• 沒有找到結果。

Individual Support Regressions

5.2 Regression Results

5.2.2 Individual Support Regressions

立 政 治 大 學

N a

tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

32

From Table 9, the dependent variables of interest were addiction levels, daily consumption, and number of quit attempts in the past 12 months. Here, many of the results were significant, and exhibiting the same negative effect on support for the dependent variables. The largest beta was the 10% reduction in support for restaurant bans for those with high levels of addiction. This is consistent with the Ikeda finding [33]

that naïf hyperbolic discounters show higher rates of addiction and consumption because this group doesn’t foresee their future self control problem, and therefor doesn’t demand costly self-imposed external controls. The exception is the positive demand, albeit relatively small, for cigarette taxes as the number of failed quit attempts in the last 12 month increases. The literature review suggested a causal mechanism for this as self-awareness increases, thus suggesting people are transient between naïve and sophisticated in their forecasting.

5.2.2 Individual Support Regressions

The second and third sets of regressions are about this paper’s most important

contribution: testing for utilization of individual cessation supports like counseling, quit lines, and medication. Because of data attrition, the independent variables that we were most interested in, addiction levels, daily consumption, and number of failed quit

attempts in the past 12 months, were included in different regressions. Consequently, the reporting tables look different than the previous results.

Table 10. Individual Support Regression 1-3 (from left to right)

* p<0.10, ** p<0.05, *** p<0.010

* p<0.10, ** p<0.05, *** p<0.010

* p<0.10, ** p<0.05, *** p<0.010

Table 11. Individual Support Regression 4-6 (from left to right)

* p<0.10, ** p<0.05, *** p<0.010

* p<0.10, ** p<0.05, *** p<0.010

* p<0.10, ** p<0.05, *** p<0.010

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a

tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

35

From table 10, the only significant predictor of cessation support (quit lines or counseling) utilization was a high addiction level. Those who reported smoking a

cigarette within five minutes of waking up demanded higher levels of counseling or quit-lines. Because these types of support serve to make quitting less awful, the difference is noteworthy.

From table 11, the results are slightly ambiguous. High addiction levels and consumption increase demand for these supports, however a high number of failed quit attempts decreases demand for medication. It is definitely worth investigating the causal mechanism behind this observation, and opens the door to future research.

Comparing table 10 and 11, an increase in income reduces demand for time intensive supports like quit lines and counseling, but increases demand for the less time intensive medication. Logically, this is because of increased opportunity cost of lost time. Health care coverage had significance across the board and increases demand for all types of cessation supports: counseling, quit lines, and medication. This has important policy implications.

6 Discussion

The first set of hypotheses concerned the demand for costly external cessation supports as predicted by observed smoking behavior, which was argued to be a causal pathway for manifestations of consequences per theoretical model.

For the high addiction level predictor, there are somewhat inconsistent results based on the type of ban. Only the restaurant ban has explanatory power from this variable. The high addiction level decreases demand for smoking bans at restaurants. This is consistent with the Ikeda correlation. [27]

The model with cigarette taxes was the only regression that yielded any significant results for quit attempts. Hersch [24] had shown that previous quit attempts increased the

demand for smoking bans using panel data. We were not able to replicate his results for smoking bans, however the result for taxes is similar. We can speculate that the possible

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a

tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

36

reason could be that as smokers increase in quit attempts, their self-awareness changes them from naïf to sophisticated.

For consumption levels, our model yielded the most consistent results. For each of the external cessation support categories, higher levels of cigarette consumption decreased the demand. This is consistent with the Ikeda [27] analysis linking the naïve hyperbolic type to higher rates of consumption. However, it runs contrary to the Hersch findings.

Both this study and the Hersch study did not have information about time preferences and discounting, so we were not able to disaggregate the types. The Ikeda study, on the other hand, didn’t answer questions about support for external cessation supports. Because the models remain indeterminate in these questions, future research would be invaluable in trying to explain the links between types, behaviors, and preferences throughout a smoking lifecycle.

The second set of hypotheses were about the previously un-researched questions about smoking behaviors and support for individual cessation supports.

