• 沒有找到結果。

樂觀的性別差異—雅虎奇摩財經新聞民調的證據

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "樂觀的性別差異—雅虎奇摩財經新聞民調的證據"

Copied!
79
0
0

加載中.... (立即查看全文)

全文

(1)

管理科學系

博 士 論 文

N0. 047

樂觀的性別差異—雅虎奇摩財經新聞民調的證據

Gender Difference in Optimism- Evidence from Yahoo! Kimo

Taiwan’s Business Poll Center

研 究 生:張玲玲

指導教授:李經遠 教授

(2)

管理科學系

博 士 論 文

N0. 047

樂觀態度性別差異-雅虎奇摩財經新聞民調的證據

Gender Difference in Optimism- Evidence from Yahoo! Kimo

Taiwan’s Business Poll Center

研 究 生:張玲玲

研究指導委員會:黃仁宏 教授

蔡彥卿 教授

李經遠 教授

指導教授:李經遠 教授

(3)

樂觀態度性別差異—雅虎奇摩財經新聞民調的證據

Gender Difference in Optimism- Evidence from

Yahoo! Kimo Taiwan’s Business Poll Center

研 究 生:張玲玲

Student:Ling-Ling Chang

指導教授:李經遠

Advisor:Gin-Yuan Lee

國 立 交 通 大 學

管 理 科 學 系

博 士 論 文

A Thesis

Submitted to Department of Management Science College of Management

National Chiao Tung University in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements

for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy

In

Management

November 2009

Hsin-Chu, Taiwan, Republic of China

(4)

樂觀態度性別差異—奇摩網站財經新聞民調的證據

研究生:張玲玲

指導教授

李經遠

國立交通大學管理科學系博士班

摘 要 社會情緒(social mood)會影響個人情緒及其對財務風險的看法,進而影響風險決策 的選擇,過去女性在財務相關決策上被認為比男性更規避風險(risk averse)、自信程度較 低且較悲觀。本研究以奇摩網站財經新聞的民調中心資料進行樂觀態度性別差異研究, 發現在社會情緒悲觀時,女性比男性更悲觀,而社會情緒樂觀時則視投資標的是否為股 票而定。本研究發現社會情緒若悲觀則女性比男性更悲觀,而社會情緒樂觀下,與股市 有關議題女性也呈現較悲觀,與前人研究結論相同(Jacobsen, Lee, & Marquering, 2008)。

但若非股市相關議題,則社會情緒樂觀,女性比男性更樂觀。

(5)

Gender difference in Optimism- evidence from

Yahoo! Kimo Taiwan’s Business News Poll Centre

Student:Ling-Ling Chang Advisor:Gin-Yuan Lee

Department of Management Science

National Chiao Tung University

ABSTRACT

“Social Mood” is one factor that affects people’s emotions and their perspectives in risk

management, which then impact their final decisions on actual investments. It is believed that

women, in general, are more risk aversive, pessimistic and have lower confidence than men.

This study investigated the optimism level difference between women and men using data

from Yahoo! Kimo Taiwan’s Business News Poll Centre. It is proven in this study that

social mood does have significant effects on individual’s emotional responses. Women

respond more pessimistically than men when the social mood is pessimistic; if the social

mood is optimistic, women also respond more pessimistically than men on stock-related

topics. These finding are consistent with Jacobsen, Lee, & Marquering (2008) experiment.

However, women respond more optimistically on non-stock related topics when the social

mood is optimistic, which are inconsistent with previous evidence.

(6)

誌謝

本論文得以順利完成首須感謝張保隆老師、黃仁宏老師、楊千老師、蔡彥卿老師及 李經遠老師的寶貴意見,使本論文更臻嚴謹與完善。 博士班的入學與求學過程有太多要感謝的人,回首一路走來的路上,老師們的溫暖 與提攜讓我終生難忘-黃仁宏老師溫文儒雅的風範與鼓勵、許和鈞老師的真誠熱情、謝 國文老師的認真指導與不倦教誨、楊千老師的智慧與寬容,王耀德老師的溫柔協助,李 經遠老師的良善與敦厚、蔡彥卿老師的無私與治學嚴謹的態度….,點點滴滴讓學生難 以忘懷,真幸運讓我遇上了這麼多貴人與高人,才能讓我順利入學、快樂又感激的求學。 對於管科所和交大,一直有著深厚的情感,身為交大管科所的學生,心中深覺幸運又幸 福,更感謝上天對我的厚愛;對於台大更有無盡的感激,雖非台大的註冊學生,但老師 仍不吝指導與教誨並無私的分享經驗。 求學過程中可用驚險刺激來形容,繁重的課業壓力、難度頗高的資格考,投稿國外 期刊的歷程,讓我又累又怕-擔心學科、擔心資格考、擔心論文投稿,求學以來,博士 生涯最難捱,所幸老師們的鼓勵與經驗分享,同學的感情互相支持-特別是彬彬及學長 俊閎,一路走來加油聲不絕。 五年多來最常被我忽略的家人,總是在背後默默的支持我、成就我-年邁的雙親總 忘記我已長大而依舊呵護備至,盡心照料家庭的先生讓我心無旁騖的專心學業,而提早 學會不吵媽媽看書的小女,總是在我又睏又累時送上她親手完成的小禮物幫我打氣,並 告訴我她有多愛我。 我的環境充滿著愛,老師、同學、家人,都以他們愛交織成一張愛的氛圍網-協助

(7)

Table of Contents

摘 要 ... i ABSTRACT ... ii 致謝 ... iii Table of Contents ... iv List of Tables ... v 1. Introduction ... 1 2. Literature Review ... 7

2.1 Gender difference in risk aversion ... 7

2.2 Gender difference in confidence ... 8

2.3 Gender differences in information processing ... 12

2.4 Association between risk and emotion ... 14

3. Research method ... 18

3.1 Data source ... 18

3.2 Selection of the research problem and explanation of investigation topic and social mood ... 19

3.3 Data coding method and testing methods ... 37

4. Research Results ... 39

5. Conclusions and suggestions ... 56

Reference ... 59

Appendix Investigation Questions ... 66

(8)

List of Tables

Table 1 Summary of Investigation question, period and social mood ... 36

Table 2 Number and gender of participants ... 39

Table 3 Cross table of Question 1: “After the political turbulence in September 2006, would you anticipate Taiwan’s stock market to rise or fall?” ... 41

Table 4 Cross table of Question 2: “What do you think of the real estate business in 2007 as compared to 2006?” ... 42

Table 5 Cross table of Question 3: “Will Taiwan’s stock market break the 10,000-point threshold?” ... 43

Table 6 Cross table of Question 4: “Should the government liberalize investment to mainland China?” ... 44

Table 7 Cross table of Question 5: “After the presidential election, will you invest in Taiwanese stocks (or Taiwanese funds)?” ... 45

Table 8 Cross table of Question 6: “Do you want mainland investors to invest in Taiwan Stock Market?” ... 46

Table 9 Cross table of Question 7: “With high inflationary pressure and steep falls in Taiwan Stock Market, what do you think?” ... 47

