• 沒有找到結果。

Summary and conclusion

Earlier studies have reported that adults are more likely to interrupt children, assuming that the powerful one interrupts the less powerful one (Gleason 1987).

However, the findings of this study showed that the mothers did not interrupt more than their children. In fact, the children may be even more likely to interrupt their mothers. This may result from the children’s less compliance with the turn-taking principles and their inability to observe the transitional relevance places. Moreover, it was found that a greater proportion of the mothers’ interruptions were related to rapport rather than power. Most of the mothers’ interruptions resulted from active listening and high involvement, and resulted in encouragement and facilitation of the children’s speech. The findings were in contrast to the view that interruptions manifest dominance and power (James and Clarke 1993) but were consistent with the view that interruptions can be supportive and cooperative (James and Clarke 1993). We may speculate that the mothers’ interruption behaviors may also reflect the mothers’

socio-economic status (SES). As pointed out by Romaine (2001), language use varies with social classes. It has been suggested that different SES parents interact with their children in different ways in book reading and free plays (Snow et al. 1976 and Hoff-Ginsberg 1991). For example, parents from the upper-middle class produce more open-ended questions and fewer yes/no questions than parents from low and lower-middle classes; in addition, parents from higher SES tend to use fewer directives (Snow et al. 1976). Thus, it is possible that the two mothers’ more rapport-oriented style of interruptions was associated with the fact that both of the mothers were from the upper-middle class. However, since this can only be a speculation in this study, further research is needed to systematically investigate the relationship between parental interruption behaviors and social classes.

As for the children’s interruptions, it was found that the children at age three also produced relatively more rapport-oriented interruptions. Like their mothers, the children would also use interruptions to collaboratively construct propositions with

their mothers or to request more information. However, the children also made power-oriented interruptions, in which the children may reveal their intent to discontinue the current topic or they may simply initiate a new topic.

In terms of relevance and information status, our findings showed that the children’s interruptions were more likely to include relevant and new information, which indicated that the children at this stage were capable of tracking the current topic and making contributions to the conversation. Such interruptions were likely to succeed in getting the attention of and responses from the mothers. The ability to relate one’s own utterance to the preceding utterance of the interlocutor contingently and in a topic-related way is an essential component of conversational skills and is an important task for language-learning children (Huang 2004). It has also been reported that the development of children’s conversational skills can be characterized by an increasing ability to supply new information in order to maintain discourse topics (Bedrosian 1985). In this present study, it appeared that the children’s ability to provide interruptions with relevant and new information demonstrated the children’s communicative ability of topic management, and the skills of assessing the listener’s perspectives and the new/given information status.

Despite all the findings, this study leaves room for improvement and further inquiry. First of all, the number of subjects and the tokens of interruptions were limited. Therefore, to better our understanding of interruption in Mandarin adult-child conversation, the access to a larger amount of data is needed. Moreover, as previous studies indicate, gender may have an effect on how often interruptions are made and how the interruptions are treated. Although the child subjects of this study included a boy and a girl, the number of interruptions in our data was not large enough to analyze the children’s interruptions appropriately in terms of gender differences. Thus, further studies on the relationship between gender and interruption behaviors are needed in order to obtain a more complete picture of interruption in Mandarin adult-child conversation.

References

Beattie, Geoffery W. 1981. Interruptions in conversational interaction, and its relation to the sex and status of the interactants. Linguistics 19:15-35.

Bedrosian, Jan L. 1985. An approach to developing conversational competence.

School Discourse Problems, ed. by D. N. Ripich, and F. M. Spinelli, 231-255.

San Diego: College-Hill Press.

Bresnahan, Mary, and Deborah Cai. 1996. Gender and aggression in the recognition

of interruption. Discourse Processes 21:171-198.

Coates, Jennifer. 1989. Gossip revisited: Language in all-female group, Women in Their Speech Community, ed. by J. Coates, and D. Cameron, 94-122. New York:

Longman.

Coon, Christine A., and Paula J. Schwanenflugel. 1996. Evaluation of interruption behavior by naïve encoders. Discourse Processes 22:1-24.

Drummond, Kent. 1989. A backward glance at interruptions. Western Journal of Speech Communication 53:150-166.

Dunn, Judy, and Marilyn Shatz. 1989. Becoming a conversationalist despite (or because of) having an older sibling. Child Development 60:399-410.

Ervin-Tripp, Susan. 1979. Children’s verbal turn-taking. Developmental Pragmatics, ed. by E. Ochs, and B.B. Schieffelin, 391-414. New York: Academic Press.

Esposito, Anita. 1979. Sex differences in children’s conversation. Language and Speech 22:213-220.

Ferguson, Nicola. 1977. Simultaneous speech, interruptions and dominance. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 16:295-302.

Gleason, Jean Berko, and Esther B. Greif. 1983. Men’s speech to young children.

