• 沒有找到結果。

Comparison of children's self-reports of depressive symptoms among different family interaction types in northern Taiwan

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Comparison of children's self-reports of depressive symptoms among different family interaction types in northern Taiwan"

Copied!
10
0
0

加載中.... (立即查看全文)

全文

(1)

Open Access

Research article

Comparison of children's self-reports of depressive symptoms

among different family interaction types in northern Taiwan

Wen-chi Wu

1

, Chi-Hsien Kao

2

, Lee-Lan Yen*

1,3

and Tony Szu-Hsien Lee

4

Address: 1Center for Health Policy Research and Development. National Health Research institutes. No. 35, Keyan Road, Zhunan Town, Miaoli County 350, , R.O.C, Taiwan, 2Institute of Health and Welfare Policy, National Yang-Ming University. No. 155, Sec. 2, Linong St., Beitou District, Taipei City 112, , R.O.C, Taiwan, 3Institute of Health Policy and Management, College of Public Health, National Taiwan University. No.17 Xu-Zhou Road, Taipei 10020, , R.O.C, Taiwan and 4Department of Health Promotion and Education, National Taiwan Normal University, Taiwan Email: Wen-chi Wu - wuvirsan@ms17.hinet.net; Chi-Hsien Kao - arron_0704@hotmail.com; Lee-Lan Yen* - leelan@ntu.edu.tw; Tony Szu-Hsien Lee - tonylee@ntnu.edu.tw

* Corresponding author

Abstract

Background: Previous research has shown that family interactions are associated with depressive

symptoms in children. However, detailed classifications of family interaction types have not been studied thoroughly. This study aims to understand the types of family interactions children experience and to identify the specific types of family interactions that are associated with a higher risk of depressive symptoms in children.

Methods: Data used in the study was collected as part of the Child and Adolescent Behavior in

Long term Evolution (CABLE) project in 2003. CABLE is a longitudinal cohort study that commenced in 2001 and collects data annually from children in Taipei city and Hsinchu county in northern Taiwan. The data analyzed in this study was that obtained from the sixth graders (aged 11 to 12 years old) in 2003. Of the 2,449 sixth graders, 51.2% were boys and 48.8% were girls. Factor analysis and cluster analysis were used to investigate the types of family interactions. One way ANOVA was used to establish the relationship between family interaction types and children's self-reports of depressive symptoms.

Results: Based on the results of factor analysis, the latent factors for family interactions included

supporting activities, psychological control, parental discipline, behavioral supervision, and family conflict. After conducting cluster analysis using factor scores, four types of family interactions were revealed: supervised (29.66%), disciplined (13.56%), nurtured (40.96%) and conflict (15.82%). Children from the disciplined or conflict families were more likely to report depressive symptoms. Children from the nurtured families were least likely to report depressive symptoms.

Conclusion: Family interactions can be classified into four different types, which are related to

children's self-reports of depressive symptoms. The creation of a family interaction environment that is beneficial for children's mental health is an important issue for health education and health promotion professionals.

Published: 20 June 2007

BMC Public Health 2007, 7:116 doi:10.1186/1471-2458-7-116

Received: 31 August 2006 Accepted: 20 June 2007 This article is available from: http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2458/7/116

© 2007 Wu et al; licensee BioMed Central Ltd.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

(2)

Background

According to the 2001 WHO World Health Report [1], the worldwide prevalence of depression is 3%, which equates to a total of 120,000,000 sufferers of depression. It is esti-mated that by 2020, depression will be the second cause of the Disability-Adjusted Life-Year (DALY) behind heart disease [2]. (The DALY is a quantitative indicator of bur-den of disease that reflects the total amount of healthy life lost to related causes during a period of time [3].) Depres-sion has even been called the 'plague' of modern times by the mental health community [4]. People with symptoms of depression are more likely to have engaged in binge drinking and smoking, and are also more likely to have high blood pressure, high cholesterol, arthritis and asthma [5]. A US study has estimated that the health serv-ice costs for individuals with major depression are approx-imately 70% higher than for those without any depressive disorder [6]. Therefore, in addition to its influence on individual health, depression can increase overall health service costs.

A US review of depression related literature in the decade prior to 1996 showed that the onset of depressive symp-toms is occurring at younger ages. Children and adoles-cents who were born at the end of the 20th century have a higher chance of developing mood disorders and of this continuing into adulthood. The review provided preva-lence rates of depression ranging from 0.4–2.5% in chil-dren and 0.4–8.3% in adolescents. Furthermore, the lifetime prevalence rate of major depressive disorder in adolescents was estimated to range from 15% to 20%. [7]. In Taiwan, research conducted in 1998 measuring depres-sive symptoms in 1,434 seventh graders in Taipei found that 55.5% of adolescents reported having mild depres-sive symptoms (having 1–10 symptoms), 27.9% reported having more depressive symptoms (having 11–20 symp-toms) and 9.9% reported serious depressive symptoms (having over 20 symptoms)[8]. The 1999 National Survey of Physical and Mental Health of Youth in Taiwan col-lected data from 3,487 adolescents aged 12–18 years old. It found that when the respondents encountered obstacles or stressful life events, 30.5% of them experienced depres-sive symptoms as their most frequent response out of eight possible emotional responses [9]. The variety of methods used to measure depressive symptoms and the different statistical methods used make it impossible to compare the rates of depression between studies. How-ever, the high prevalence of symptoms found in the Tai-wanese research indicates that childhood depression is still a very important issue for Taiwan.