Table 10 Cross table of Question 8: “Do you think the price of oil will rise to $200 per barrel before the end of the year?” ... 48

Table 11 Cross table of Question 9: “Are you worried that labor insurance finances are unstable and that you won’t be able to receive old age payments?” ... 49

Table 12 Cross table of Question 10: “Do you believe that this stock market disaster will follow in the footsteps of the Great Depression in the

(9)

Table 14 Cross table of Question 12: “Do you think that gold prices will continue to rise in the future?” ... 52

(10)

1. Introduction

Gender differences have become an important issue in social sciences research. As

the social status of women rises with higher education and disposable income, their

behavior as consumers and investors is becoming more valued. Consequently, studies

done on gender differences have become more popular. Previous studies on gender

differences in investment or financial issues have focused on whether females are more

risk aversive than men, on differences in investment strategy confidence, and differences

in information processing in the decision-making process. In the past, studies done on

gender differences in investments and financial issues have mostly indicated that women

are more risk aversive than men; investments with lower risks involved were often

preferred by women (Bajtelsmit, Bernasek, & Jianakopolos, 1999; Bernasek & Shwiff,

2001; Bajtelsmit & VanDerhei, 1997; Jianakoplos & Bernasek, 1998; Schubert, 2006;

Sunden & Surette, 1998), while males have more confidence in investment decisions.

When it comes to decision making, women tend to put more factors into consideration

than men do before taking any action (Chung & Monroe, 2001; Graham, J., Stendardi,

Myers, & Graham M., 2002).

(11)

emotional reactions are generally considered to be the primary factors in irrationality

(Maital, Filer, & Simon, 1986; Shefrin, 2001). Previous research has generally concluded

that males appear to be more confident than females, even to the point of overconfidence

(Barber & Odean, 2001). Scholars have proposed reasons such as that risk attitudes are

impacted by the social role played by the individual in question. Other scholars have

suggested that the confidence of females is determined by the certainty of decision results;

more uncertain results lead to less confidence in females.

Other scholars have suggested that the comparatively risk-averse decisions made by

females may not be the result of differences in risk attitudes, but may instead be caused

by differences in information processing. Females tend to process broad amounts of

information and will incorporate uncertain cue information, thereby exhibiting risk

aversion. Males tend towards simplified heuristics in the decision-making process and are

more prone to be more careless in accepting information; females will make detailed

considerations of all usable clues. Scholars suggest that females will use all available

clues and information and are more suited to the processing of broad information; they

also have more positive evaluations of uncertain information, so it appears that women

are more risk-averse than are men.

(12)

difference towards risk are caused by emotional fluctuation. People are more optimistic

when they are in good moods and are more pessimistic when they are in bad moods;

consequently, moods may influence the probability of individuals investing in risky

assets. Schubert (2006) noted that risk assessment and behaviors are typically impacted

by cognitive and emotional responses, and emotional responses typically have a stronger

impact on attitude and behavior than do cognitive responses. Consequently, the mood of

a decision-maker and the time of decision will have an impact on decision selection.

Mood is affected by the state of the economy, unemployment rate, and inflation, which

can affect individuals’ decisions on purchases and investments. Nofsinger (2005)

suggested that social mood is a strong factor in decision making for the investors,

controlling the atmosphere of financial and economic activities. If there is a high degree

of optimism in a society, then more optimistic investors will be produced, which will

influence the views of decision-makers on risk and uncertainty. Consequently, many

investors will purchase stocks and more companies will become publicly traded, leading

to excessive market transactions. If social emotions become excessively high, extreme

overconfidence and excitement will lead to the bursting of the market bubble. As such,

(13)

trading volume or market activities. (Mitchell & Mulherin, 1994; Antweiler & Frank,

2004) Jocobsen, Lee and Marquering (2008) studied the optimism levels of men and

women by using consumer confidence data of 18 different country, they reported men are

strikingly more optimistic than women about the future performance of key economic

and financial indicators. . They suggested the severity of risk aversion may be influenced

by the differences in optimism level between men and women. The consumer confidence

index is a direct investigation, involving random sampling and the use of set questions to

track changes in consumer confidence; the consumer confidence index has the advantage

of directly exploring the intentions of people involved. However, sampling subjects may

not have considered economic problems in depth, and set questions make it more difficult

for respondents to express their views on economic incidents occurring at the time of

investigation.

This study emphasizes the confirmation of whether females are more pessimistic

than males with regard to expectations of specific economic incidents and, when

influenced by social emotions, whether there are differences in the optimism or

pessimism of males and females towards specific economic incidents. The data in this

study was obtained from the most popular investment portal site in Taiwan – Yahoo!

Kimo Taiwan Business News Poll Centre. Data was collected from June 2006 to the 1st

(14)

study was to identify the difference in reactions between men and women for positive and

negative financial events.

Yahoo! Kimo Taiwan is a popular portal site with a significant number of voting

users. Finance-related discussions were particularly popular at the time of data collection;

numerous “optimistic and pessimistic contexts” were included in the study. The

advantages of using data collected from Yahoo! Kimo Taiwan included: realistic

financial issue discussion, close connection to daily experience, voters were frequent

internet users that paid attention on financial news with social mood being somehow

affected by media and internet news, large quantity of samples, and people who voted

were the ones that had thought about the topics and issues already. Data collected from

the aforementioned samples are very valuable in terms of testing whether women possess

a more pessimistic view on certain financial events than men. Results supported the

hypothesis and are consistent with the results from Jacobsen’s (2008) study, showing that

women are indeed more pessimistic than men when the social mood is gloomy. However,

when social emotion is optimistic, the responses of both genders are determined by the

target investment. If the investment target is stocks, then female investors exhibit more

(15)

conclusions of previous research which suggest than women are more pessimistic than

men.

Results of this study show the difference in attitude between men and women on

financial topics, which come from the analysis of data from subjective internet surveys.

The results also explain why women are more conservative in financial and investment

decisions, which can serve as a reference to policy makers and the business world.

However, because the sample consisted of internet users, there may be limitations due to

generalization of the results. The following paragraphs of this paper are arranged as

follows: examines relevant literature, describes the research method, compiles the results

obtained from research, and the final chapter discusses results and provides suggestions

(16)

2. Literature Review

2.1 Gender difference in risk aversion

In general, females are said to be more apt than men to choose low-risk cases in the

face of risk decisions, preferring to make more conservative investment decisions and

exhibiting greater risk aversion. Previous research conclusions have also generally

supported the notion that females are more risk-averse than men and that they prefer

lower-risk investment portfolios (Bajtelsmit, Bernasek, & Jianakopolos, 1999; Bernasek

& Shwiff, 2001; Bajtelsmit & VanDerhei, 1997; Jianakoplos & Bernasek, 1998; Schubert,

2006; Sunden & Surette, 1998).

Bernasek and Shwiff (2001) collected detailed data on the family decisions of

faculty and administrators from five universities in the state of Colorado and used this

data to analyze the asset allocations of individual defined contribution plans. They found

that females included significantly fewer stocks in their retirement portfolios than did

males, supporting the argument that females are more risk-averse.

