• 沒有找到結果。

General conclusions and perspectives

Overviews

My results reveal cryptic sexual dichromatism in both carotenoid-based and melanin-based traits that cannot be detected by the human visual system. The subsequent molting experiments demonstrated that the bill coloration of a bird was highly correlated with its physical conditions:

redder bills had higher immunocompetence and incurred lower levels of oxidative stress. A significant correlation was observed between a bird’s melanin-based plumage and oxidative stress levels, indicating that the black plumage may be a cue about the physical condition of a bird.

However, the insignificant female preference for redder males rejected the hypothesis that the bill coloration served as a sexually selected trait.

Although I did not discover the functions of both carotenoid-based and melanin-based traits in the Himalayan black bulbul, according to the communication theory, I suggest that both trait types can play a role in focal species communication.

From the summary of my results (Table 6.1), I found that not all sexually dichromatic characteristics, such as the tarsi, remige and tail, can reflect an individual bird’s immunocompetence or oxidative stress level. This may be because I examined only the “quality” of an individual bird in a limited number of aspects. Several types of the quality have been reported to be correlated with the coloration, including parental caring, nesting

sites or territory quality and social ranking or good genes (Dale 2006). In the current study, the sexually dichromatic parts that did not reflect individual bird’s physical conditions may serve as indicators of other dimensions of the quality to the birds.

My results suggest the role of carotenoid-based bared parts (such as the bill, eye rings, leg or wattle) in avian species. In contrast to feathers, the color of bared parts may change rapidly and may therefore reflect the short-term physical conditions of the bearer (e.g., Faivre et al. 2003b, Pérez-Rodríguez and Viñuela 2008, Rosenthal et al. 2012). Compared with studies on the plumage of birds, studies exploring the links between birds’ physical conditions and coloration of carotenoid-based bared parts are relatively scant. To my knowledge, the roles of the carotenoid-based bill have been studied in only four avian species: European blackbirds (Turdus merula, Faivre et al. 2003a, Faivre et al. 2003b), red-legged partridges (Alectoris rufa, Pérez-Rodríguez and Viñuela 2008), zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata, Blount et al. 2003), and mallard ducks (Anas platyrhynchos, Peters et al. 2004). In these species, the bill coloration has been determined to be positively correlated with immunocompetence (Faivre et al. 2003b, Jawor and Breitwisch 2003, Mougeot et al. 2009, Peters et al. 2004). However, no study has examined the correlation between bill coloration and oxidative stress. My works provide another case that bill coloration was positively correlated with a bird‘s immunocompetence and negatively correlated with its oxidative stress level. Regarding the functions of the carotenoid-based bill, my results are not conclusive. The carotenoid-based bared parts are the sexually selected traits in the mentioned species. Nevertheless, a recent

comparative study revealed that carotenoid-based bill is highly common in species living in social groups during the nonbreeding season, and in species nesting in colonies; it is not related to either sexual dichromatism or sexual size dimorphism (Dey et al. 2015). The carotenoid-based bill was suggested to primarily evolve as a social signal. Although my results do not provide the function of the carotenoid-based bared parts, they indicated that the red bared parts alone may not be cues for female preference in Himalayan black bulbuls. Himalayan black bulbuls are socially monogamous, but they live in groups during non-breeding seasons; therefore, my results support the hypothesis proposed by Dey et al. (2015).

The concept of sexual selection being the major force producing conspicuous coloration in birds has been subjected to debate recently.

Dunn et al. (2015) suggested that natural selection is also attributable to evolution of diverse colorations. Because species with less sexually dichromatic traits may be under lower pressures of sexual selection (Andersson 1994), studying the functions of different pigmentations on less sexually dichromatic species can facilitate understanding the mechanism behind evolution of coloration in birds. To my knowledge, such studies have been conducted on only three less sexually dichromatic species: the great tits (Parus major), the blue tits (Parus caeruleus) (Delhey and Peters 2008), and the king penguins (Aptenodytes

patagonicus, Jouventin et al. 2005). Similar to Himalayan black bulbuls, all these species are sexually monomorphic in human visions but sexually dichromatic in avian visions (Delhey and Peters 2008). In all three

species, carotenoid-based ornaments serve as only quality signals

(reviewed in Delhey and Peters 2008, Viera et al. 2008), but not sexually selected traits. In great tits, the sexually selected trait is the area of melanin-based plumage on the breast (Delhey and Peters 2008, Norris 1990); whereas in blue tits, it is the UV plumage on the head (Delhey and Peters 2008, Hunt et al. 1999) as for the mutual sexually selected trait.

Moreover, in king penguins, it is the UV coloration in the colorful bill as for the mutual selected traits (Nolan et al. 2010). My results are similar to those of aforementioned studies mentioned: carotenoid-based ornaments are quality cues but might not solely serve as sexually selected traits.

However, so far I do not have further evidence revealing the mechanism associated with that causes the different functions of carotenoid-based ornaments in less sexual dichromatic avian species.

