• 沒有找到結果。

Chapter 2. Experiment 1: The influence of verb bias on ORC processing

2.3 Discussion

The present study aimed to examine the verb bias effect on the incremental ORC

processing on P600, frontal negativity, and N400 components. The cluster-based

permutation analysis was performed to characterize the spatial and dynamic of the verb

bias effect on ORC processing by contrasting DO-SC, DO-EQ and SC-EQ bias

conditions. The result of DO-SC contrast was summarized in Table 2.6 The marked

ones were specifically pointed out for the following discussion. (For the complete

ERPs result on RC verb, DE, and head noun with the focus on the DO-SC contrast.

Table 2.6. Summary of the ERPs result on DO-SC contrast

Our result indicated that verb bias incrementally influences the ORC processing, as

first indexed by the late frontal positivity effect on RC verb in the DO-SC contrast. RC

verb in DO-bias condition elicited a greater late positivity from 600 to 1000ms in

frontal regions than that in SC-bias condition.

According to the literature, the less preferred but grammatical structure causes

processing difficulty, as indexed by longer reading time (Lin & Gansey, 2011, Wilson

&Garnsey, 2008) and P600 components (Osterhout & Holocomb, 1992). When reading

the DO-bias verb as the main verb, the parsers would less likely to expect another verb

which indicates the appearance of the clause. Given that the DO-bias verb can be

followed by both direct object and sentential complement, with higher proportion of

taking direct object, the appearance of “another verb” does not violate the sentence

structure. However, DO-biased verb followed by another verb is a “less preferred but ERP

component

Embedded RC noun

RC verb DE Head noun

N400 No difference No difference 318-455ms 322-477ms late

components may reflect this phenomenon, P600 in posterior regions or P600 in frontal

regions. The posterior-distributed P600 is known as an index of grammatical violation

and has been associated with the less preferred but grammatical structure (Osterhout &

Holocomb, 1992). Recent studies have reported that frontal positivity can also reflect

the processing difficulty on the non-preferred but grammatical continuations (Kann &

Swaab, 2003; Leone-Fernandez et al, 2012) and suggested that processing non-preferred

grammatical continuations and ungrammatical continuations involve different

mechanisms (Kann & Swaab, 2003). Kaan & Swaab (2003) manipulated four

conditions to compare the non-preferred and ungrammatical continuations. Their data

showed that, the ungrammatical continuations elicited a typical posterior-distributed

P600, whereas, the non-preferred grammatical continuations elicited a greater positivity

from 500 to 1100 ms in more anterior sites. Leone-Fernandez’s (2012) study also

observed a similar frontally distributed positivity was also elicited by the non-preferred

grammatical continuations. For instance, in Spanish, two different locate predicates (e.g.

estar en and ser en :“to be in” in English) require different subjects, object and events,

respectively. event + ser en was rated as a non-preferable but acceptable continuation.

Comparing with the object + ser en which is the preferable continuations, event + ser

en, the non-preferable but acceptable continuations, elicited positive-going waveform

starting around 400ms till 700ms, especially for central and frontal regions. In our

study, RC verb in DO-bias condition did not elicit a typical posterior-distributed P600,

but a frontally distributed positivity. Our finding of verb bias effect on RC verb

demonstrated that RC verb is not preferable for the DO-bias verb in ORC processing.

This verb bias effect was also shown on the subsequent RC marker DE and head

noun as indexed by the larger N400 responses on RC marker DE and head noun in

DO-bias condition (ex: 他 想起 里長 資助 的 街友。). These findings are consistent

with Lin & Garnsey’s (2011) study which has demonstrated the long-lasting difficulty

on the non-preferred but grammatical continuations, the parsers had difficulty on the

subsequent words – DE and head noun, as indexed by the larger N400 responses on RC

marker DE and head noun in DO-bias condition. When encountering the RC verb

following the DO-bias verb, the parsers became more alert to expect a clause which

may be more likely to be conceptualized as an event. However, when DE appeared, it

signaled that it would be followed by a noun. Thus, the appearance of DE contradicted

to the expectation of an “event”, leading to a greater difficulty of integrating DE to “the

concept of an event” formed earlier and elicited a greater N400. Nevertheless, for the

SC-bias condition, the clause that follows the SC-bias verb can be constructed by

various syntactic structures. Therefore, the parsers had less integration difficulty on the

RC marker DE.