Jianakoplos and Bernasek (1998) used the 1989 Survey of Consumer Finances and

the theoretical framework developed by Friend and Blume (1975) to establish an index

(17)

Bajtelsmit et al. (1999) also used the 1989 Survey of Consumer Finances to test for

gender differences in the financial risk chosen in contribution retirement plans; their

results showed that women are more risk-averse than men.

Bajtelsmit and VanDerhei (1997) used a database of employee retirement funds and

population characteristics to test for the distribution of individual characteristics and

retirement funds. Their results showed that females prefer to invest in fixed-return

securities and also prefer more conservative investment strategies.

The research results of the scholars described above show that female investors tend

to prefer conservative or lower-risk investment portfolios. Consequently, this paper

hypothesizes that if the risk aversion of female investors is higher than that of males,

females should also be more pessimistic towards economic incidents than males.

2.2 Gender difference in confidence

In uncertain situations, decision-making is subject to the influence of many irrational

emotional factors (Kehnemen & Tversky, 1979). Gender, age, personality characteristics,

sources and interpretation of information, external stimuli, and emotional responses are

considered to be the primary factors in irrationality (Maital, Filer, & Simon, 1986;

Shefrin, 2001). Overconfidence refers to the excessive belief in the correctness of one’s

(18)

information, expert knowledge, and judgment that one possesses, leading to

underestimation of the potential risks and overestimation of one’s control (Shefrin and

Statman, 1994). Lichtenstein, Fischohoff and Phillips (1977, 1982) noted in research on

psychological behaviors in human decision-making that people tend to overestimate their

own powers of judgment, suggesting that the majority of people exhibit psychological

overconfidence. DeBondt and Thaler (1985) asserted that overconfidence will increase

the heterogeneity of information. This heterogeneity is the result of selective acceptance

of information based on one’s own experience principles–selectively discarding

undesired information, leading to the ignoring of potential risk. Daniel, Hirshleifer and

Subrahmanyam (1998) further suggested that overconfident investors will overemphasize

the accuracy of the information they have collected in the course of information

processing, and that they selectively believe information supporting their own past

investment decisions and that they ignore information not supporting their past

investment decisions, thereby maintaining their own confidence and self-esteem. Shefrin

and Statman (1985) introduced the “Disposition Effect,” which suggests that investors

tend to hold losing stocks and sell profitable stocks in order to avoid regret, because

(19)

that the stock will rise in price in the future. Consequently, investors will hold losing

stocks in anticipation of potentially compensating for losses. Odean (1998) noted that

overconfidence in investors will typically lead to early realization of the profitable

portions of investment portfolios and the holding of losing portions, meaning that

overconfident investors will realize profits immediately and put off losses until later.

Such actions help investors to feel that the amount of their correct decisions exceeds the

amount of incorrect decisions, strengthening self-evaluations.

Previous conclusions drawn by research on gender differences in overconfidence

have supported the idea that males appear more confident or overconfident than females

in investment decisions (Barber and Odean, 2001). Potential reasons suggested by

scholars include that personal risk attitudes and personalities are influenced by the social

role played (Voelz, 1985). Consequently, when making risk decisions, males appear more

confident or overconfident compared to females. Scholars have also suggested that

gender differences in self-confidence are derived from the certainty or uncertainty of

decision results; females appear to be less confident when decisions are less certain

(Lenney, 1977).

Evidence suggesting overconfidence in men includes that presented by Odean

(20)

portfolios than other investors in identical situations of risk and that higher transaction

frequencies also result. The overconfidence hypothesis suggests that if investors are

overconfident, then profit will be accompanied by more proactive transaction behaviors.

Barber, Odean and Zheng (2000) found in testing the overconfidence model that males

are more prone to overconfidence than females. Overconfident investors perform

transactions with excessive frequencies, leading to losses in net returns; research results

did show that the net returns of males are lower than those of females after adjusting for

risk. Barber and Odean (2001) performed research after separating data on 35,000

accounts in a brokerage firm offering large sum discounts; their results showed that males

perform a greater number of transactions, suggesting that males are significantly more

prone to overconfidence than females.

Lenney (1977) asserted that gender differences in confidence are the result of

certainty (or uncertainty) in decision results. When results are uncertain or unclear,

females seem to be more prone to underestimating their own capabilities; conversely,

when the results of decisions are clear and immediate, the confidence of females appears

to be no different from that of males. The results of decisions on the stock market are

(21)

It can be seen from the inferences described above that males appear more confident

and willing to invest in riskier investment decision targets or appear to have more

optimistic attitudes due to expectations of social roles even though they are uncertain of

the decision results.

2.3 Gender differences in information processing

Due to uncertainties in decision situations and limitations in information processing

cognitive abilities, actual decision making processes pursue “satisfactory” decision

results rather than “optimal” decision results. There is more of a tendency to use rules of

thumb rather than objective probabilities in making judgments when processing large

amounts of information (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979); decision makers evaluate the

frequency or probability of an event based on representative heuristics or available

heuristics. Meyers-Levey (1986) developed a selectivity model to test for gender

differences in information processing and proposed that males tend to use simple

heuristics in the decision making process more than females do and are also more prone

to roughly accept external information and selectively perform information processing;

females will perform detailed consideration of all usable clues prior to decision making.

Consequently, the researchers concluded that females have more of a tendency to use all

available clues and information and are adept at processing broad amounts of information.

(22)

linking a selectivity model and examination of literature on investment decisions,

Graham, J., Stendardi, Myers, & Graham M. (2002) proposed that women may make

more risk-averse decisions not because of differences in risk aversion, but rather because

of differences in information processing. Women tend to process broader information and

include uncertain clue information, thereby appearing risk averse.

Evidence from empirical research includes a study by Chung and Monroe (1998),

who studied gender differences in information processing based on information auditing

methods of accounting students. The study found that females use information more

broadly than do males and appear to have higher evaluations for uncertain information.

O'Donnell and Johnson (2001) performed research on gender differences in the

information usage of employees of auditing firms, and found that significant interaction

occurs between task complexity and gender. These results show that males are more

efficient at information processing in low-complexity tasks, while females seem to be

more efficient at processing information in high-complexity tasks. These results show

that females are more adept than males at processing broad and complex information.

Burke (2001) found that gender differences in information processing are more

(23)

emphasize broad information sources, while there are no gender differences in the

information processing methods in partner-level and other high-level employees.

It can be seen that differences in attitudes towards risk arise due to different

information processing methods in males and females. Females take into account more

uncertain information and so appear more risk-averse than males.