Individuals who have depressive symptoms during child-hood are more likely to develop psychiatric symptoms, aggression, poor adaptive function and low self-esteem during young adulthood [10,11]. Children with

depres-sion are more likely to suffer from separation anxiety, phobias, somatic complaints and behavioral problems [7]. They are also more likely to have difficulties at home, at school and with their peers and have less social adapta-bility [12]. Hence, depressive symptoms in children should be addressed early in order to prevent later psychi-atric problems [10].

Previous research has investigated the relationship between family interactions and childhood depression [13-20]. In the United States, family environments of depressed children were found to be less rewarding, more aversive and more disengaged than those of non-depressed children [13], and adolescents with parents who frequently display anger or rejecting behaviors, and who rarely communicate with or encourage their chil-dren, are more likely to have depressive symptoms [14,15]. Children with a safer attachment relationship with their family and who receive more information, material goods, warmth and support from their family, are more likely to be able to use these resources to over-come stressful life events and as a result have better phys-ical and mental health [16-19]. Similar results have been found in Taiwan where poor supervision, and strict, indi-rect and severe parenting styles have all been positively associated with depressive symptoms in adolescents [8]. In contrast, perceived positive messages from parents are negatively associated with depression in children [20].

A specific branch of research has focused on the associa-tions between types of family interacassocia-tions and children's depressive symptoms [14,21-25]. Stein et al [21] investi-gated the relationship between depression and family interactions in American children aged between 7 and 16 years. Scores for the two components of Care (which refers to the level of emotional expression, feeling of closeness and empathy between family members) and Protection (which refers to the level of parental control, supervision, and infantilization of children) were used to divide parent-child bonding into Affectionless-Control (care and high-protection), Weak (care and low-protection), Affective Constraint (high-care and high-pro-tection), and Autonomy Supporting (high-care and low-protection). Stein's study showed that children with a high risk of depressive symptoms were more likely to report parental bonding as affectionless-control than low-risk children.

In the field of family system research, Olson et al [22] cat-egorized family interactions according to three important components: Cohesion (refers to the strength of emo-tional connection in the family), Adaptability (refers to the ability of the family to change family norms, structure of rights and responsibilities, and role relationships), and Communication (refers to the ability of family members

(3)

to exchange information). The components of cohesion and adaptability were then used to develop the Cir-cumplex Model. This model proposes that family systems may range from extremely low cohesion to extremely high cohesion and from extremely low adaptability to extremely high adaptability. The families at the central level of cohesion and adaptability would function better than those who are at the highest and lowest levels of both dimensions. Lavee & Olson [23] further used these two dimensions to divide families into the following four types: Flexible-separated families (scored above the mean on the adaptability scale and below the mean on the cohe-sion scale), flexible-connected family (scored above the mean on both the cohesion and adaptability scales), structured-separated families (scored below the mean on both the cohesion and adaptability scales), and struc-tured-connected families (scored below the mean on adaptability and above the mean on the cohesion scale). The results showed that people from flexible-separated and structured-separated families experienced more strain and poorer well-being than those from the other two groups.

Lamborn and Mounts [24] referred to Maccoby and Mar-tin's conceptual framework [25] and divided parenting styles into four types (authoritative, authoritarian, indul-gent, and neglectful) based on 2 dimensions: acceptance/ involvement and strictness/supervision. The research revealed that adolescents who characterize their parents as neglectful (scores in the lowest tertiles on both dimen-sions) expressed more psychological problems then those who described their parents as authoritative.

In summary, the majority of previous research has divided family interactions into four types based on scores of two dimensions. However, in reality, the components of fam-ily interactions may be more complex and the range of types of family interactions could be more comprehensive if the components were measured more precisely. In this study, we collected data on a large number of items related to family interactions such as family activities, parental discipline and control, family support and family conflict. By including an extensive range of items we hoped to measure family interactions more completely. Our study aims to understand the types of family interac-tions of elementary school students and to further eluci-date the relationship between family interaction types and childhood depressive symptoms.

Methods

Study participants

Data analyzed in this study was taken from the 2003 data collected as part of the Child and Adolescent Behavior in Long-term Evolution (CABLE) project [26]. CABLE com-menced in 2001 with the random selection of 18 public

elementary schools from Taipei city (representing a met-ropolitan area) and Hsinchu county (representing a rural area) in northern Taiwan. The CABLE cohort was estab-lished from the first and fourth grade students of the cho-sen schools and their parents [27]. Students whose parents agreed to their participation in the CABLE study filled out questionnaires in class. Data analyzed in our study was collected from the sixth grade student cohort in 2003 (which was the representative fourth grade sample in 2001). The age of the participants ranged from 11 to 12 years old. The original sample consisted of 2499 students, and after exclusion of incomplete questionnaires, a total of 2,449 participants were included in the analysis: 1,254 boys (51.2%) and 1,195 girls (48.8%). A total of 1,329 (54.3%) children were from Taipei city and 1,120 (45.7%) were from Hsinchu county. Approximately 57% of fathers had a high school level of education, and 28.9% had at least a college degree. About 65.9% of mothers had a high school level of education, and 18.9% had at least a college degree. In addition, 87.6% of parents were mar-ried.

