexical or grammatical language performance, they would be one and the same thing. Epidemiological studies showing a genetically relat-ed factor in language disorders that affects multiple levels provide supportive data for this view (Dale et al., 1998; Flint, 1999; Lai, Fisher, Hurst, Vargha-Khadem, & Monaco, 2001). On this account, a 6-month-old’s skills at phonetic discrimination should predict that same child’s language skills much later because both reflect the same capacity for language learning.

A second mediating factor account is that the as-sociation we have observed between speech per-ception and later language relies on infants’ more general cognitive or sensory abilities rather than on their phonetic abilities per se. Cognitive factors, such as attention and learning, could be argued to play a role, for example, especially when measured using complex tasks such as HT conditioning, a task that taps general cognitive abilities (Kuhl, 1985; Polka et al., 1995). The HT task requires infants to learn a complex contingency between two independent events, a change in a sound with the presentation of a reward. Infants with higher cognitive skills may therefore perform better in the HT task, independent of their phonetic abilities. Cognitive factors may also play a role in learning the arbitrary pairing of sound patterns and words. Some evidence in support of cognitive factors is the fact that among the two measures chosen to assess infant phonetic discrimi-nation, number of criterion trials was a better pre-dictor than percent correct. The number of criterion trials taps the rapidity with which infants make the correct association and could rely on infant’s cogni-tive abilities, as well as on their capacity to respond to phonetic differences. No studies have attempted to assess speech perception, when measured in the HT task, and cognitive skills in the same infants. It is possible that cognitive abilities play a role.

Infants’ sensory abilities might similarly be ar-gued to explain the observed association between phonetic perception and later language. Group studies suggest that poor auditory perceptual skills, when measured using both speech and nonspeech signals, is a significant factor in children who have difficulty with language and reading (Tallal, 1980; Tallal et al., 1996; Tallal, Stark, & Mellits, 1985). Children with SLI use fewer spectral cues to segre-gate a target tone from a masking noise when com-pared with normal controls (Wright et al., 1997). In a recent study of 6-month-old infants with and

(15)

with-out positive histories of language impairment, au-ditory temporal resolution thresholds of individual infants for nonspeech stimuli were associated with language comprehension and production scores at age 2 (Benasich & Tallal, 2002). These results suggest that the observed variability in typically developing infants’ phonetic processing abilities, those critical for language development, could be influenced by variation in infants’ complex auditory abilities.

The current study cannot definitively rule out these alternative explanations. However, it is also clear that there is a potential advantage afforded a child whose abilities to discern differences between phonetic events are advanced. Exposure to ambient language causes a perceptual transition from a lan-guage-general pattern of speech perception to a language-specific pattern (Best, McRoberts, LaFleur, & Silver-Isenstadt, 1995; Kuhl et al., 1992; Werker & Tees, 1984). Advanced speech discrimination skills would allow more efficient detection of the statistical regularities in ambient speech (Goodsitt et al., 1993; Saffran, 2001; Saffran et al., 1996), and this would move infants to the native language listening pattern earlier in development. Native language listening affords an advantage because native language words use language-specific phonology. Better phonetic perception assists the detection of phonotactic pat-terns in speech (Jusczyk, Luce, & Charles-Luce, 1994), which may in turn allow infants to accelerate their discovery of words in running speech and, eventually, to advance their word production. In other words, infants who are more skilled in de-tecting phonetic differences may simply progress faster on the ladder toward language. Advanced phonetic abilities in infancy may bootstrap language learning at higher levels (Morgan & Demuth, 1996), propelling infants to more sophisticated levels ear-lier in development. However, infants’ sensory and cognitive skills may also play a role, either inde-pendently or in concert with infants’ phonetic skills. To separate the two general mediating factor ex-planationsFthe integrated language faculty expla-nation and general cognitive and sensory abilities explanation from our proposed hypothesis (that the ability to discriminate phonetic units plays a decisive role in language learning)Fadditional longitudinal studies will need to be conducted. In such studies, early linguistic, cognitive, social, and sensory factors could be assessed in the same population of infants, with the goal of examining the intercorrelations and predictive value of the early measures for later lan-guage development.