2.4 Association between risk and emotion

In general, expectations towards risk are reflected by cognitive evaluations of risk as

expressed through emotion. Loewenstein, Weber, Hsee, & Welch (2001) noted that

gender differences in risk attitudes are caused by emotional variables. People are more

optimistic in their views when they are in good moods and are more pessimistic when

they are in bad moods. Consequently, emotions may impact the probability of individuals

investing in risky assets. Schubert (2006) noted that, in general, risk assessment and

behavior are subject to the influence of cognitive and emotional responses; the influence

of emotional responses typically has a greater impact on attitudes and behavior. As a

result, the mood of the decision maker and the decision time at the time of the decision

will impact choices made in a decision. Emotions are easily influenced by factors such as

economic trends, unemployment rate, and inflation, and may change personal

consumption and financial decisions. Elster (1998) noted that happy people are more

(24)

Discouragement and risk aversion are directly correlated and increase the correlation

between worry and risk aversion. Pretty, Gleicher and Baker (1991) found that

individuals often make more detailed analysis and critical evaluations when they are in

bad moods. Forgas (1995) asserted that, according to the “Affect Infusion Model,”

positive emotions can increase risk tolerance, while negative emotions will reduce risk

tolerance. Individuals in good moods focus on positive information in their environments,

while individuals in bad moods will focus attention on negative characteristics. The

influence of emotions on decisions increases as situations become increasingly complex

and uncertain. Grable and Roszkowski (2008) found that happy emotions will raise

financial risk tolerance. Wurgler (2002) emphasized that links between investor

psychology and actual economic situations are necessary, and that media messages have

an impact on securities markets activities when economic events occur (Antweiler &

Frank, 2004), and that decision makers tend towards making judgments based on

conveniently obtainable information (such as events that recently occurred), which

become bases for decision-making due to deep impressions, during decision-making due

to available heuristics. Clarke and Statman (1998) found that short-term high returns in

(25)

nervousness more strongly than do males. Females are less sensitive to probabilities of

favorable circumstances than men and are more pessimistic towards gains. Consequently,

if the probabilities for favorable circumstances increase, females will not necessarily alter

their original selections; this phenomenon is referred to as risk aversion and also referred

to as pessimism. Fehr, Epper, Bruhin and Schubert (2007), however, found that females

make optimistic subjective probability appraisals in experiments regardless of favorable

or unfavorable circumstances when they are in good moods. In other words, females are

easily affected by good moods, while males are unaffected by good moods. The sum of

personal emotions forms social mood. Nofsinger (2005) once noted that individuals’

decisions, rather than being made independently and in isolation from society, are

determined by interactions between individuals and the social environment. As a result of

Nofsinger just mentioned “the level of social mood affects the economy because

participants interact”. Consequently, optimism and pessimism of the social mood will

affect the decisions of financial investors. Consequently, social mood directs the

atmosphere of financial and economic activities. When a high degree of optimism exists

in social mood, more optimistic investors are produced, which will affect the views of

decision-makers towards risk and uncertainty. Consequently, many investors will enter

the stock market, purchasing more stocks and public listings, leading company managers

(26)

and acquisition activities. If social mood becomes excessively high, extreme

self-confidence and excitement will lead to the bursting of the stock market bubble.

However, because not all financial decisions can be rapidly completed (for example,

Initial Public Offering or performing optimistic and active asset budgeting require time to

complete), so when social mood improves, many mergers and acquisition activities,

company public listings, and asset expenditures are completed only after a delay of a

number of months. By contrast, decisions to invest in asset markets can be executed

almost immediately, so the impact of social mood on stock markets is very strong;

consequently, the stock market itself is an excellent index for measuring social mood.

Jocobsen, Lee and Marquering (2008) once used the consumer confidence indices of 18

countries to perform research on gender differences in optimism and reached the

conclusion that males are more optimistic than females, and noted that risk aversion may

be influenced by gender differences in optimism. Social mood will influence the

optimism and views on risk of investors, thereby influencing investment decisions as a

whole. In general, females have stronger responses to fear and nervousness. When they

are impacted by negative social mood, females are more prone to adopting conservative

(27)

3. Research method

3.1 Data source

The study was based on data from surveys done on financial issues and topics from

the Yahoo! Kimo Business News Survey Centre. Testing was performed to observe if

there were differences in the optimistic and pessimistic reactions of males and females

when facing optimistic economic events and negative economic events.

The advantage of the data used in this study is that the financial issues are real, were

closely related to life at the time, and were the subject of concern for many people.

Respondents were internet users concerned about financial topics and were many in

number. However, the limitations of internet investigation include the sampling process,

sample representativeness and voluntary participants. As for the problem of sample

representativeness, Hewson et al. (2003) noted that many studies were established on

highly homogeneous selective samples, particularly when research topics emphasized

specific groups, thereby of necessity facing the problem of an inability to obtain a

complete sampling framework. Subjects required for the subject is concerned about the

financial and economic topics, that is, a homogeneity sample set. Thereby overcome the

problem of sample representativeness. As for the problem of voluntary samples,

according to annual report -Taiwan's Broadband Internet Usage Survey of 2009 from the

(28)

increased by 71% (about 14.19 million individuals). Divided by age range, 4.08 million

(about 28.81%) individuals aged 24 and under have used the internet, while 10.10 million

(about 71.19%) individuals over the age of 25 have also used the internet. Of internet

users, males constitute approximately 7.33 million (51.64%), while females constitute

approximately 6.86 million (48.36%); meaning that the number of males is slightly

higher than the number of females. The most commonly used type of website is portal

websites. With increases in internet penetration throughout Taiwan, because the Yahoo!

Kimo website is the most popular online portal and because there are many online

browsers and voters, error caused by voluntary samples probably be diluted and

decreased, preventing to a certain extent excessive deviation of the sample (Babbie,

1990).

3.2 Selection of the research problem and explanation of investigation topic and social mood

Investor mood indices can be divided into direct indices and indirect emotion indices.

Indices established based on regular surveys distributed by agencies to collect the views

of respondents on future economic trends (e.g. the Investor Sentiment Index or the

Consumer Sentiment Index) are referred to as direct indices, while indices that apply

(29)

emotional indices. Methods used in researching gender differences in investment can be

divided into experimental, investigative, and indirect data inference methods. The

experimental method involves researchers manipulating experimental variables to

understand the choice behaviors of test subjects in experimental situations. However,

because environmental situations are designed and are not actual dynamic economic

environments, because experimental subjects may not be familiar with the topic of

research, and because of the difficulty of obtaining large samples, deviations may be

produced due to limitations in the results. The investigative method involves actively

distributing surveys for investigation, allowing for the understanding of the subjective

intent of respondents; however, subjective emotional indices may be unable to capture

true intent due to random sampling and because it is unknown whether the sample is

concerned about economic topics. In addition, survey questions are set, and respondents

may not think about or realize set economic or financial problems. The purpose of this

type of investigation is to reflect long-term trends; it is more difficult for these

investigations to reflect attitudes towards specific topics at specific times. If gender

differences are understood by way of indirect inference from collected individual

transaction behaviors rather than through direct association, while specific outward

conduct can be observed, an excess of impact factors leads to weaker evidence capacity.

(30)

specific economic incidents than do men, this study determined to employ the direct

survey results of an online poll distributed from June 2006 to 1st Quarter 2009 in the

financial news opinion center of the largest portal website in Taiwan – Yahoo! Kimo.