The first type of support was assistance like quit lines, counseling, or therapy. Of the predictor variables only level of addiction had a significant p-value. Here, there was a modest increase in demand for these types of cessation support. Although fuzzy, being a male did decreases the demand in one sample. In all the regressions, demand was also decreased with higher income, which could be explained by the higher opportunity cost of these therapies. Across the groups, however, having healthcare coverage that paid for these supports increased demand. Here, there is a salient policy implication: the

government could subsidize these types of treatments to increase utilization levels.

For nicotine replacement therapy or medicine support categories, higher levels of

addiction increased the demand. This is consistent under all of the models, given that the expected reward in utility is greater than price. An important policy implication is that, regardless of likely heterogeneity amongst smoking groups, this type of support remains attractive.

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a

tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

37

Higher daily consumption levels also, albeit modestly, increased the demand for nicotine replacement therapy or medicine. Logically, this makes sense because higher levels of consumption will likely translate to higher disutility in the withdrawal phase. A similar rationale is behind the addiction proxy. Therefore, regardless of type, reducing disutility from smoking cessation is demanded.

A high number of quit attempts decreased the demand for nicotine replacement therapy or medication. This is an important finding because it is a different type of predictor than addiction levels and consumption, in that it is actualized failure to carry out the desired task. The mental process that differs between addiction levels, daily consumption, and failed quit attempts could be further investigated.

Lastly, amongst these types of support, healthcare coverage increased demand as did a higher income. Here, income moves demand in the opposite direction compared to the time-intensive quit lines or counseling.

Comparing the regressions between the external supports like taxes and bans compared to individual supports like counseling or medication, this also introduces an inconsistency because it implies awareness and forecasting about disutility in the withdrawal period, but not about time inconsistencies. Further research would benefit by asking questions to obtain information about time preferences, discounting, and sophistication or naivety.

7 Conclusion

Lastly, models of smoking are largely used for prescriptive reasons: policy aimed at reducing smoking. Therefore this paper takes a special interest in explaining the

heterogeneity of preferences amongst the group that is currently smoking, but expresses a desire to quit. While it may be more difficult for policymakers to collect information about time preferences and discounting, a link between certain observed behaviors from our survey – quit attempts, addiction levels, and consumption – may be more readily observable.

Finally, also recognizing that different supports will be utilized differently, this paper was the first to explore how quit attempts, addiction levels, and consumption influence the

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a

tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

38

utilization of individual cessation supports. High addiction levels and high consumption levels increased demand for nicotine replacement therapy and medication.

Future research is still required to sort out the heterogeneity in observed behavior as it relates to time preferences and support for different types of cessation supports. Like this, public policy can have different approaches available given different observed

characteristics of smoking groups.

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a

tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

39

References

[1] World Health Organization. (2016, June) WHO Media Centre. [Online].

http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs339/en/

[2] U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, "The Health Consequences of Smoking: A Report of the Surgeon General," Washington, DC, 2004.

[3] David Collins, Helen Lapsley, and James Lightwood, "Estimating the costs of tobacco use,"

Tobacco Control in Developing Countries, 2000.

[4] Mitchell Hamline School of Law. (2016) Master Settlement Agreement. [Online].

http://publichealthlawcenter.org/topics/tobacco-control/tobacco-control-litigation/master-settlement-agreement

[5] National Institute on Drug Abuse. (2012, July ) Tobacco/Nicotine Research Report Series.

[Online]. https://www.drugabuse.gov/publications/research-reports/tobacco/nicotine-addictive

[6] American Society of Addiction Medication. (2011, April ) ASAM. [Online].

http://www.asam.org/quality-practice/definition-of-addiction [7] World Health Organization. (2016) ICD-10. [Online].

http://apps.who.int/classifications/icd10/browse/2016/en

[8] Homer, "Book XII," in The Odyssey.: Harvard Classics 1909, 850 B.C.

[9] Center for Science and Technology Studies. (2016) VOSviewer. [Online].

www.vosviewer.com

[10] Paul A Samuelson, "A Note on Measurement of Utility," The Review of Economic Studies 4, vol. 4, no. 2, pp. 155-161, 1937.

[11] Gary S. Becker and Kevin M. Murphy, "A Theory of Rational Addiction," Journal of Political Economy, vol. 96, no. 4, pp. 675-700, 1988.

[12] R. H. Strotz, "Myopia and Inconsistency in Dynamic Utility Maximization," The Review of Economic Studies, vol. 23, no. 3, pp. 165-180, 1955-1956.

