• 沒有找到結果。

T (Initiate): No? OK! Why don’t we continue reading the story and find out?

After analyzing the above two excerpts, the researcher found that despite the teacher’s efforts in providing scaffolding moves, students’ decisions whether or not to participate decided the type of classroom interaction. As indicated by Bruner (1983), in storytelling the teller often structures the process to provide a scaffold to ensure that the listeners’ ineptitudes can be rescued or rectified by appropriate intervention. With the teacher’s scaffolds, student participation became easier, and the more students that participated, the more scaffolding moves the teacher could provide to promote their language learning. As a result, the teacher did not need to restructure or reformulate her questions into yes-no ones to elicit student responses as she did with the control group, and thus “the interaction flows in a natural way” (Van Lier, 1996, p. 195).

One thing needs to be mentioned is the limitation of the recording technique. One might notice that there were many overlapping turns in the storytelling group, since many students responded at the same time on many occasions. The actual number of turns might in fact be higher if all the overlapping turns could be observed and recorded.

Perspectives from the Teacher

In addition to the above test results and the comparisons of the excerpts from the classes, support for the hypotheses could also be found from the reactions of the teacher. In the open-ended teacher interview, the English teacher mentioned that the nature of storytelling affected the classroom interaction and considerably changed the classroom atmosphere and students’ motivation to participate. In order to understand whether the instructor was in any way biased toward one way of teaching, which might influence the results of the study, the interview questions asked her to compare her teaching in the different classrooms. In general, she mentioned that although she liked storytelling and had learned the techniques needed to carry it out, she was more confident in teaching story reading in the control group, and enjoyed this teaching very much. When asked to compare the two groups, she was able to clearly describe the differences.

Specifically, the teacher mentioned that the students in the storytelling group often repeated or chanted with the teacher during the storytelling process or mouthed words or sentences during their recall writings. The latter of these occurrences, an example of “private speech” from Vygotsky’s perspective, indicates that the learners were

engaged in the process of learning (Hall, 2001). On the other hand, the students in the control group were not as ready to vocalize during class, and there was little evidence of engagement in private speech during their recall writings. The teacher believed that students in the storytelling group were more active, because the nature of storytelling involved interactions between the teller and the listeners.

Likewise, the teacher indicated that in writing recalls the students in the storytelling group usually wrote up to the last minute, but many students in the control group finished long before the allotted time was over. Without storytelling instruction, she further commented, the students in the control group could not achieve the same degree of interaction in spite of many attempts on the part of the teacher to encourage them. Without the listener-hearer involvement in the storytelling process, it is not easy to trigger voluntary student interaction. The teacher also added that students in Taiwan are very used to lectures and worksheets. Thus, the teacher believed that such difference might have also contributed to the longer recalls of the storytelling group.

CONCLUSIONS

This study examined whether storytelling instruction can improve both the quantity and the quality of interaction in a teacher-centered English language classroom and facilitate learning. The findings indicate that storytelling instruction significantly increased the number, if not the length, of student turns. A closer examination of the type of interaction that occurred in the storytelling classroom shows that the teacher tended to use more open-ended questioning

techniques, maintained direction more often, and used less feedback that simply consisted of making a qualitative judgment on the content of student responses. Overall, the discourse in the storytelling class created more opportunities for scaffolding, and thus supported student interaction in the Zone of Proximal Development. With regard to language development, there were statistically demonstrable differences favoring the storytelling group in terms of comprehending reading material and employing story structures in writing.

The teacher thought that the two-way communication of storytelling naturally nurtured meaningful interactions between the teller and the listeners, and also motivated learners to participate orally in class. In this way, the art of storytelling smoothly transformed a written text into oral presentation and provided excellent examples to help language learners link these two forms of language. In addition, linking the oral and written forms of language probably helped students whose preparation had been largely based on the written word to participate more fully in oral interaction, and to make connections between the written and spoken language.

The positive influence of storytelling on this particular large, teacher-centered English language class in terms of increasing the amount of interaction, improving its quality, and encouraging increased language development, suggests that storytelling could be a fruitful technique to use in similar contexts. Perhaps the notion that not having enough access to oral language to be able to learn to communicate in that mode can be dispelled, and learners can greatly benefit by improving in English and other foreign languages.

In conclusion, the researcher would like to suggest that storytelling could be a persuasive vehicle for helping teachers

accustomed to a teacher-centered classroom to elicit more frequent and effective student oral participation. Research indicates that educational innovations are most likely to be accepted when they align with current teacher beliefs or when those beliefs can be slightly modified to accept them (Ambrose 2004; Errington 2004), although such modifications are slow, incremental, and tied to broader personal, social and historical contexts (Stephens, Gaffney, Weinzierl, Shelton,

& Clark, 1993). As discussed above, the changes made in the implementation of the storytelling approach were not a dramatic departure from standard practice, but the resulting differences in classroom interaction were both qualitatively and quantitatively significant, and students showed marked improvements in other skills compared to the control group. Hence, the researcher concludes that storytelling is an approach that should be explored and promoted in the Taiwanese and similar contexts.

ACKNOWLEDGMENT

This work was supported by grants from National Science Council (NSC 92-2411-H-024-002) in Taiwan. We would like to thank the anonymous reviewers for their incisive comments.

REFERENCES

Alexander, R. (2000). Culture and pedagogy: International comparisons in primary education. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

Ambrose, R. (2004). Initiating change in prospective elementary school teachers’ orientations to mathematics teaching by

building on beliefs. Journal of Mathematics Teacher Education, 7, 91-119.

Bearne, E. (1999). Use of language across the secondary curriculum.

London: Routledge.

Bruner, J. (1983). Child’s talk: Learning to use language. New York:

Norton.