After the appearance of DE, for both DO and SC-bias conditions, the parsers

would expect a noun. Yet, the larger N400 amplitude and a frontal negativity on the

head noun in DO-bias conditions suggested that the processing of the head noun in

these two conditions were different. In DO-bias condition, the following noun was the

actual direct object of the main verb, so the parsers had to integrate the head noun with

the preceding context. This integration difficulty was reflected on N400. During the

integration process, the parsers had to re-assign the object of the main verb. Take one of

the experimental stimuli as an example “他 想起 里長 資助 的 街友。” . The

parsers would first assign “里長” as the direct object of the main verb “想起”, but when

they read “DE”, they would expect the following noun “街友” was the actual direct

object of “想起”. This re-assignment was indexed by a late frontally-distributed

negativity. Previous studies have reported that frontal negativity appears in the process

involved in establishing reference (Barkley et al., 2015; Van Berkum et al, 2007;

Nieuwland et al., 2006). Referentially ambiguous nouns or pronouns has been found to

elicit a sustained frontal negativity, relative to the unambiguous ones. Although the

manipulation of the head noun in this study was not associated with the referential

ambiguity, the process of integrating head noun to the preceding context involved the

referential binding. That is, the parsers looked for the suitable referent (里長 or 街友)

for the main verb. Therefore, frontal negativity elicited by the head noun in DO-bias

condition may reflect another kind of “process of establishing referential binding”.

As for the EQ-bias verbs that do not have a clear tendency of taking more direct

objects or sentential complements, our findings showed that it tends to exhibit similar

pattern as the DO-bias verb did. The result probably indicated that when processing the

verbs without clear syntactic or semantic tendency, parsers tended to expect a simplest

grammatical structure — direct object following the main verb. When they found that

the main verb was not followed by a direct object, they experienced difficulty, as shown

a similar pattern of processing DO-bias verb condition.

Nevertheless, out of expectation, differences in embedded RC noun in late time

window were found in DO-SC condition. Since the syntactic structure of noun

following verb is grammatical in either DO-bias or SC-bias condition, no difference

shall be found between noun following DO-bias and SC-bias verb. Yet, the result

showed that embedded noun following DO-bias verb elicited a larger late positivity than

that following SC-bias verb. The possible explanation to that is the limited S+V+O

condition. There are three types of fillers. One is SVO, the other two types are sentential

complement following the main verb. Only nine sentences of SVO type out of total 68

sentences in the experiment. Therefore, the participants would have less expectations on

the sentence that ends with “object”. When encountering the DO-bias verb, the

participant would be more alert that it would not be followed only by the direct object,

but might be the sentential complement, as shown by the sustained negativity in

DO-bias condition. In the light of this, types of filler sentences were better controlled in the

second experiment, with the inclusion of more simple SVO structure of sentences.

To sum, our findings suggested verb bias effect on ORC processing and proposed

the possible explanations for its long-lasting effect. The findings implied that ORC

following the DO-bias verb is more difficult to process than that following the SC-bias

verb and exhibits the similar pattern as ORC following EQ-bias verb

Chapter 3. Experiment 2: The influence of verb bias on SRC processing

Experiment 2 intends to examine the verb bias effect on real-time SRC processing.

Based on the categorization of verb bias defined in the first experiment, three types of

verb bias (DO, SC, and EQ bias verbs) would be followed by an object-modifying

Subjective Relative Clause (SRC) in this experiment. ERP analysis on four regions of SRC sentences, namely the RC verb, embedded RC noun, RC marker DE, and the head

noun, would be conducted to investigate the incremental influence of verb bias on SRC comprehension. Similar to the SC advantage in ORC processing that demonstrated in

Experiment 1, it was also expected to show the SC advantage in SRC processing in

Experiment 2. However, aside from the verb bias effect, the influence of word order in

sentence processing should be considered in the current experiment. Different from

ORCs in which the syntactic structure follows the typical Mandarin word order

Subject-Object-Verb (SVO) (e.g. subject+ main verb+ RC noun: SVO), SRCs (e.g. subject+

main verb+ RC verb: SVV) violates the typical SVO word order. Since ORCs follows

the typical word order, the clear contrasts between conditions in the first experiment are

simply resulted from the verb bias effect. Yet, the violation of typical word order in

SRCs might lead to the influence of both the role of word order and verb bias in SRCs

3.1 Materials and Methods

3.1.1 Participants

Thirty-four right-handed undergraduate and graduate students between the age of

18 and 26 participated in this study. Participants were all native speakers of Mandarin

Chinese in Taiwan with no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders. Each

participant signed the consent for before the experiment and were paid $500 NT of their

participation. All participants were evaluated for their working memory and verbal

memory capacities with working memory test of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence

Scales-fourth edition (WAIS-IV; Weschler, 2008) and reading span test. Detailed

information about the behavioral tests was listed in Chapter 2. Table 1 presents the

result of the behavioral tests.