[13] Shin-Ho & Richard J. Herrnstein Chung, "Relative and Absolute Strengths of Response as a Function of Frequency of Reinforcement," Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Animal Behavior , vol. 4, pp. 267-272, 1961.

[14] E.S., Pollak, R.A. Phelps, "On second best national saving and game—theoretic growth.,"

Review of Economic Studies, vol. 35, pp. 185–199., 1968.

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a

tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

40

[15] David Laibson, "Hyperbolic discount functions, undersaving, and savings policy," National Bureau of Economic Review, no. Working paper, 1996.

[16] Kamhon Kan, "Cigarette smoking and self-control," Journal of Health Economics, vol. 26, pp. 61-81, 2007.

[17] Botond Köszegi Jonathan Gruber, "IS ADDICTION "RATIONAL"? THEORY AND EVIDENCE,"

NATIONAL BUREAU OF ECONOMIC RESEARCH, vol. Working Paper 7507, 2000.

[18] Ted O'Donoghue and Matthew Rabin, "Doing It Now or Later," The American Economic Review, vol. 89, no. 1, pp. 103-124, 1999.

[19] B. DOUGLAS BERNHEIM AND ANTONIO RANGEL*, "Addiction and Cue-Triggered Decision Processes," THE AMERICAN ECONOMIC REVIEW, vol. 94, no. 5, pp. 1558-1587, 2004.

[20] Marc Potenza, "The neurobiology of pathological gambling and," Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, vol. 363, pp. 3181-3189, 2008.

[21] Roland Bénabou and Jean Tirole, "Willpower and Personal Rules," Journal of Political Economy, vol. 112, no. 4, pp. 848-886, 2004.

[22] ICEK AJZEN, "The Theory of Planned Behavior ," Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, vol. 50, pp. 179-211, 1991.

[23] Dan Ariely and Klaus Wertenbroch, "PROCRASTINATION, DEADLINES, AND PERFORMANCE," PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE, vol. 13, no. 3, 2002.

[24] JONI HERSCH, "Smoking Restrictions as a Self-Control Mechanism," The Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, vol. 31, no. 1, pp. 5-21, 2005.

[25] Michel Grignon∗, "An empirical investigation of heterogeneity in time preferences and,"

The Journal of Socio-Economics, vol. 38 , pp. 739–751, 2009.

[26] Takanori Ida, "A quasi-hyperbolic discounting approach to," Health Economics Review , vol.

4, no. 5, 2014.

[27] Myong-Il and Shinsuke Ikeda Kang, "Time discounting and smoking behavior: Evidence from a panel survey," Health Economics, vol. 23, pp. 1443-1464, 24.

[28] Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, "Adult SAMMEC: Smoking-attributable Mortality, Morbidity, and Economic Costs," Atlanta, GA, 2010.

[29] American Cancer Society, "Tobacco: The True Cost of Smoking," 2015.

[30] P.A.,Khan Z.M., Thompson, E.E. & Curry, S.J Fishman, "Health Care costs among Smokers, Former Smokers, and Never Smokers in an HMO," Health Services Research, vol. 38, no. 2, pp. 733-749, 2003.

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a

tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

41

[31] [Online]. http://www.cdc.gov/tobacco/data_statistics/surveys/nats/pdfs/methodology-report.pdf

[32] [Online]. http://www.cdc.gov/tobacco/data_statistics/surveys/nats/pdfs/weighting-specs.pdf

[33] and SHINSUKE IKEDA MYONG-IL KANG, "TIME DISCOUNTING AND SMOKING BEHAVIOR:

EVIDENCE FROM A PANEL SURVEY ," HEALTH ECONOMICS, vol. 23, pp. 1443–1464, 2014.

[34] Peter Huber, "Robust Estimation of a Location Parameter," The Annals of Mathematical Statistics, vol. 35, no. 1, pp. 73-101, 1964.

[35] David I. Laibson, "Hyperbolic Discount Functions, Undersaving, and Savings Policy," NBER Working Papers, 1996.

[36] H., & Turner, J. C. Tajfel, "An integrative theory of intergroup conflict," The social psychology of intergroup relations, pp. 33-48, 1979.

[37] Daniel Frings, and Ian P. Albery Sarah A. Buckingham, "Group Membership and Social Identity in Addiction Recovery," Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, vol. 27, no. No. 4, pp.

1132–1140, 2013.

相關文件