Study instrument and research variables

The questionnaire developed by the CABLE team for the sixth graders in 2003 included the core questions of previ-ous years (including behavioral habits, psychological health, family interactions, peer relationships etc). Slight modifications were sometimes made to the annual ques-tionnaires according to special issues in a particular year. We conducted a pilot study on June 2003 at two schools that were not part of this study. The results of this pilot study and the validity and reliability of questions were taken into consideration when modifying the question-naire to produce the final version [27]. Data used in this study included information about the children's gender, children's self-reported depressive symptoms and chil-dren's perceptions of family interactions (see Appendix). Further explanations of the definition and measurement of children's self-reports of depressive symptoms and chil-dren's perceptions of family interactions are given below.

Depressive symptoms

Children's self-reports of depressive symptoms refer to the results of the CABLE Depression Scale filled out by the study participants. This scale was based on Kovac's [10,28] Children's Depression Inventory (CDI) and Faulstich et al's Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale for Children (CES-DC) [29]. Partici-pants rated how often in the past two weeks they experi-enced the following seven emotions: "Didn't feel like eating favorite foods", "Felt very sad", "Cried for no rea-son", "Found it hard to carry out tasks", "Felt frightened", "Didn't sleep well", and "Lacked motivation". Each item had a possible score of one to three points (1 = never, 2 = once or twice, 3 = many times). The scores from each of

(4)

the seven items were added together to give an overall total score ranging from seven to twenty-one. Higher scores indicated that children reported more depressive symptoms. The Cronbach's α of the depressive symptom scale was 0.75. The results of exploratory factor analysis indicated that the seven items could be categorized into one factor.

Family interactions

The development of the CABLE Family interactions Scale was based on relevant literature [7,8,14-25] and also included some culturally specific questions. It included six aspects: family activities, parental discipline, parental support, psychological control, behavioral supervision and family conflict. Each item was scored from one to four (1 = never, 2 = one or two days, 3 = many days, 4 = every day). Family activities included activities that children performed with their parents during the past week such as talking, eating, doing housework, doing homework, play-ing at home, and goplay-ing out somewhere. 'Parental disci-pline' referred to methods that children reported their parents used to punish them such as being grounded, banned from doing something they like, pocket money taken away, made to do housework, made to kneel, made to stand or physically punished. 'Parental support' referred to the level of positive support given to children by parents including encouragement, praise, consolation when upset, caring, listening, concern for school life, helping when needed and offering reasons when asking them to do something. 'Psychological control' referred to the reported level of parental control over children and was assessed by questions such as: "Do your parents make you feel they are always right?", "Do your parents often say to you 'Children don't understand things. Wait until you are older and then you will understand?", "Do your parents remind you about past mistakes when you are in trouble?, "Do your parents interrupt you when you are talking?", "Do your parents blame you for things that other people in the house have done?", and "When you and your parents disagree about something, are they not nice to you?". 'Behavioral supervision' referred to the reported level of understanding and control that parents have over children's behavior and was assessed by ques-tions such as: "Do your parents know what you do in your free time?", "Do your parents know what you do after school?", "Do your parents know who you mostly hang out with?", and "Do your parents know how you spend your pocket money?". 'Family conflict' referred to any reported episodes of conflict between the child and other family members within the last month, and included quarrels or fighting with siblings, and quarrels with par-ents or other senior family members.

Data management and statistical analysis

Data analysis was conducted using SPSS statistical soft-ware version 11.0. We used frequencies and percentages to express the distributions of depressive symptoms and we used means and standard deviations to express the characteristics of the total depressive symptom scores. To clarify the family interaction types of study participants, first we conducted factor analysis to analyze the latent fac-tor structure of family interactions. Then cluster analysis was performed to group participants according to the fac-tor scores for characteristics of family interactions. Finally, family interaction types were defined based on the results of one way ANOVA which showed differences in each fac-tor between different clusters. In addition one way ANOVA was also used to establish the relationship between family interaction types and childhood depres-sive symptoms.

Results

Children's self-reports of depressive symptoms in study participants

As shown in Table 1, the average depression score in the study sample was 10.56. Girls (10.71) reported slightly higher scores for depressive symptoms than boys (10.42). Since the range of the depressive symptoms score was 7 to 21 points, the results indicate that the participants in this study reported only mild depressive symptoms. Gener-ally, more than half of participants reported that they felt very sad (67.2%), found it hard to carry out tasks (54.4%) or lacked motivation (54.3%). Out of the seven depressive symptoms, girls were significantly more likely to report feeling sad, crying for no reason, and feeling frightened. Boys were more likely than girls to report finding it diffi-cult to carry out tasks.

Types of children's perceived family interactions among study participants

Factor structure of children's perceived family interactions

Table 2 shows the results of the factor analysis of "Family interactions". The Bartlett's test of sphericity and Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) were used to determine the appro-priateness of the data set for factor analysis [30]. Bartlett's test of sphericity was evaluated for the factorability of the correlation matrix (i.e., to determine whether the items could be classified into categories). The significant Bar-tlett's test result (p < 0.001) indicated that the correlation matrix was significantly different from an identity matrix and appropriate for factor analysis (table 2). The KMO measure of sampling adequacy was 0.920, indicating that the correlation matrix had sufficient structure to result in a factorable solution [30]. Therefore, the 34 items under family interactions were suitable for factor analysis.