Toward that end, we have begun a longitudinal study following 40 children from 6 months to 3

years, assessing infants’ performance on auditory, phonetic, babbling, and other tasks, and relating these early skills to language outcomes. Of interest to the present discussion, phonetic perception was assessed using the same vowel stimuli and HT discrimination task used in the present study. Pre-liminary analyses of the language outcome measures at 24 months confirm, with a much larger sample, the strong and significant association between early phonetic perception and later language reported here. Analysis of the entire study will eventually reveal the patterns of association between and among the various predictor measures.

We ascribe to the view that multiple attentional, social, and linguistic cues contribute to infants’ word understanding and production in early language development (Hirsh-Pasek & Golinkoff, 1996; Ho-llich, Hirsh-Pasek, & Golinkoff, 2000), and therefore expect that large-scale studies of the type just de-scribed will show a complex pattern in predicting later language. The contribution of the current study is that it demonstrates, for the first time, that per-formance on a standard measure of speech percep-tion at 6 months reliably predicts language scores at 2 years in a population of typically developing chil-dren. The current study serves as an initial test of the hypothesis that infants’ early speech perception abilities play a significant role in their later language development. As discussed, however, this result leaves open several possibilities regarding the role that sensory and cognitive skills may play in the ob-served association.

Implications for the Early Identification of Language-Related Difficulties

The results of this study, showing significant cor-relations between infants’ speech perception at 6 months and language performance as long as 18 months later, may eventually have clinical implica-tions for the early identification of language-related difficulties. Further research is needed, but it is possible that phonetic sensitivity measures might be employed as one of the early identification tools at 6 months to screen infants at high risk for later speech and language difficulties, such as SLI, reading disa-bility, or phonological disorders. Several studies have reported that children who were late talkers at 24 months were likely to be delayed in expressive language development 1 year later (Paul, 1991; Paul & Smith, 1993; Rescorla, Roberts, & Dahlsgaard, 1997; Rescorla & Schwartz, 1990). Significant correla-tions between 6-month-olds’ phonetic discrimination abilities and 24-month-olds’ language performance

(16)

found here could eventually lead to diagnostic tests for at risk populations.

Conclusions

The present study demonstrates that a standard measure of speech perception taken at 6 months of age in a population of typically-developing childrenF the discrimination of two simple vowelsFpredicts language development at 13, 16, and 24 months of age. These findings have both theoretical and prac-tical value. From a theoreprac-tical standpoint, the find-ings are consistent with the idea that phonetic perception plays a critical role in the early phases of language acquisition. Practically, the results suggest the possibility that early measures of speech discrim-ination may provide information helpful to the early detection of infants at risk for language difficulties.

References

Aaltonen, O., & Suonpa¨a¨, J. (1983). Computerized two-dimensional model for Finnish vowel identifications. Audiology, 22, 410 – 415.

Adlard, A., & Hazan, V. (1998). Speech perception in chil-dren with specific reading difficulties (dyslexia). Quar-terly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51A, 153 – 177. Aldridge, M. A., Stillman, R. D., & Bower, T. G. R. (2001).

Newborn categorization of vowel-like sounds. Develop-mental Science, 4, 220 – 232.

Bates, E., Dale, P., & Thal, D. (1995). Individual differences and their implications for theories of language devel-opment. In P. Fletcher & B. MacWhinney (Eds.), The handbook of child language (pp. 96 – 151). Oxford, England: Blackwell.

Benasich, A. A., & Tallal, P. (2002). Infant discrimination of rapid auditory cues predicts later language impairment. Behavioural Brain Research, 136, 31 – 49.

Benedict, H. (1979). Early lexical development: Compre-hension and production. Journal of Child Language, 6, 183 – 200.

Best, C. T., McRoberts, G. W., LaFleur, R., & Silver-Isenstadt, J. (1995). Divergent developmental patterns for infants’ perception of two nonnative consonant contrasts. Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 339 – 350. Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, M. O., & Painter, K. M. (1998).

Sources of child vocabulary competence: A multivariate model. Journal of Child Language, 25, 367 – 393.

Bradlow, A. R., Kraus, N., Nicol, T. G., McGee, T. J., Cun-ningham, J., & Zecker, S. G., et al. (1999). Effects of lengthened formant transition duration on discrimina-tion and neural representadiscrimina-tion of synthetic CV syllables by normal and learning-disabled children. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106, 2086 – 2096.