Language, Gender and Society, ed. by Barrie Thorne, Cheris Kramarae, and Nancy Henley, 140-150. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

Gleason, Jean Berko. 1987. Sex difference in parent-child interaction. Language, Gender and Sex in Comparative Perspective, ed. by S. Philip, S. Steele, and C.

Tanz, 189-199. New York: Cambridge University.

Goldberg, Julia A. 1990. Interrupting the discourse on interruptions. Journal of Pragmatics 14:883-903.

Hoff-Ginsberg, Erika. 1991. Mother-child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Development 62:782-796.

Huang, Chiung-chih. 2004. Conversational contingency and topic management in Mandarin child language. Taiwan Journal of Linguistics, Monograph Series No.

1.

Hung, Sik, Mark Brooke, and Michael Dunne. 1995. Interruption and influence in discussion groups. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 14:369-381.

Hsu, Joseph. 2000. A study of the acquisition of communicative competence: Social appropriateness in interactional speech. National Science Council project report.

James, Deborah, and Sandra Clarke. 1993. Women, men and interruptions: A critical review. Gender and Conversational Interaction, ed. by D. Tannen, 231-280.

New York and Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Kennedy, Carol W., and Carl Camden. 1983. A new look at interruptions. Western Journal of Speech Communication 47:45-58.

MacWhinney, B. 1995. The CHILDES Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

McTear, Michael. 1985. Children’s Conversation. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

Murray, Stephen, and Covelli Lucille. 1988. Women and men speaking at the same time. Journal of Pragmatics 12:103-111.

Ninio, Anat, and Catherine E. Snow. 1996. Children as conversationalist. Pragmatic Development. Boulder, Colorado: Westview.

Nohara, Michiko. 1992. Sex differences in interruption: An experimental reevaluation.

Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 21:127-146.

Octigan, Mary, and Sharon Niederman. 1975. Male dominance in conversation.

Frontiers 4:50-54.

Piaget, Jean. 1926. The Language and Thought of the Child. New York: Harcourt Brace.

Romaine, S. 2001. Language and social class. International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences, ed. by J. Smelser, and B. Baltes, 8303-8312.

New York: Elsevier.

Sacks, Harvey, Emanuel A. Schegloff, and Gail Jefferson. 1974. A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language 50:696-735.

Snow, C. E., A. Arlman-Rupp, Y. Hassing, J. Jobse, J. Joosten, and J. Vorster. 1976.

Mothers’ speech in three social classes. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 5:

1-20.

Tannen, Deborah. 1989. Talking Voice: Repetition, Dialogue, and Imagery in Conversational Discourse. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

West, Candace, and Don H. Zimmerman. 1975. Sex roles, interruptions and silences in conversation. Language and Sex: Difference and Dominance, ed. by Barrie Thorne, and Nancy Henley, 105-129. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

West, Candace, and Don H. Zimmerman. 1977. Women’s place in everyday talk:

Reflection on parent-child interaction. Social Problems 24:521-528.

West, Candace, and Don H. Zimmerman. 1983. Small insults: A study of interruptions in cross-sex conversations between unacquainted persons. Language, Gender and Society, ed. by Barrie Thorne, Cheris Kramarae, and Nancy Henley, 102-117.

Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

[Received 5 June 2006; revised 18 July 2006; accepted 22 July 2006]

Graduate Institute of Linguistics National Chengchi University Taipei, TAIWAN

Pei-chun Lu: echo0820@hotmail.com Chiung-chih Huang: cchuang@nccu.edu.tw

Appendix

CHAT symbols

(adopted from MacWhinney,1995)

. period

? question

! exclamation

0 action without speech ,, tag question

-: lengthening

+… trailing off +/. Interruption +, self-completion +^ quick uptake

# pause

%sit situation coding [?] best guess

[/] retracting without correction [//] retracting with correction [= ] explanation

[% ] comment on main line [>] [<] overlapping utterance xxx/xx unintelligible speech

漢語母子對話中打斷現象之研究

呂佩君、黃瓊之 國立政治大學

本研究的目的為瞭解母子對話中的打斷現象,分析的語料來自於 兩對以漢語為母語的母子對話,兩位小孩皆為三歲。首先,為瞭解母 親與孩子打斷句之本質,我們採用 Goldberg(1990)的分類來做判斷,

結果顯示兩者在對談中皆大多使用 rapport-oriented interruptions,代表 在母子對話中,兩方傾向在打斷句中表達對對方話語的興趣與投入,

並藉由著打斷來一同建立談話主題。再者,小孩的打斷句更進一步使 用 Dunn and Shatz(1989)的研究方法來分析其與對話言談的語意關 連,結果發現小孩的打斷句大多包含了與之前相關並且新的訊息,而 這類的打斷句是最有可能得到母親的注意與回應。

關鍵字:打斷現象、母子對話、語言習得

相關文件