Principle component analysis was used to extract factors. The analysis was performed with an orthogonal (varimax)

(5)

rotation and five factors were extracted based on eigenval-ues (measures of variance) greater than 1. Each of these five factors explained 16.6%, 7.6%, 7.3%, 6.9% and 5.0% of variability respectively, which gave a combined total explanation of total variance of 43.5% (Table 2). The scale for measuring children's perceptions of family interac-tions in the CABLE study looked at the six aspects of parental support, family activities, psychological control, parental discipline, behavioral supervision, and family conflict. Apart from a small number of items, the results of factor analysis were mostly in agreement with the above six categories (Table 2). Factor one included twelve items, had a Cronbach's α value of 0.9 and was called 'Support-ive activities'. Factor two included seven items, had a Cronbach's α value of 0.71 and was called 'Psychological control'. Factor three had six items, a Cronbach's α value of 0.70 and was called 'Parental discipline'. Factor four also included six items, had a Cronbach's α score of 0.72 and was called 'Behavioral supervision'. Factor five included three items, had a Cronbach's α of 0.66 and was called 'Family conflict'. The values of the associations between the five factor scores ranged from -0.005 to 0.009.

Clustering of children's perceived family interactions

Cluster analysis is used to classify subjects into different groups. Subjects in the same group share some common traits which means they have the smallest distance or larg-est similarity [31]. In this study, children with the greatlarg-est similarity in factor scores of children's perceived family interactions were classified into the same group. To iden-tify the different types of children's perceived family inter-actions, a two-stage cluster analysis was carried out based on the above-calculated factor scores and with "children" as the unit of analysis. Firstly, Ward's hierarchical cluster-ing method was used to choose the ideal number of clus-ters. When four groups were condensed into three groups,

agglomerative coefficients increased from 11322.842 to 12253.122, and the greatest rate of change in the agglom-erative coefficient was observed (5.95%–7.59%). As a result, it was decided to divide the study sample into four groups. K-means nonhierarchical cluster analysis was then used to divide the study sample into actual clusters. Scheffe's method was used to compare differences in the five factors among four clusters.

As shown in table 3, cluster one included 709 participants (29.66%), cluster two included 324 participants (13.56%), cluster three included 979 participants (40.96%), and cluster four included 378 participants (15.82%). The different clusters were given names based on which factor scores were high and low. The factor score for behavioral supervision in cluster one was 0.2130, which was higher than that for the other three clusters. The factor scores for the other interaction factors were lower and therefore this cluster was called the 'Supervised group'. The factor scores for psychological control (0.6512) and parental discipline (1.7356) were higher in cluster two and so this cluster was called the 'Disciplined group'. The factor scores for supportive activities (0.7472) and behavioral supervision (0.0813) were higher in clus-ter three and so this clusclus-ter was called the 'Nurtured group'. The factor scores for family conflict (1.6387) were higher in cluster four and so this cluster was called the 'Conflict group'.

Relationships between children's perceived family interaction types and children's self-reports of depressive symptoms

The bottom row of table 3 shows the relationship between children's perceptions of family interaction types and chil-dren's self-reported depressive symptom scores. Children with different types of family interactions had signifi-cantly different scores for depressive symptoms. The

aver-Table 1: Descriptive statistics for depressive symptoms in the study sample

Depressive symptoms Never Once or twice Many times Chi-square test

boys girls boys girls boys girls

n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%)

1. Didn't feel like eating 713 (56.9) 656 (54.9) 483 (38.5) 497 (41.6) 58 (4.6) 42 (3.5) 3.714 2. Felt very sad 457 (36.4) 344 (28.8) 676 (53.9) 689 (57.7) 121 (9.6) 161 (13.5) 20.280*** 3. Cried for no reason 1056 (84.3) 876 (73.4) 166 (13.2) 262 (22.0) 31 (2.5) 55 (4.6) 43.555*** 4. Found it hard to carry out tasks 546 (43.6) 570 (47.8) 556 (44.4) 531 (44.5) 151 (12.1) 92 (7.7) 13.953** 5. Felt frightened 968 (77.2) 832 (69.6) 250 (19.9) 309 (25.9) 36 (2.9) 54 (4.5) 18.692*** 6. Didn't sleep well 774 (61.8) 726 (60.8) 376 (30.0) 391 (32.7) 103 (8.2) 78 (6.5) 3.910 7. Lacked motivation 596 (47.5) 526 (44.0) 529 (42.2) 556 (46.5) 129 (10.3) 113 (9.5) 4.678

Whole sample Boy Girl T-test

depressive symptoms score(7~21points) n = 2441 mean = 10.56 SD = 2.66 n = 1251 mean = 10.42 SD = 2.58 n = 1190 mean = 10.71 SD = 2.73 -2.65** **: p < 0.01; ***: p < 0.001

(6)

age score for depressive symptoms was highest in the Disciplined group (11.94), followed by the Conflict group (11.79) and the Supervised group (10.56). The average depressive symptom score was lowest in the Nur-tured group (9.61). The children in this study reported only mild depressive symptoms. This is probably because the participants come from the general population.

Discussion

Our results confirm that children's perceptions of family interactions have five underlying factors and that partici-pants can be grouped into four different interaction types according to their factor scores. In addition, we were able to discern the types of family interactions that are associ-ated with children's self-reports of depressive symptoms.