Dale, P. S., Simonoff, E., Bishop, D. V. M., Eley, T. C., Oliver, B., & Price, T. S., et al. (1998). Genetic influence on

language delay in two-year-old children. Nature Neuro-science, 1, 324 – 328.

Eimas, P. D. (1974). Auditory and linguistic processing of cues for place of articulation by infants. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 513 – 521.

Eimas, P. D. (1975). Auditory and phonetic coding of the cues for speech: Discrimination of the [r-l] distinction by young infants. Perception and Psychophysics, 18, 341 – 347. Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, E. R., Jusczyk, P., & Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science, 171, 303 – 306. Feldman, H. M., Dollaghan, C. A., Campbell, T. F., Kurs-Lasky, M., Janosky, J. E., & Paradise, J. L. (2000). Meas-urement properties of the MacArthur Communication Development Inventories at ages one and two years. Child Development, 71, 310 – 322.

Fenson, L., Bates, E., Dale, P., Goodman, J., Reznick, J. S., & Thal, D. (2000). Measuring variability in early child language: Don’t shoot the messenger. Child Development, 71, 323 – 328.

Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D., & Pethick, S. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, (5, Serial No. 242).

Ferguson, C. A., Menn, L., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (Eds.). (1992). Phonological development: Models, research, impli-cations. Timonium, MD: York Press.

Flint, J. (1999). The genetic basis of cognition. Brain, 122, 2015 – 2031.

Fodor, J. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Friederici, A. D., & Wessles, J. M. I. (1993). Phonotactic knowledge of word boundaries and its use in infant speech perception. Perception and Psychophysics, 54, 287 – 295. Godfrey, J. J., Syrdal-Lasky, A. K., Millay, K., & Knox, C. M.

(1981). Performance of dyslexic children on speech perception tests. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 32, 401 – 424.

Goodsitt, J. V., Morgan, J. L., & Kuhl, P. K. (1993). Percep-tual strategies in prelingual speech segmentation. Jour-nal of Child Language, 20, 229 – 252.

Goodsitt, J. V., Morse, P. A., & ver Hoeve, J. N. (1984). Infant speech recognition in multisyllabic contexts. Child Development, 55, 903 – 910.

Hamilton, A., Plunkett, K., & Schafer, G. (2000). Infant vocabulary development assessed with a British com-municative development inventory. Journal of Child Language, 27, 689 – 705.

Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Brookes.

Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1999). The social world of children learning to talk. Baltimore: Brookes.

Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (1996). The origins of grammar: Evidence from early language comprehension. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Hoff-Ginsberg, E., & Tardif, T. (1995). Socicoeconomic sta-tus and parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting (Vol. 2, pp. 161 – 188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

數據

Figure 2. Relation between number of criterion trials at 6 months and number of words understood at 13 months (r s 5 – .700, po.001)
Table 6 shows the item and percentile scores of the CDI subscales at this age. Substantial variation was again observed in percentile scores on the CDI  sub-scales (10% to 97%).
Figure 5. Relation between percent correct at 6 months and number of words produced at 16 months (r s 5 .470, po.05)
Figure 6. Relation between number of criterion trials at 6 months and percentile of words produced at 24 months (r s 5 – .523, po.05)

參考文獻

相關文件

Developing Students’ Multimodal Literacy in the Secondary English Language Classroom is a resource package produced by the English Language Education Section,

To help students appreciate stories related to the theme and consolidate their knowledge and language skills in writing stories, the English Club has organised a workshop on story

(d) While essential learning is provided in the core subjects of Chinese Language, English Language, Mathematics and Liberal Studies, a wide spectrum of elective subjects and COS

“Chinese Language Assessment Tools” tailored for NCS students and a longitudinal study, further evaluate the effectiveness of measures to support NCS students’

9 The pre-S1 HKAT is conducted in all secondary schools in July every year to assess the performance of students newly admitted to S1 in Chinese Language, English Language

The early development of Kiyozawa’s philosophy is deeply grounded in religious belief, which is fundamentally a philosophy of self-exertion. That is, Kiyozawa’s early philosophy is

Learning Strategies in Foreign and Second Language Classrooms. Teaching and Learning a Second Language: A Review of

A majority of the secondary schools adopted project learning to develop students’ language development strategies and generic skills but this was only evident in a small number of