After filtering and downloading opinion surveys appropriate for analysis of topics related

to optimism and pessimism, research was performed on whether males and females have

identically optimistic responses in the face of optimistic or pessimistic economic events.

The research period of this study spanned from the second half of 2006 to the first

part of 2009, which included large economic or political incidents that impacted the stock

market or the economy, including protests demanding a presidential recall, massive

inflation of international, food, and goods prices, the financial crisis caused by American

subprime mortgages and the bankruptcy of Lehman Brothers, and the Taiwan’s

presidential election in 2008; this period and these events are of great value in examining

gender differences in the responses of internet users towards financial topics. However,

due to the emphasis on gender differences in financial topics, the questions chosen had to

include selections to differentiate optimists and pessimists. In addition, the research topic

had to be able to connect to economic news events of the time in order to differentiate

(31)

The investigative problems selected for this study included 12 questions. The

questions and answers were directly downloaded from the Yahoo! Kimo website.

Relevant news events that occurred in 12 months before the research period were

searched using the news database of the China Times series (China Times, Commercial

Times, China Times Express) in order to understand the background of the investigation

problems; social mood was determined to be optimistic or pessimistic based on the

attitudes exhibited by the media towards the topic. The backgrounds, social mood

categorizations, and definitions of optimistic and pessimistic responses are explained as

follows:

1. After the political turbulence in September 2006, would you

anticipate Taiwan’s stock market to rise or fall?

(20060828-20060904)

In 2006, due to charges of corruption involving President Chen Shui-bian

and his family, cries for a presidential recall increased and a series of

anti-corruption and anti-Chen movements1 occurred. The political

situation was highly unstable, and the media fought to report on relevant

conflicts. Investor confidence in the Taiwanese economy was damaged.

      

1 News reports from 27 August 2006 – [Commercial Times/Fortune/Page C1], 29 August 2006 –

(32)

According to Taiwanese investor emotional indices at the time, political

issues were the most important factor influencing Taiwanese markets; an

investor emotional index fell to -92.822, the lowest point of the year. We

therefore considered that the social mood was pessimistic. Observations

voting “rise” in the stock market were considered optimistic, while “fall”

was considered pessimistic.

2. Will Taiwan’s stock market break the 10,000-point threshold?

(20061228-20070111)

A Taiwan’s real estate market conditions quarterly report released by the

Architecture and Building Research Institute, Ministry of the Interior

showed that Property transactions in Taiwan 2006 broke records standing

since 1998; 70% of the news with regard to real estate was positive and

uplifting. According to a home demand movement investigation

published by the Institute for Physical Planning & Information in the 4th

quarter of 2006, the combined confidence of home purchasers across the

nation reached 110.27. Home purchasers had optimistic confidence in

      

(33)

home prices, particularly exhibiting increasing confidence in mid- to

long-term trends. The atmosphere investors were exposed to exhibit an

optimistic atmosphere; we thus considered that the social mood was

optimistic. Votes for that the real estate market “will improve” were

considered optimistic, while votes for that the real estate market “will

worsen” were considered pessimistic.3

3. Will Taiwan’s stock market break the 10,000-point threshold?

(20070424-20070507)

Taiwan’s stock market repeatedly rose; the stock index rose from 7,344

to 8,075 between March 5 and April 12 in 2007, constituting a 10%

increase within 6 weeks. The index stayed around 8,000 until the end of

April 2007, this is the first time the index had returned to 8,000 in 7 years.

Together with continual company mergers and acquisitions in Taiwan,

bull markets were pervasive in the world, stimulating the imaginations of

investors. The total transaction value of Taiwanese starts in March was

      

3 The home price confidence composite score is calculated from weighted views on home prices from three

months to 1 year later. The score ranges from 0 to 200. A score of 100 represents there are identical proportions of bullish investors and bearish investors. A score above 100 represents that there is a higher proportion of bullish investors; a score below 100 represents the opposite. The meaning of the home price confidence composite score lies in demand growth and short-term support strength for home prices. If the confidence score exceeds 100, home buyers are psychologically prepared for increases in home prices, providing support for increases in home prices; a score under 100 may pose a challenge to home price stability (Extracted from Institute for Physical Planning & Information).

(34)

the second-highest within 10 months, just behind January. As the stock

market is a positive measurement indicator of investor mood, we

considered social mood to be optimistic. Votes for “yes” were considered

optimistic, while votes for “no” were considered pessimistic.

4. Should the government liberalize investment to mainland China?

(20070807-20070820)

According to a 3 June 2009 China Times editorial, “the KMT, acting

through 44 legislators including Li Jizhu and Lei Jing, proposed a draft

amendment to the Cross-strait Ties statute, which proposes that products

or operations currently being mass-produced in the mainland China

region will not be categorized as illegal as long as they do not contravene

international treaties. However, mainland affairs committee chairman

Chen Ming-Tong believed that, “This proposal will lead to complete

liberalization of Taiwanese investment in the mainland, which will have

a huge impact on industry. Taiwanese competitive advantage will

disappear in the face of liberalization.”

(35)

entering the mainland market in order to avoid capital usage limitations.

Capital and companies continually left Taiwan, causing discussion on

whether limitations on mainland investment should be loosened to slow

this trend as well as the pain of worsening unemployment. Optimists

believed that liberalized investment in the mainland could reduce

outward capital flow and allow Taiwan to enjoy the fruits of economic

development on the mainland, while pessimists believed that total

liberalization would lead to outward movement of companies, leading to

a further downturn in Taiwanese industry and exacerbating the problem

of domestic unemployment.4 We classified this social mood as neutral.

Votes for “should” liberalize were categorized as optimistic, while votes

for “should not” liberalize were categorized as pessimistic.

5. After the presidential election, will you invest in Taiwanese stocks (or

Taiwanese funds)? (20080401-20080410)

After the presidential election results were revealed on March 22, 2008,

the Taiwanese stock market immediately rose by 524 points the next

business day. After the presidential election, a short-term inflow of

      

4From 3 June 2007 - [Commercial Times/News in Policy Focus/ Page A2] editorial: Comment on

(36)

capital and a sharp increase in potential trade and business activities with

Three-Links between Taiwan and Mainland China brought hope and

prosperity to people. Property transactions were hot again5 and investors

were expecting the stock market index to break the 10,000-point

threshold. At the same time, however, a global inflation crisis was caused

by an explosion of prices in oil, food, and materials. Moreover, people

were concerned with the potential impact of America’s subprime

mortgage crisis on Taiwan’s stock market. The unemployment rate stayed

high, potentially impacting Taiwanese stocks. High returns and no returns

were comingled. We classified the social mood as neutral. Votes for “I

will” were considered optimistic, while votes for “I won’t” were

considered pessimistic; other options were considered to be neutral.