Table 2: Rotated factor structure of family interactions in the study sample

Family interactions Factor 1 Supporting activities Factor 2 Psychological control Factor 3 Parental discipline Factor 4 Behavioral supervision Factor 5 Family conflict Communalities

1. Parents praise you .804 -.011 -.104 .065 -.043 .663

2. Parents comfort you .800 -.074 -.095 .094 -.080 .670

3. Parents encourage you .792 -.054 -.152 .066 -.060 .662

4. Parents help you solve problems .756 -.045 -.103 .172 -.069 .619 5. Parents listen to what you say .711 -.162 -.136 .135 -.112 .581 6. Parents care about what happens at school .674 -.012 -.014 .196 -.086 .501 7. Parents look after you when you are sick .664 .023 -.060 .142 -.030 .465 8. Parents tell you why they want you to do things .613 .046 -.090 .152 -.082 .416

9. Parents talk with you .579 -.126 -.017 .295 .060 .442

10. Parents play with you at home .502 -.362 .153 .293 .096 .502

11. Parents take you on outings .466 -.320 .143 .200 .162 .406

12. Parents do your homework with you .454 -.308 .253 .290 .082 .455 13. Parents remind you about past mistakes when you

are in trouble -.162 .634 .303 .052 .051 .525

14. Parents interrupt you when you are talking -.233 .589 .247 .028 .101 .473 15. Parents shout and swear at you -.052 .518 .408 -.028 .096 .448 16. Parents blame you for things other people have

done -.267 .492 .267 -.010 .228 .437

17. Parents tell you that children don't understand

things .153 .467 .112 -.008 .133 .272

18. Parents make you feel that they are always right .337 .400 .061 .045 -.063 .284 19. Parents are not nice to you when you disagree

with them

-.183 .389 .286 -.013 .168 .295

20. Parents hit you -.175 .331 .590 .022 .121 .504

21. Parents punish you by making you do housework

or write lines -.011 .068 .587 .018 .048 .352

22. Parents punish you by making you kneel or stand -.086 .229 .581 -.009 .130 .415 23. Parents take away your pocket money or toys -.051 .206 .563 -.066 -.060 .370 24. Parents ban you from doing something you like -.051 .373 .496 -.012 -.014 .388

25. Parents lock you in a room .004 .022 .427 -.079 .080 .195

26. Parents know what you do on the way home from

school .227 .066 -.187 .676 .023 .549

27. Parents know what you do in your free time .315 -.015 -.170 .660 .021 .564 28. Parents know who you spend most of your time

with

.300 .098 -.229 .593 -.019 .505

29. Parents know how you spend your pocket money .147 .055 -.057 .582 -.114 .380 30. You do housework with your parents .321 -.251 .252 .452 .009 .434

31. You eat with your parents .326 -.131 .165 .402 -.009 .312

32. You have physical fights with your brothers and

sisters -.142 .016 .052 -.100 .815 .697

33. You have arguments with your brothers and

sisters -.108 .124 .044 -.122 .805 .692

34. You have arguments with your parents or other

elders -.077 .350 .055 -.228 .399 .343

Eigenvalue 8.0 3.6 1.6 1.4 1.3

Post varimax explained variability (%) 16.6 7.6 7.3 6.9 5.0 Accumulated explained variability (%) 16.6 24.2 31.5 38.4 43.5

Cronbach's α .900 .680 .740 .717 .657

KMO = 0.920

(7)

Children from the disciplined or conflict groups were more likely to report depressive symptoms. On the other hand, children from the nurtured group were least likely to report depressive symptoms.

Relationships between different types of family interactions and children's self-reports of depressive symptoms

The results found that children from the disciplined group were most likely to report depressive symptoms. In this group, factor scores for parental discipline and psycholog-ical control were much higher than in other groups. This means that children in disciplined families may be pun-ished more frequently and may be more powerless than those from other groups. This result is similar to the pre-vious finding that children from low affection, negative communication and high control families report more depressive symptoms [21,23,24]. The parents in disci-plined families in Taiwan are comparable to the authori-tarian families proposed by Lamborn [24]. These parents may be used to the traditional parenting style where chil-dren are raised in a strict or serious way [32,33] and where it is thought that severe methods of teaching, physical and oral punishment are a kind of general form of education [34]. A Taiwanese proverbs says "Children only have ears, and don't have a mouth". These kinds of statements may have an indirectly detrimental impact on a child's mental health and may lead children to think that they are inca-pable, unacceptable, or inadequate. Continual negative comments and inappropriate punishments from parents may be associated with the development of depression [35]. Therefore, it is important that Taiwanese parents pay more attention to the potential effect that negative state-ments and strict punishstate-ments have on children in the long term.

We found that children from the conflict group reported higher levels of depressive symptoms. Children's percep-tions of family interacpercep-tions in this group were character-ized as having higher factor scores than the other groups

for family conflict and lower scores for supervision. Few studies have included the concept of conflict in the family interaction structure. 'Conflict' in the context of this study is conceptualized as children answering back to their par-ents or elders or quarrelling with their siblings. It is similar to the daily hassles proposed by Chen's research involving 463 Taiwanese mothers and contributes to the mother's psychological distress [33]. Children in a family environ-ment full of conflict have increased psychological burdens and chronic melancholia due to their powerlessness to change the situation [36].

Children from the supervised group reported lower levels of depressive symptoms than the previous two groups. This group was characterized by having the highest factor scores for behavioral supervision and the lowest factor scores for supportive activities, parental discipline and family conflict. Behavioral supervision was defined in this study as the level of parental knowledge about their child's daily life and included the level of parental partic-ipation and involvement in their child's life. A study from eastern Taiwan found that higher parental involvement was associated with lower childhood depression [20]. Our study also indicates that appropriate behavioral supervi-sion is associated with fewer reported depressive symp-toms. However, further research is still required to determine whether increasingly higher levels of behavio-ral supervision lead to increasingly lower levels of depres-sion or if behavioral supervidepres-sion of children can actually lead to an increase in childhood depression if it surpasses appropriate levels.