My results indicate that several carotenoid-based and melanin-based parts were sexually dichromatic or informative. This suggests that

multiple ornaments as sexually selected traits in Himalayan black bulbuls.

Studies have revealed that females might choose a mate according to multiple sexual ornaments (Chaine and Lyon 2008, Doucet and Montgomerie 2003). The benefit of multiple sexual ornaments is that these traits provide females with different types of information at various stages of the mate choice process (Borgia 1995) or function as redundant signals to improve the accuracy of mate assessment (Johnstone 1994, Moller and Pomiankowski 1993). Furthermore, females can use diverse characteristics in different breeding seasons as the current environmental conditions vary (Chaine and Lyon 2008, Qvarnström et al. 2000). Like in Northern cardinals (Cardinalis cardinalis), females use several

carotenoid-based parts and the size of melanin-based masks

simultaneously to choose their mates (Jawor and Breitwisch 2004, Jawor et al. 2003).

Future perspectives Social signal testing

In my dissertation, I did not test whether carotenoid-based or melanin-based traits function as indicators of the social status of an individual or contain information about individual qualities that can be useful in their social interactions, such as hierarchy or aggressiveness.

Studies have suggested that both types of traits are functional in social interaction in several avian species (Jawor and Breitwisch 2003, Roulin et al. 2008). Because Himalayan black bulbuls live in groups during

nonbreeding seasons and exhibit strong aggressiveness when in captivity with other individuals (personal observation), it is imperative for this species to develop social signals reflecting their physical conditions and fighting ability to reduce conflicts in social interactions. Therefore, I suggest that future studies could examine the correlation between color variations and social ranking (or fighting ability) of both trait types in black bulbuls.

Testing the possibility of other signal traits

In addition to carotenoid-based and melanin-based colorations, other traits can signal an individual’s quality or be used for mate choice, such as song (Shutler and Weatherhead 1990) and UV coloration (Prum 2006).

I observed UV light reflection from the bill and tarsi in Himalayan black

bulbuls. This can also serve as a target for signaling in black bulbuls.

Apart of UV light, bird songs have also been proved to be quality signals in other species (e.g., Gil and Gahr 2002). The songs of

Himalayan black bulbul are simple and can be performed by both sexes (personal observation) in an acoustic range (from20Hz to 20KHz, Pytte et al. 2004); however, such songs have not been analyzed systematically before. Nevertheless, Himalayan black bulbuls have been reported to sing ultrasonic sound (singing range >20KHz, Li et al. 2011). Several avian species were reported to sing in the ultrasonic sound range, which was determined to be indicator of individual quality in amphibians (e.g., Arch et al. 2008). Testing whether the songs both in acoustic and ultrasonic ranges could be vital functions in the focal species is imperative.

References

Andersson M (1994) Sexual Selection. Princeton University Press, Princeton.

Arch V S, Grafe T U, Narins P M (2008) Ultrasonic signalling by a Bornean frog. 4:19-22.

Blount J D, Metcalfe N B, Birkhead T R, Surai P F (2003) Carotenoid modulation of immune function and sexual attractiveness in zebra finches. Science 300:125-127.

Borgia G (1995) Complex male display and female choice in the spotted bowerbird: specialized functions for different bower decorations.

Anim Behav 49:1291-1301.

Chaine A S, Lyon B E (2008) Adaptive plasticity in female mate choice

dampens sexual selection on male ornaments in the lark bunting.

Science 319:459-462.

Dale J (2006) Intraspecific Variation in Coloration. In: Hill and McGraw (ed) Bird coloration: function and evolution 3rd edn. Harvard University Press, United Kingdom.

Delhey K, Peters A (2008) Quantifying variability of avian colours: are signalling traits more variable? PLOS ONE 3:e1689.

Dey C J, Valcu M, Kempenaers B, Dale J (2015) Carotenoid­based bill coloration functions as a social, not sexual, signal in songbirds (Aves:

Passeriformes). J Evol Biol 28:250-258.

Dunn P O, Armenta J K, Whittingham L A (2015) Natural and sexual selection act on different axes of variation in avian plumage color.

Sci Adv 1:e1400155.

Faivre B, Gregoire A, Preault M, Cezilly F, Sorci G (2003a) Immune activation rapidly mirrored in a secondary sexual trait. Science 300:103.

Faivre B, Préault M, Salvadori F, Théry M, Gaillard M, Cézilly F (2003b) Bill colour and immunocompetence in the European blackbird. Anim Behav 65:1125-1131.

Gil D, Gahr M (2002) The honesty of bird song: multiple constraints for multiple traits. Trends Ecol Evol 17:133-141.

Hunt S, Cuthill I C, Bennett A T, Griffiths R (1999) Preferences for ultraviolet partners in the blue tit. Anim Behav 58:809-815.

Jawor J M, Breitwisch R (2003) Melanin ornaments, honesty, and sexual selection. Auk 120:249-265.