6. Do you want mainland investors to invest in Taiwan Stock Market?

(20080428-20080510)

The Three-Links created many potential market opportunities, causing

domestic stock markets and real estate markets to operate energetically. It

(37)

was anticipated that opening mainland investment in Taiwanese stocks

would lead to capital injection and creation of new cross-strait ties. News

media continued to report that potential mainland investments could

provide investors with many opportunities in the market. We therefore

considered the social mood to be optimistic. Votes for “yes” were

considered optimistic, while votes for “no” were pessimistic; other

responses were neutral.

7. With high inflationary pressure and steep falls in Taiwan Stock

Market, what do you think? (20080701-20080710)

In the first half of 2008, international prices of oil, food and materials all

increased significantly. The Consumer Price Index (CPI) soared and was

breaking historical records. The Academia Sinica modified a 2008 annual

economic forecast. The Consumer Price Index (CPI) experienced an

annual growth of 3.93%, constituting the highest figure in inflation

released by the various Taiwanese economic forecast agencies in 14

years.6 The Taiwan weighted stock price index fell from a high of 8,865

points following the presidential election to 7,076 points within three

      

6 From 11 July 2008 – [China Times/Topics/Page A5] Goods Price Index Increases by 4%, a 14-year High 

(38)

months (July 10), constituting a drop of 20%. Negative news was

pervasive, stock markets were continuously falling worldwide, and

investors lacked confidence in the future. In the previous month, the

government repeatedly encouraged foreign capital to return to Taiwan

and was determined to encourage insurance industry-type investment in

Taiwan stock market. Publicly listed companies were encouraged to

release treasury stocks, and inadequacies in the shareholdings of directors

and supervisors in publicly listed companies were to be covered in order

to save market confidence. Optimists believed that government

intervention would be able to cause the stock market to rise, while

pessimists believed that government intervention would not be able to

save the stock market in the face of the global crisis. We considered the

social mood to be pessimistic. Votes for “that’s great” were considered

optimistic, while votes for “that’s terrible” were considered pessimistic;

the rest were considered to be neutral.

8. Do you think the price of oil will rise to $200 per barrel before the

(39)

In 2008, oil prices continued to rise in the first half the year, reaching a

high of $147/barrel by June, marking a 172% rise over 18 months.

Increases in oil prices led to increases in commodities and goods prices,

inciting expectations of inflation and leading to hardship from increased

goods prices. Oil prices continued to rise, and the prices of electricity,

shipping, bulk materials, and commodities faced upward pressure,

leading to increased company costs and lowered profits; these forces led

to increased goods prices and reductions in consumer demand, a situation

creating hardship that was disadvantageous for economic growth. We

categorized the social mood as pessimistic. Votes for “will not” were

classified as optimistic, while votes for “will” were classified as

pessimistic; other responses were neutral.

9. Are you worried that labor insurance finances are unstable and that

you won’t be able to receive old age payments? (20080801-20080810)

The Legislative Yuan of Republic China reviewed and amended the

Labor Insurance Act in July 2008, considering the trends of an aging

society and low birthrates to establish a labor insurance pension system

with an income replacement rate of 1.55% and an initial insurance rate of

(40)

that outstanding payments had accumulated to a total of 7-8 hundred

billion NT dollars since July 2008. Performance had been continuously

negative for 6 months.7 The Council of Labor Affairs indicated that due

to excessively low labor insurance rates, together with an aging society,

labor insurance old-age payments have entered the mature stage. The

number of individuals receiving payments has increased on an annual

basis leading to a fiscal imbalance, has reach substantial loss. Without

other measures, the fund would go bankrupt 19 years after it began

operations. In responding to the advent of the aging era, important

reasons for the Council actively promoting the annuity system may lead

to losses to the labor insurance fund. There remains no adequate response

to the problem of low birthrates. Previously, elderly workers who had

low contribution rates could immediately enjoy the benefits of labor

insurance annuity. However, there was no appropriate principle of

fairness for user payments. A serious gap in insurance fee burdens has

resulted; citizens born in the 1980s must pay 1.8 times that of a citizen

(41)

receive old age payments. We classified the social mood as negative.

Votes for “not worried” were classified as optimistic, and votes for the

opposite were classified as pessimistic; other responses were neutral.

10. Do you believe that this stock market disaster will follow in the

footsteps of the 1930s Great Depression? (20081111-20081120)

In September 2008, the fall of Lehman Brothers created a global

financial tsunami, causing severe turbulence in the global financial

market. Countries like Taiwan, with its growth of the economy relying on

exports, potentially faced serious consequences. Negative news in

Europe and the U.S. warning of a depression caused panic among

investors. A global trend of unemployment also resulted. Domestic

industries faced with poor economic conditions were challenged by the

disappearance of orders and indefinite plant shutdowns. A new

unemployment high was recorded; companies that did not lay off

employees instead chose to reduce production or adopt unpaid leave

policies. Though employees did not lose their jobs in full, they were

essentially semi-unemployed or approaching unemployment.8 Economic

      

8 From 9 November 2008 – [Commercial Times/Global View/Page C3] New York Times; With the

Financial Crisis Worsening, is an Economic Crisis in the Making? 10 November 2008 – [Commercial Times/News in Focus/Page A2] Editorial: An Economic Freeze Nightmare Approaches by the Day

(42)

growth in Southeast Asia and other developing countries is primarily

reliant on consumption in the American and European regions. An

economic recession in the United States poses serious problems for

export-dependent countries, Taiwan among them. The American

financial crisis severely damaged global stock markets, causing

instabilities in international financial markets. Former Federal Reserve

chairman Alan Greenspan described the situation as a financial crisis

seen only once in a hundred years. The media competed to report on

economic recessions in the United States and Europe, immersing

investors in pessimistic trend reports on a daily basis. The weighted

index in Taiwan stock markets fell approximately 2,600 points from a

high of 6,800 points at the beginning of September 2008, constituting a

39% drop. Based on figures from an investigation published in October

2008, domestic investment mood dropped from 102 points in April 2008

to a historical record low of -176.53 in October 2008. Economic

fundamentals failed to recover. We classified the social mood as

(43)

11. Are you continuing to invest in stocks? (20081212-20081222)

The bankruptcy of Lehman Brothers in September 2008 led to a global

financial crisis, causing stock markets to suffer great losses throughout

the world. The Taiwan stock market fell by more than 30%. However,

the three largest institutional investors purchased a total of $58 billion

NTD in the stock market from 8 December to 19 December in 2008,

causing investors to consider whether the Taiwanese stock market had hit

bottom and if investment could be considered. Losses in the stock market

reflected a pessimistic social mood. An investor mood index

investigation published in December 2008 also showed that investor

mood was highly pessimistic, with an index of -168.15 points. We

classified this social mood as pessimistic. Votes for “want to invest or are

investing” were classified as optimistic, and votes for the opposite were

considered pessimistic.