Children from the nurtured group reported the lowest lev-els of depressive symptoms. This group was characterized by having the highest frequency of supportive activities and the second highest frequency of behavioral supervi-sion. This result demonstrates that when parents are more supportive of children or when positive interactions with family members are more common, children are less likely to report depressive symptoms. This result is

sup-Table 3: Comparison of factor and depressive symptom scores in the study sample according to the four family interactions types

Means of Factor Scores and Depressive Symptom Scores Factors Cluster 1 Supervised group

(n = 709) (29.66%) Cluster 2 Disciplined group (n = 324) (13.56%) Cluster 3 Nurtured group (n = 979) (40.96%) Cluster 4 Conflict group(n = 378) (15.82%) F value Scheffe's test Supporting activities -0.8974 -0.1913 0.7472 -0.0513 708.48*** 3 > (2,4) > 1 Psychological control 0.0587 0.6512 -0.3404 0.2035 101.60*** 2 > (1,4) > 3 Parental discipline -0.4312 1.7356 -0.1528 -0.2821 756.54*** 2 > 3 > 4 > 1 Behavioral supervision 0.2130 -0.1448 0.0813 -0.4454 42.69*** (1,3) > 2 > 4 Family conflict -0.3735 -0.2199 -0.2810 1.6387 815.80*** 4 > (1,2,3) Depressive symptom score 10.56 11.94 9.61 11.79 113.297* ** (2,4) > 1 > 3 ***: p < 0.001

(8)

Appendix 1: Family interaction items and depressive symptoms

Interaction types and specific questions Scale Family activities:

The following questions are about your experiences in your family.

1. In the past week, did you talk with your parents? 1 = never

2. In the past week, did you eat with your parents? 2 = one or two days 3. In the past week, did you do housework with your parents? 3 = many days 4. In the past week, did your parents help you with your homework? 4 = every day 5. In the past week, did your parents play with you at home?

6. In the past week, did your parents take you on outings? Parental discipline:

When you misbehave, do your parents use the following ways to punish you?

1. Lock you in a room? 1 = never

2. Ban you from doing something you like? 2 = once or twice 3. Take your pocket money or toys away? 3 = many times 4. Make you do housework or write lines? 4 = always 5. Shout or swear at you?

6. Make you stand or kneel? 7. Hit you?

Parental support:

The following questions are about your experiences of getting along with your parents lately.

1. Do your parents encourage you when you are having trouble doing something? 1 = never 2. Do your parents praise you when you are a good boy/girl? 2 = once or twice 3. Do your parents comfort you when you are in a bad mood? 3 = many times 4. Do your parents look after you when you are sick? 4 = always 5. Do your parents listen to you when you have something to say?

6. Do your parents care about what happens at school? 7. Do your parents help you solve problems?

8. Do your parents tell you why they want you to do something? Psychological control:

The following questions are about your experiences of getting along with your parents lately.

1. Do your parents make you feel that they are always right? 1 = never 2. Do your parents often say to you 'Children don't understand things. Wait until you are older and then you will

understand?'

2 = once or twice 3. Do your parents remind you about past mistakes when you are in trouble? 3 = many times 4. Do your parents interrupt you when you are talking? 4 = always 5. Do your parents blame you for things that other people in the house have done?

6. When you and your parents disagree about something, are they not nice to you? Family conflict:

The following questions are about the relationships among your family members.

1. In the past month, did you have arguments with your brothers and sisters? 1 = never

2. In the past month, did you have physical fights with your brothers and sisters? 2 = one or two days 3. In the past month, did you have arguments with your parents or other elders? 3 = many days

4 = every day Behavioral supervision:

Please answer the following questions according to your experiences lately.

1. Do your parents know what you do in your free time? 1 = don't know a thing 2. Do your parents know what you do after school? 2 = know a little bit 3. Do your parents know who you mostly hang out with? 3 = almost everything 4. Do your parents know how you spend your pocket money? 4 = definitely know Depressive symptoms:

The following questions are about your experiences in the past two weeks:

1. In the past two weeks, did you not feel like eating even your favorite foods? 1 = never 2. In the past two weeks, did you feel sad or in a bad mood? 2 = once or twice 3. In the past two weeks, did you feel like crying for no reason? 3 = many times 4. In the past two weeks, did you find it difficult to carry out tasks?

5. In the past two weeks, did you feel very frightened? 6. In the past two weeks, did you have trouble sleeping? 7. In the past two weeks, did you lack motivation to do things?

(9)

ported by previous studies [18,19] and shows that chil-dren perceived positive support from parents and positive family interactions are related to fewer reported depres-sive symptoms.

Gender differences in children's self-reports of depressive symptoms

As shown in Table 1, girls reported higher depressive symptom scores than boys. Moreover, girls were signifi-cantly more likely to report that they feel sad, cry for no reason, and feel frightened. Boys were more likely to report that they find something hard to carry out than girls. These findings are similar to the results of previous research [11,19,20,37,38]. This is probably because women are more likely to experience strain, to have a low sense of mastery, and to engage in ruminative coping [37]. It is also possible that girls are more vulnerable to stressful life events than boys, such as living in a nonsupportive, single-father household [19,38]. However, no conclu-sions can be reached in regards to the mechanism between gender and children's self-reports of depressive symptoms. Furthermore, the results of this study do not indicate that the effect of children's perceived family inter-action types on childhood depressive symptoms is modi-fied by gender.