Table 3.1. The result of behavioral tests

Behavioral tests Mean score Range

WAIS-IV (working memory capacity) 37(3.8) 31-44

Reading span test 2.4(0.7) 1.5-4

Note. Standard deviations are in parentheses.

3.1.2 Experimental Design

This study aimed to manipulate three types of verb bias, Direct Object (DO),

Sentential Complement (SC), and Equilibrium Balanced (EQ) bias verb, which are

followed by a SRC, leading to the following syntactic structure of the target sentences:

Subject + DO/SC/EQ-bias main verb + SRC structure (RC verb + embedded RC noun +

RC marker DE + head noun). In order to make the sentences with SC-bias verb more

complete, in SC-bias condition, the SRC was followed by a verb, leading to the

structure of sentential complement. The current experiment shared the same verbs with

the first experiment. The classification of verb bias has already been defined in the first

experiment.

The construction of target sentences and fillers

The present experiment consisted of 41 target sentences with the manipulation of 14 DO-bias, 13 SC-bias, and 14 EQ-bias verbs as well as 42 fillers (Table2). Each

critical region of the SRC, were matched for word length, word frequency and the associations between critical words with no differences between conditions. The

embedded RC noun, RC verb, and head noun were all two-character words. The word frequency of 1st noun and head noun in each condition was low-medium word

frequency which has a frequency of 65 per million with no differences between groups

(embedded RC noun: F = 1.0, p > .05; head noun: F= 0.99, p > .05). To further control

the word predictability, the association between words were computed using word2vec

(Mikolov et al., 2013). The values of the association between words, , as shown in Table

3, including the association between 1st noun and RC verb (p > .05), and the association

between head noun and main verb ( F= 1.461, p >.05) , RC verb ( F= 1.11, p >.05), or

embedded RC noun( F= 0.328, p >.05 has to be lower than the value 0.3 (more than 0.3:

high associations between words, 0.2-0.1: medium-low associations, lower than 0.1:

low associations) with no differences between groups.

Additionally, 42 filler sentences were also created for preventing the participants

from developing strategies, including three syntactic structures—a. simple SVO

structure: subject + main verb + (adjectives) object b. subject + main verb + sentential

complement with embedded ORC/SRC, and c. subject + main verb + sentential

complement (Table 2).

Table 3.2. Examples of target and filler sentences

Target sentences

Main Clause Subjective Relative Clause (SRC) Sentential Complement

EQ-bias:

看見

招募 志工 的 導演

Types of filler sentences Examples subject + main verb + (adjectives) object 他蒐集證據。

subject + main verb + sentential

complement with embedded ORC/SRC

他懷疑老師讚美的學生作弊。

subject + main verb + sentential complement

他聽說產品出現瑕疵。

Table 3.3. The characteristics of the stimuli Frequency (per million words)

One-third of the experimental sentences were followed by one true/false comprehension

question to ensure that participants pay attention on reading comprehension during the

experiment.

3.1.3 Predictions

Based on the result of the Experiment 1 in which verb bias did influence the online

ORC processing, it was expected that verb bias would also affect the SRC processing.

Inasmuch as the syntactic structure of target sentences adopted in the current

experimental design violates the typical SVO word order in Mandarin, the role of word

order should also be taken into consideration for the evaluation of verb bias effect in the

present study. Two general hypotheses were made as follows. Firstly, if only verb bias

plays the role in SRC processing, it was expected that SRC following DO-bias verb

would be more difficult to process than that following SC-bias verb. Secondly, if both

“word order” and “verb bias” influence the SRC processing, processing difficulty might

arise in either condition since all the main verbs were followed by the other verb (RC

verb).

In addition, this processing difficulty may be shown on RC verb and head noun.