12. Do you think that gold prices will continue to rise in the future?

(20090301-20090310)

13. With an uncertain global economy, the value preservation benefits of

(44)

gold markets, causing the price of gold to climb9; a gold investment trend

also occurred around the same time domestically. Investors remained

skeptical due to the global economic recession. With considerations of

asset preservation and risk aversion, the capital of European and

American investors and opportunists flooded into the gold market,

causing the price of gold to climb quickly. Following the bankruptcy of

Lehman Brothers, global gold prices rose by over 30% over the previous

6 months. Media continued to report that gold prices were rising and that

investors were fighting to invest. We classified the social mood as

optimistic. Votes for “will” continue rising were classified as optimistic,

while votes for “will not” were considered pessimistic.

For convenient reference, categorizations of investigation questions, investigation

period, and social mood are summarized in Table 1. Of a total of 12 investigation

questions, from the perspective of social mood categorization, 4 were optimistic, 6 were

pessimistic, and 2 were neutral.

(45)

Table 1 Summary of Investigation question, period and social mood

No Questions Period SM

1 After the political turbulence in September 2006, would you anticipate Taiwan’s stock market to rise or fall?

20060828-20060904

P

2 What do you think of the real estate business in 2007 as compared to 2006?

20061228-20070111

O

3 Will Taiwan’s stock market break the 10,000-point threshold? 20070424-20070507

O

4 Should the government liberalize investment to mainland China?

20070807-20070820

N

5 After the presidential election, will you invest in Taiwanese stocks (or Taiwanese funds)?

20080401-20080410

N

6 Do you want mainland investors to invest in Taiwan Stock Market?

20080428-20080510

O

7 With high inflationary pressure and steep falls in Taiwan Stock Market, what do you think?

20080701-20080710

P

8 Do you think the price of oil will rise to $200 per barrel before the end of the year?

20080710-20080717

P

9 Are you worried that labor insurance finances are unstable and that you won’t be able to receive old age payments?

20080801-20080810

P

10 Do you believe that this stock market disaster will follow in the footsteps of the 1930s Great Depression?

20081111-20081120

P

11 Are you continuing to invest in stocks? 20081212-20081222

P

12 Do you think that gold prices will continue to rise in the future? 20090301-20090310

O

(46)

3.3 Data coding method and testing methods

Responses to questions were categorized into optimistic and pessimistic responses.

The coding method for the question responses involved assigning a positive whole

number greater than zero based on the degree of optimism and assigning a negative

whole number based on the degree of pessimism. A greater absolute value indicated

greater strength. Neutral responses were assigned a value of zero. For example, gains in

the first question include three different options with differing degrees of gains and also

three different options with different degrees of falls. The coding method involves

expressing gains (optimism) from lowest to highest by 1-3, while falls (pessimism) are

expressed from lowest to highest by (-1)-(-3). After developing a cross-table based on the

options and genders determined by the questions, Chi-square χ2 independence testing was

performed. In understanding the distribution of various options, a Mann-Whitney test

using two nonparametric independent samples was used to confirm whether females are

more pessimistic than males. The Mann-Whitney test, also called the rank sum test, is a

nonparametric test that compares two unpaired groups, is for assessing whether two

independent samples of observations come from the same distribution. It is one of the

best-known non-parametric significance tests and does not assume that the populations

(47)

groups. It is possible to have a tiny P value - clear evidence that the population medians

(48)

4. Research Results

The number of voters and gender distribution are shown in Table 2. It can be seen

from the table that voter numbers gradually increased with the financial news website

poll Center was established longer. Samples from 2006 averaged 4,000 individuals,

increasing to 8,000 in 2007 and 10,000 in 2008.

Table 2 Number and gender of participants

NO Total Male Female M% F%

Q1 3,547 2,303 1,244 65 35 Q2 5,428 3,187 2,241 59 41 Q3 6,919 4,394 2,525 64 36 Q4 10,873 5,979 4,894 55 45 Q5 10,544 5,116 5,428 49 51 Q6 13,160 6,396 6,764 49 51 Q7 7,899 3,856 4,043 49 51 Q8 15,387 6,284 9,103 41 59 Q9 5,879 2,731 3,148 46 54 Q10 13,662 7,001 6,661 51 49 Q11 9,511 5,305 4,207 56 44 Q12 8,363 3,791 4,572 45 55

(49)

Cross-table analysis and Mann-Whitney testing with two independent samples were

performed for questions individually. Cross-table results are as shown in Tables 3 – 14.

Chi-square (χ2) test and Mann-Whitney test results are organized in the format of Table

15 for convenience.

In the cross-table analysis for Table 3, the table for Question 1: “Following the

political turbulence in September 2006, would you anticipate Taiwan’s stock market to

rise or fall?” it can be seen that females are significantly higher than males in the

pessimistic options, while males are significantly higher than females in the pessimistic

options. Chi-square (χ2) test results showed significant differences (p<0.0001) in

pessimism and optimism between males and females. The social mood of this question

was pessimistic. It could be seen from the results of both pessimistic and optimistic

options that females are more pessimistic than males. Mann-Whitney testing results also

showed significant differences in pessimism and optimism between males and females

(50)

Table 3 Cross table of Question 1: “After the political turbulence in September 2006,

would you anticipate Taiwan’s stock market to rise or fall?”

Cross table of Q1 -3 -2 -1 1 2 3 Total M 645 373 190 220 341 534 2,303 M% 28.0% 16.2% 8.3% 9.6% 14.8% 23.2% 100.0% F 370 342 136 118 138 140 1,244 F% 29.7% 27.5% 10.9% 9.5% 11.1% 11.3% 100.0% Total 1,015 715 326 338 479 674 3,547 Total % 28.6% 20.2% 9.2% 9.5% 13.5% 19.0% 100.0%

Note. “M” denotes male, “F” denotes female;

In the cross-table analysis for Table 4, the table for Question 2: “What do you think

of the real estate business in 2007 as compared to 2006?”, it can be seen that the

percentage of males who expected real estate to drop was higher than the percentage of

females who expected the same; the proportion of females who believed that real estate

would gain in price was higher than the proportion of males who believed the same.

Chi-square (χ2) test results and Mann-Whitney test results both reached significant levels

(51)

greater than male optimism occurred, differing significantly from previous results that

suggested that females are more pessimistic.

Table 4 Cross table of Question 2: “What do you think of the real estate business in

2007 as compared to 2006?” Cross table of Q2 -2 -1 0 1 2 Total M 510 807 419 1,127 324 3,187 M% 16.0% 25.3% 13.1% 35.4% 10.2% 100.0% F 161 523 341 854 362 2,241 F% 7.2% 23.3% 15.2% 38.1% 16.2% 100.0% Total 671 1,330 760 1,981 686 5,428 Total % 12.4% 24.5% 14.0% 36.5% 12.6% 100.0%

Note. “M” denotes male, “F” denotes female;

In the cross-table analysis for Table 5, the table for Question 3: “Will Taiwan’s

stock market break the 10,000-point threshold?” it can be seen that the hope of males

that Taiwanese stocks would reach 10,000 points exceeded that of females. The

proportion of females who believed that Taiwanese stocks would not reach 10,000 points

(52)

reached significant levels (p<0.0001). The social mood of this question was categorized

as optimistic, and results showed that males are more optimistic than females.