Study limitations

Our study had the following limitations. Firstly, the meas-ures in the study were reported by children alone. How-ever, although we cannot cross validate the answers with parents' or teachers' reports, the answers still represent the children's subjective perceptions of depressive symptoms and family interactions. Secondly, although an attempt was made to include a broad range of family interaction types, if the input items for factor analysis or cluster anal-ysis are different, this may result in different kinds of grouping. Nonetheless, the results still show that family interaction types are not restricted to merely two compo-nents but rather can have multiple compocompo-nents. In addi-tion, there is a definite relationship between types of children's perceived family interactions and children's self-reported depressive symptoms. However, as our study was cross-sectional in nature, we were unable to establish the direction of a possible causal association between family interactions and depressive symptoms. In other words, it may be possible that a child experiencing depres-sive symptoms will perceive their family environment and interactions as negative as opposed to negative family interactions leading to depressive symptoms. Finally, due to the sampling strategy used in this study, the results can be only generalized to public elementary school students in Taipei city and Hsinchu County in Taiwan.

Conclusion

In conclusion, there are five latent components that can be extracted from children's perceived family interactions: supporting activities, psychological control, parental dis-cipline, behavioral supervision, and family conflict. According to the factor scores of these five components, family interactions can be classified into four types: super-vised (29.66%), disciplined (13.56%), nurtured (40.96%) and conflict (15.82%). Children's perceptions of family interaction types are related to children's self-reports of depressive symptoms. Children from disci-plined families reported the most depressive symptoms and children from nurtured families reported the least depressive symptoms. Health and child care professionals should place more importance on the relationship between family interaction types and childhood depres-sive symptoms.

Competing interests

The author(s) declare that they have no competing inter-ests.

Authors' contributions

WW has contributed to writing the paper and conducting statistical analyses. CHK reviewed the literature and com-pleted the first draft. LLY has contributed to the study design and led the CABLE research team. TSHL provided suggestions for the discussion. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgements

Data used in this study was taken from the Child and Adolescent Behavior in Long-term Evolution (CABLE) project in 2003, which is a research project supported by the National Health Research Institutes, project number HP-092-SG-02.

References

1. World Health Organization: World Health Report 2001: Mental Health:

New Understanding, New Hope Geneva; 2001.

2. Eisenberg L: Psychiatry and health in low-income populations.

Compr Psychiatry 1997, 38:69-73.

3. Homedes N: The disability-adjusted life year (DALY):

defini-tion, measurement and potential use. In Human Capital

Devel-opment and Operations Policy Working Paper Washington, DC: The

World Bank; 1995.

4. Holden C: Global survey examines impact of depression.

Sci-ence 2000, 288:9-40.

5. Bethell CD, Read D, Blumberg SJ: Mental health in the United

States: health risk behaviors and conditions among persons with depression – new Mexico, 2003. MMWR 2005, 54:981-991.

6. Simon GE, Katon WJ, Lin EH, Ludman E, Vonkorff M, Ciechanowski P, Young BA: Diabetes complications and depression as

pre-dictors of health service costs. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 2005, 27:344-351.

7. Birmaher B, Ryan Neal, Williamson DE, Brent DA, Kaufman J, Dahl RE, Perel J, Nelson B: Childhood and adolescent depression: A

review of the past 10 years (Part I). J Am Acad Child Adolesc

Psy-chiatry 1996, 35:1427-1439.

8. Kau MY, Wu CI, Lue BH: The Relationships between Inept

Parenting and Adolescent Depression Dimension and Con-duct Behaviors. Chinese J Fam Med 1998, 8:11-21.

(10)

Publish with BioMed Central and every scientist can read your work free of charge "BioMed Central will be the most significant development for disseminating the results of biomedical researc h in our lifetime."

Sir Paul Nurse, Cancer Research UK Your research papers will be:

available free of charge to the entire biomedical community peer reviewed and published immediately upon acceptance cited in PubMed and archived on PubMed Central yours — you keep the copyright

Submit your manuscript here:

http://www.biomedcentral.com/info/publishing_adv.asp

BioMedcentral 9. Department of Statistics, Ministry of The Interior in Taiwan: The

National Survey of Physical and Mental Health of Youth in Taiwan Taipei;

1999.

10. Aronen ET, Soininen M: Childhood depressive symptoms

pre-dict psychiatric problems in young adults. Can J Psychiatry 2000, 45:465-470.

11. Lewinsohn PM, Roberts RE, Seely JR, Rohde P, Gotlib IH, Hops H:

Adolescent psychopathology: α Psychosocial Risk Factors for Depression. J Abnorm Psycho 1994, 103:302-315.

12. Levendosky AA, Okun A, Parker JG: Depression and

maltreat-ment as predictors of social competence and social problem-solving skills in school-age children. Child Abuse Negl 1995, 19:1183-1195.

13. Messer SC, Gross AM: Childhood depression and family

inter-action: A naturalistic observation study. J Clin Child Psychol

1995, 24:77-88.

14. Burbach DJ, Borduin CM: Parent-child relations and the

etiol-ogy of depression: A review of methods and findings. Clin

Psy-chol Rev 1986, 6:133-153.

15. Cole DA, Rehm LP: Family interactions patterns and childhood

depression. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1986, 14:297-314.

16. Peluso PR, Peluso JP, Kern RM, Kern RM, White JAF: A

Compari-son of Attachment Theory and Individual Psychology: A Review of the Literature. J Couns Dev 2004, 82:139-145.

17. Armsden GC, McCauley E, Greenberg MT, Burke PM, Mitchell JR:

Parent and peer attachment in early adolescent depression.

J Abnorm Child Psychol 1990, 18:683-697.