Jawor J M, Breitwisch R (2004) Multiple ornaments in male northern cardinals, Cardinalis cardinalis, as indicators of condition. Ehtology 110:113-126.

Jawor J M, Linville S U, Beall S M, Breitwisch R (2003) Assortative mating by multiple ornaments in northern cardinals (Cardinalis cardinalis). Behav Ecol 14:515-520.

Johnstone R A (1994) Honest signalling, perceptual error and the evolution of'all-or-nothing'displays. P Roy Soc B 256:169-175.

Jouventin P, Nolan P M, Örnborg J, Dobson F S (2005) Ultraviolet beak spots in king and emperor penguins. Condor 107:144-150.

Li J, Zhang Y, Zhang Z (2011) High frequency components in avian vocalizations. Chinese birds 2:125-131.

Moller A, Pomiankowski A (1993) Why have birds got multiple sexual ornaments? Behav Ecol Sociobio 32:167-176.

Mougeot F, Pérez­Rodríguez L, Sumozas N, Terraube J (2009) Parasites, condition, immune responsiveness and carotenoid­based

ornamentation in male red­legged partridge Alectoris rufa. J Avian Biol 40:67-74.

Nolan P M, Stephen Dobson F, Nicolaus M, Karels T J, McGraw K J, Jouventin P (2010) Mutual mate choice for colorful traits in king penguins. Ethology 116:635-644.

Norris K (1990) Female choice and the quality of parental care in the great tit Parus major. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 27:275-281.

Pérez-Rodríguez L, Viñuela J (2008) Carotenoid-based bill and eye ring coloration as honest signals of condition: an experimental test in the

red-legged partridge (Alectoris rufa). Naturwissenschaften 95:821-830.

Peters A, Denk A, Delhey K, Kempenaers B (2004) Carotenoid­based bill colour as an indicator of immunocompetence and sperm performance in male mallards. J Evol Biol 17:1111-1120.

Pytte C L, Ficken M S, Moiseff A (2004) Ultrasonic singing by the blue-throated hummingbird: a comparison between production and perception. J Comp Physiol A 190:665-673.

Qvarnström A, Pärt T, Sheldon B C (2000) Adaptive plasticity in mate preference linked to differences in reproductive effort. 405:344-347.

Rosenthal M F, Murphy T G, Darling N, Tarvin K A (2012) Ornamental bill color rapidly signals changing condition. J Avian Biol

43:553-564.

Roulin A, Gasparini J, Bize P, Ritschard M, Richner H (2008)

Melanin-based colorations signal strategies to cope with poor and rich environments. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62:507-519.

Shutler D, Weatherhead P J (1990) Targets of sexual selection: song and plumage of wood warblers. 1967-1977.

Viera V M, Nolan P M, Côté S D, Jouventin P, Groscolas R (2008) Is territory defence related to plumage ornaments in the king penguin Aptenodytes patagonicus? Ethology 114:146-153.

Table 6.1 The summary of my results

Item Sexual

dichromatism

Quality cues

Sexual selected cues

(by females) Carotenoid-based

traits

Bill x

Tarsus x x

Melanin-based traits

Back x x Belly x x Breast x

Forehead x x

Nape x x

Remige x x

Scapula x

Tail x

“ ” indicates “yes” to the question.

“x” indicated “no” to the question

Table S2.1 Two-way ANOVA of different melanin-based parts in skin specimens

Parts Variables Total brightness Chromauv

F p F p df: year-2, sex-1, sex*year-2

Year: indicating the years after making into skin specimens, including within 5yrs, 10yrs and 15yrs.

Bold type indicates statistic significant (Bonferroin adjusted p = 0.005).

Table S2.2 Two-way ANOVA of different melanin-based parts between live birds and skin specimens

Parts Total brightness Chromauv

Variables F p F p Item: live bird and specimen, df= 1

Sex: female and male, df= 1 Item*sex: df=2.

Bold type indicates statistic significant (Bonferroin adjusted p = 0.005).

Table S 4.1 Multiple regressions of female preference and male morphometrics.

Term Estimate Std Error t p Intercept -2219.97 1352.23 -1.64 0.13

Bill 425.68 479.23 0.89 0.39

Head -50.79 301.64 -0.17 0.87

Tarsus 418.40 453.18 0.92 0.38

Wing 30.12 18.58 1.62 0.13

Weight 7.82 7.14 1.09 0.30

Female preference : (the amount of time a female spent on the perch in front of the male* 100) / (total choice time)

Table S 5.1 Multiple regressions of female preference and male morphometrics.

Term Estimate Std Error t p Intercept -2219.97 1352.23 -1.64 0.13

Bill 425.68 479.23 0.89 0.39

Head -50.79 301.64 -0.17 0.87

Tarsus 418.40 453.18 0.92 0.38

Wing 30.12 18.58 1.62 0.13

Weight 7.82 7.14 1.09 0.30

Female preference: (the amount of time a female spent on the perch in front of the male* 100) / (total choice time)