Table 5 Cross table of Question 3: “Will Taiwan’s stock market break the 10,000-point

threshold?” Cross Table of Q3 -2 -1 1 2 Total M 847 2,199 768 580 4,394 M% 19.3% 50.0% 17.5% 13.2% 100.0% F 1,073 797 386 269 2,525 F% 42.5% 31.6% 15.3% 10.7% 100.0% Total 1,920 2,996 1,154 849 6,919 Total % 27.7% 43.3% 16.7% 12.3% 100.0%

Note. “M” denotes male, “F” denotes female;

In the cross-table analysis for Table 6, the table for the Question 4: “Should the

government liberalize investment to mainland China”, it can be seen that the percentage

(53)

levels (p<0.0001). The social mood of this question was neutral, and the results showed

that males are more optimistic than females.

Table 6 Cross table of Question 4: “Should the government liberalize investment to

mainland China?” Cross Table of Q4 -2 -1 1 2 Total M 960 583 1,261 3,175 5,979 M% 16.1% 9.8% 21.1% 53.1% 100.0% F 748 694 1,407 2,045 4,894 F% 15.3% 14.2% 28.7% 41.8% 100.0% Total 1,708 1,277 2,668 5,220 10,873 Total % 15.7% 11.7% 24.5% 48.0% 100.0%

Note. “M” denotes male, “F” denotes female;

In the cross-table analysis for Table 7, the table for Question 5: “After the

presidential election, will you invest in Taiwanese stocks (or Taiwanese funds)?” it can

be seen that the percentage of males who would choose to invest is higher than the

corresponding percentage of females, while the percentage of females who choose not to

invest is higher than the corresponding percentage of males; the percentage of females

(54)

percentage of males. Chi-square (χ2) test results and Mann-Whitney test results also

reached significant levels (p<0.0001). The social mood of this question was categorized

as neutral, and results showed that females are more pessimistic than males.

Table 7 Cross table of Question 5: “After the presidential election, will you invest in

Taiwanese stocks (or Taiwanese funds)?”

Cross Table of Q5 -1 0 1 Total M 1,480 594 3,042 5,116 M% 28.9% 11.6% 59.5% 100.0% F 1,704 1,035 2,689 5,428 F% 31.4% 19.1% 49.5% 100.0% Total 3,184 1,629 5,731 10,544 Total % 30.2% 15.4% 54.4% 100.0%

Note. “M” denotes male, “F” denotes female;

In the cross-table analysis for Table 8, the table for Question 6: “Do you want

mainland investors to invest in Taiwan Stock Market?”, it can be seen that the percentage

(55)

Chi-square (χ2) test results and Mann-Whitney test results both reached significant levels

(p<0.0001). The social mood of this question was optimistic, and the results showed that

females are more pessimistic than males.

Table 8 Cross table of Question 6: “Do you want mainland investors to invest in

Taiwan Stock Market?”

Cross Table of Q6 -1 0 1 2 Total M 2,284 319 1,790 2,003 6,396 M% 35.7% 5.0% 28.0% 31.3% 100.0% F 2,692 596 2,198 1,278 6,764 F% 39.8% 8.8% 32.5% 18.9% 100.0% Total 4,976 915 3,988 3,281 13,160 Total % 37.8% 7.0% 30.3% 24.9% 100.0%

Note. “M” denotes male, “F” denotes female;

In the cross-table analysis for Table 9, the table for Question 7: “With high

inflationary pressure and steep falls in Taiwan Stock Market, what do you think?” it can

be seen that a higher percentage of males than females selected the optimistic option,

“Excellent! There are cheap assets to scoop up.”; a higher percentage of females than

(56)

Chi-square (χ2) test results and Mann-Whitney test results both reached significant levels

(p<0.0001). The social mood of this question was pessimistic, and the results showed

that females are more pessimistic than males.

Table 9 Cross table of Question 7: “With high inflationary pressure and steep falls in

Taiwan Stock Market, what do you think?”

Cross Table of Q7 -1 0 1 Total M 976 1,558 1,322 3,856 M% 25.3% 40.4% 34.3% 100.0% F 1,095 2,073 875 4,043 F% 27.1% 51.3% 21.6% 100.0% Total 2,071 3,631 2,197 7,899 Total % 26.2% 46.0% 27.8% 100.0%

Note. “M” denotes male, “F” denotes female;

In the cross-table analysis for Table 10, the table for Question 8: “Do you think the

price of oil will rise to $200 per barrel before the end of the year?”, it can be seen that the

(57)

exceed $200/barrel. In addition, a greater percentage of females than males selected the

“I don’t know” option. Chi-square (χ2) test results and Mann-Whitney test results both

reached significant levels (p<0.0001). The social mood of this question was pessimistic,

and the results showed that females are more pessimistic than males.

Table 10 Cross table of Question 8: “Do you think the price of oil will rise to $200 per

barrel before the end of the year?”

Cross Table of Q8 -1 0 1 Total M 3,294 1,202 1,788 6,284 M% 52.4% 19.1% 28.5% 100.0% F 5,017 2,622 1,464 9,103 F% 55.1% 28.8% 16.1% 100.0% Total 8,311 3,824 3,252 15,387 Total % 54.0% 24.9% 21.1% 100.0%

Note. “M” denotes male, “F” denotes female;

It can be seen from Table 11, the cross-table analysis for Question 9: “Are you

worried that labor insurance finances are unstable and that you won’t be able to receive

old age payments?”, that a greater percentage of females than males had chosen the

(58)

wouldn’t be able to receive old age payments; the percentage of males who selected that

they were not worried about labor finance instability was higher than the corresponding

percentage of females. Chi-square (χ2) test results and Mann-Whitney test results both

reached significant levels (p<0.0001). The social mood of this question was pessimistic,

and the results showed that females are more pessimistic than males.

Table 11 Cross table of Question 9: “Are you worried that labor insurance finances are

unstable and that you won’t be able to receive old age payments?”

Cross Table of Q9 -2 -1 0 1 Total M 1,747 620 117 247 2,731 M% 64.0% 22.7% 4.3% 9.0% 100.0% F 2,287 590 99 172 3,148 F% 72.6% 18.7% 3.1% 5.5% 100.0% Total 4,034 1,210 216 419 5,879 Total % 68.6% 20.6% 3.7% 7.1% 100.0%

Note. “M” denotes male, “F” denotes female;

參考文獻

相關文件

It’s (between/next to) the church and the

Listen - Check the right picture striped hat polka dotted hat.. Which hat do

Play - Let’s make a big family How many people are in your family1. Write it

Sam: I scraped my knee and bumped my head.. Smith: What happened

straight brown hair dark brown eyes What does he look like!. He has short

While Korean kids are learning how to ski and snowboard in the snow, Australian kids are learning how to surf and water-ski at the beach3. Some children never play in the snow

I am writing this letter because I want to make a new friend in another country.. Maybe you will come to Gibraltar

Sam: It’s really nice, but don’t you think it’s too expensive.. John: Yeah, I’m not going to buy it, but I wish I could