Predictions on the RC verb and head noun for each condition were listed as follows:

I. RC verb

Considering the verb bias effect demonstrated in the ORC processing, verb

bias effect in the SRC processing may be first reflected by RC verb in the contrast

between DO-bias and SC-bias conditions. RC verb in the sentence with DO-bias

verbs were expected more likely to be followed by a direct object and thus may

elicit a greater positivity of P600 to reflect the grammatical violation. Nevertheless, if “word order” also plays a role, RC verb in the sentence with SC-bias verbs may

also cause processing difficulty since it can be assigned by various grammatical

roles and which may result in a greater negativity of N400.

II. Head noun

While encountering a DO-bias verb, parsers would try to search for the direct

object for the main verb. Larger N400 on head noun under DO-bias condition than

that under SC-bias condition might be expected to reflect such a processing cost.

However, since SC-bias verb is more likely to followed by sentential

complements, the head noun might be mistaken as the subject of the sentential

complement or the object of the main verb. Such a processing difficulty may result

in thematic-role ambiguity and higher working memory demand and indexed by

the greater negativity of frontal negativity.

Aside from DO-bias and SC-bias verbs, EQ-bias verbs do not have a clear tendency of taking either more direct objects or sentential complements. The

sentences with EQ-bias verbs may serve as a baseline, as compared to the other

two conditions to see if they may exhibit a similar processing pattern to either

DO-bias or SC-DO-bias condition.

In order to delve into the role that each verb bias plays in the ORC processing,

differences between conditions were compared – SC-DO, SC- EQ, and DO-EQ bias

contrast.

3.1.4 Procedure

The procedure of the current study is as same as that of the first experiment

+

3.1.5 EEG recording and data analysis

The current study follows the same EEG recording and data analysis as the first

experiment.

3.2 Result

Three participants were excluded from further analysis due to the insufficient valid

trails; therefore, this study analyzed the total data of twenty-eight participants.

3.2.1 Accuracy of comprehension test

The overall accuracy of comprehension test was 95% (SD=0.04, range: 92%-100%),

showing that participants did not have difficulty understanding the sentences and had

pay attention in the experiment.

3.2.2 Statistical analysis for ERPs data:

cluster-based random permutation analysis

To evaluate the temporal and topographical differences between conditions (SC vs.

DO, SC vs. EQ, and DO vs. EQ), the cluster-based random permutation analysis was

conducted on each of the following critical regions – RC verb, embedded RC noun,

DE and head noun – for the mean amplitudes of two epochs, N400 from 250 to 500ms,

frontal negativity from 500 to 1000ms. The procedure of cluster-based random

permutation analysis was explained in details in Chapter2.

3.2.3 ERPs result (n = 28)

The cluster-based permutation analysis

Contrasts on RC verb

Figure 3.1 showed the grand-averaged ERMs waveforms elicited by RC verb in

three types of verb bias conditions. Visual inspection shows that RC verb in the

sentence with SC-bias verb elicited larger N400 than that in the sentence with DO-bias

verb, but it elicited a larger positive-going waveform than that in DO-bias condition in

the later time window. For the analysis of peak latency in DO-EQ and SC-EQ contrast,

a significant delay in N400 latency was shown in both DO-EQ (p < 0.01) and SC-EQ (p

< 0.01) contrast. It was found an 50ms delay in N400 peak latency on the EQ-bias

condition as compared to the DO-bias and SC-bias conditions. The cluster-based

random permutation analysis (Figure 3.2) revealed that SC-DO contrast yielded

significant negative cluster in the time window of 250-500ms (318-500ms, p<0.01).

DO-SC contrast yielded significant positive cluster in left fronto-central region

(773-956ms, p<0.01) in later time window. Both DO-bias and SC-bias condition yielded

larger positive clusters than the EQ-bias condition.

Figure 3.1. Grand averaged ERMs of the RC verb for DO-bias, SC-bias, and EQ- bias conditions.

Figure 3.2 Topographic maps of RC verb for SC-DO contrast in N400(250-500ms),

and DO-SC contrast in late time window (500-1000ms).

Note. Asterisks represent the significant differences for the contrasts.

Contrast on embedded RC noun

Figure 3.3 presented the grand-averaged ERMs waveforms elicited by embedded

RC noun in three types of verb bias conditions. The result showed that no differences

were found in DO-SC contrast; however, embedded RC noun in both the sentence with

SC-bias verb and DO-bias verb elicited larger negative-going waveform than that in

SC-bias verb and DO-bias verb elicited larger negative-going waveform than that in

相關文件