Chaudron, C. (1988). Second language classrooms: Research on teaching and learning. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Chen, M. F. (2008). The effects of storytelling on elementary school students’ English learning. Unpublished master’s thesis, National Kaohsiung First University of Science and Technology, Kaohsiung, Taiwan.

Chou, T. Y. (2006). Effects of English storytelling on English learning of elementary school students. Unpublished master’s thesis, National Kaohsiung Normal University, Kaohsiung, Taiwan.

Clark, M. C., & Rossiter, M. (2008). Narrative learning in adulthood.

In S. B. Merriam (Ed.), Third update on adult learning theory:

New directions in adult and continuing education (No. 119, pp.

61-70). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Cooper, P. (1989). Using storytelling to teach oral communication competencies K-12. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No.

ED314798)

Day, R. (1984). Student participation in the ESL classroom or some imperfections in practice. Language Learning, 34, 69-102.

Elley, W. (1989). Vocabulary acquisition from listening to stories.

Reading Research Quarterly, 24, 174-187.

Ellis, J. (1993). Japanese students abroad: Relating language ability in class and in the community. Thought Currents in English Literature, 66, 45-82.

Errington, E. (2004). The impact of teacher beliefs on flexible learning innovation: Some practices and possibilities for academic developers. Innovations in Education and Teaching International, 41, 39-47.

Garvie, E. (1990). Story as vehicle: Teaching English to young children. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.

Gass, S. (2004). Conversation analysis and input-interaction. The Modern Language Journal, 88, 597-602.

Hall, J. K. (2001). Method for teaching foreign language: Creating a community of learners in the classroom. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill Prentice-Hall.

Horner, B. (1983). To tell or not to tell: Storytelling for young adults.

Illinois Libraries, 65, 458-464.

Hsieh, L. T. (2006). The effects of storytelling instruction on Chinese EFL children’s English learning. Studies in English Learning and Literature, 17, 33-46.

Hsueh, H. Y. (2007). Relationship between picture book reading instruction and English learning motivation for elementary school EFL learners. Unpublished master’s thesis, National Kaohsiung Normal University, Kaohsiung, Taiwan.

Huang, H. (2006). The effect of storytelling on EFL young learners’

reading comprehension and word recall. English Teaching &

Learning, 30(3), 51-74.

Isbell, R., Sobol, J., Lindauer, L., & Lowrance, A. (2004). The effects of storytelling and story reading on the oral language

complexity and story comprehension of young children. Early Childhood Education Journal, 32, 157-163.

Kalmback, J. R. (1986). Getting at the point of retellings. Journal of Reading, 29, 326-333.

Lightbown, P. M. (2000). Classroom SLA research and second language teaching. Applied Linguistics, 21, 431-462.

Mercer, N. (1995). The guided construction of knowledge: Talk amongst teachers and learners. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.

Morrow, L. M. (1979). Exciting children about literature through creative storytelling techniques. Language Arts, 56, 236-243.

Myers, P. (1990). Stories from print. Language Arts, 67, 824-831.

National Storytelling Network. (2010). What is storytelling?

Retrieved October 20, 2010, from http://www.storynet.org/

resources/whatisstorytelling.html

Pavlenko, A., & Lantolf, J. (2000). Second language learning as participation and the (re)construction of identity. In J. Lantolf (Ed.), Sociocultural theory and second language learning (pp.

155-177). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Rossiter, M. (2002). Narrative and stories in adult teaching and learning. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No.

ED473147)

Stephens, D., Gaffney, J., Weinzierl, J., Shelton, J., & Clark, C.

(1993). Toward understanding teacher change. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED361667)

Stivers, T., Enfield, N. J., Brown, P., Englert, C., Hayashi, M., Heinemann, T., et al. (2009). Universals and cultural variation in turn-taking in conversation. PNAS, 106, 10587-10592.

Strong, M. (1983). Social styles and the second language acquisition of Spanish-speaking kindergartners. TESOL Quarterly, 17, 241-258.

Trostle, S., & Hicks, S. J. (1998). The effects of storytelling versus story reading on comprehension and vocabulary knowledge of British primary school children. Reading Improvements, 35(3), 127-136.

Tsou, W. (2005). Improving the speaking skill through instruction in oral classroom participation. Foreign Language Annals, 38(1), 46-55.

Tsou, W., Wang, W., & Tzeng, Y. (2006). Applying a multimedia storytelling website in foreign language learning. Computer &

Education, 47, 17-28.

Van Lier, L. (1996). Interaction in the language curriculum:

Awareness, autonomy & authenticity. New York: Longman.

Van Patten, B. (2003). From input to output: A teacher’s guide to second language acquisition. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Verdugo, D. R., & Belmonte, I. A. (2007). Using digital stories to improve listening comprehension with Spanish young learners of English. Language Learning & Technology, 11(1), 87-101.

Wang, Y. (2008). Effects of storytelling on listening and speaking vocabulary acquisition of elementary school EFL students.

Unpublished master’s thesis, National Kaohsiung Normal University, Kaohsiung, Taiwan.

Whaley, J. (1981). Readers expectations for story structure. Reading Research Quarterly, 16, 90-114.

Wolfram, W., & Schilling-Estes, N. (2006). American English:

Dialects and variation. Malden, MA: Blackwell.

Wood, D., Bruner, J. S., & Ross, G., (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17(2), 89-100.

Wright, A. (1997). Creating stories with children. Oxford, UK:

Oxford University Press.

Yau, S. (2003). Application of storytelling in an English teaching program in elementary school. Unpublished master’s thesis, National Kaohsiung Normal University, Kaohsiung, Taiwan.

Zobairi, N., & Gulley, B. (1989). The told tale: Oral storytelling and the young child. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No.

ED319517)

相關文件