18. Windle M: Temperament and social support in adolescence:

Interrelations with depressive symptoms and delinquent behaviors. J Youth Adolesc 1992, 21:1-21.

19. Patten CA, Gillin JC, Farkas AJ, Gilpin EA, Berry CC, Pierce JP:

Depressive symptoms in California adolescents: family structure and parental support. J Adolesc Health 1997, 20:271-278.

20. Liu YL: Parent-child interaction and children's depression: the

relationships between Parent-Child interaction and chil-dren's depressive symptoms in Taiwan. J Adolesc 2003, 26:447-57.

21. Stein D, Williamson DE, Birmaher B, Brent DA, Kaufman J, Dahl RE, Perel JM, Ryan ND: Parent-child bonding and family

function-ing in depressed children and children at high risk and low risk for future depression. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2000, 39:1387-1395.

22. Olson DH, Douglas HS, Cindy SR: Circumplex model of marital

and family system- Cohesion and adaptability dimensions, family types, and clinical application. Fam Process 1979, 18:3-28.

23. Lavee Y, Olson DH: Family types and response to stress. J

Mar-riage Fam 1991, 53:786-799.

24. Lamborn SD, Mounts NS, Steinberg L, Dornbusch SM: Patterns of

Competence and Adjustment among Adolescents from Authoritative, Authoritarian, Indulgent, and Neglectful Families. Child Dev 1991, 62:1049-1065.

25. Maccoby EE, Martin JA: Socialization in the context of the

fam-ily: Parent-child interaction. In Handbook of child psychology:

Socialization, personality, and social development Volume 4. 4th edition.

Edited by: Mussen PH, Hetherington EM. New York: Wiley; 1983:1-101.

26. CABLE Research Team: Child and adolescent behavior in long-term

evo-lution (CABLE): a school-based health lifestyle study. Annual Report 2003– 2004 Taipei; 2005.

27. Yen LL, Chen LW, Lee SH, Hsiao CH, Pan LY: Child and

adoles-cent behavior in long-term evolution (CABLE): a school-based health lifestyle study. IUHPE-Promotion and Education

2002:33-40.

28. Kovacs M: Rating scales to assess depression in school-aged

children. Acta Paedopsychiatr 1980, 46(5-6):305-315.

29. Faulstich ME, Carey MP, Ruggiero L, Enyart P, Gresham F:

Assess-ment of depression in childhood and adolescence: an evalua-tion of the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale for Children (CES-DC). Am J Psychiatry 1986, 143:1024-1027.

30. Kaiser HF: An index of factorial simplicity. Psychometrika 1974,

39:31-36.

31. Johnson RA, Wichern DW: Clustering distance methods, and

ordination. In Applied Multivariate Statixtical Analysis 5th edition.

Upper Saddle River, New Jersey: Prentice Hall; 2002:668-747.

32. Chao RK: Beyond Parental Control and Authoritarian

Parenting Style: Understanding Chinese Parenting Through the Cultural Notion of Training. Child Dev 1994, 65:1111-1119.

33. Chen F, Luster T: Factors Related to Parenting Practices in

Taiwan. Early Child Dev Care 2002, 172:413-430.

34. Lin WY, Wang JW: Taiwan parent's beliefs of child-raising:

severely teaching or physical and oral punishment. Indigenous

Psychological Research in Chinese Societies 1995, 3:2-92.

35. McCord J: Unintended consequences of punishment. Pediatrics 1996, 98:832-834.

36. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Girgus JS, Seligman ME: Learned helplessness

in children: a longitudinal study of depression, achievement, and explanatory style. J Adolesc 1986, 51(2):435-442.

37. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Larson J, Grayson C: Explaining the gender

difference in depressive symptoms. J Personality Soc Psychology

1999, 77:1061-1075.

38. Ge X, Lorenz FO, Conger RD, Elder GH, Simons RL: Trajectories

of stressful life events and depressive symptoms during ado-lescence. Dev Psychol 1994, 30:467-483.

Pre-publication history

The pre-publication history for this paper can be accessed here:

http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2458/7/116/pre pub

數據

Table 1: Descriptive statistics for depressive symptoms in the study sample
Table 2: Rotated factor structure of family interactions in the study sample
Table 3: Comparison of factor and depressive symptom scores in the study sample according to the four family interactions types

參考文獻

相關文件

6 《中論·觀因緣品》,《佛藏要籍選刊》第 9 冊,上海古籍出版社 1994 年版,第 1

The first row shows the eyespot with white inner ring, black middle ring, and yellow outer ring in Bicyclus anynana.. The second row provides the eyespot with black inner ring

Breu and Kirk- patrick [35] (see [4]) improved this by giving O(nm 2 )-time algorithms for the domination and the total domination problems and an O(n 2.376 )-time algorithm for

You are given the wavelength and total energy of a light pulse and asked to find the number of photons it

Chen, The semismooth-related properties of a merit function and a descent method for the nonlinear complementarity problem, Journal of Global Optimization, vol.. Soares, A new

volume suppressed mass: (TeV) 2 /M P ∼ 10 −4 eV → mm range can be experimentally tested for any number of extra dimensions - Light U(1) gauge bosons: no derivative couplings. =&gt;

incapable to extract any quantities from QCD, nor to tackle the most interesting physics, namely, the spontaneously chiral symmetry breaking and the color confinement.. 

Internal Control and Management Practices in Non-Profit Organisations Northern India Regional Council of the Institute of Chartered Accounts of India Terrance S. Demczur