• 沒有找到結果。

Chapter 5 Discussion and conclusion

5.3 Conclusion

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

children’s syllable productions are relevant to their caretakers. Children’s phonological productions may be influenced by the overall productivity and the phonetic content of the ambient language. Furthermore, it can be reasoned that a young child produces a target phonological structure which is strongly related to the input frequency (Ota, 2006). To conclude, the findings lend some credence to the accounts that children’s syllable patterns have largely been influenced by their linguistic environment (i.e. caretakers’ speech).

5.3 Conclusion

The aim of the present study is to explore young children’s syllable acquisition and deletion form composed the words in terms of frequencies and patterns.

Children’s overall syllable patterns, syllable deletion patterns and the child-caretakers’

syllable relationship were examined. From the analysis and discussion of the four children acquiring Taiwan Mandarin, the unmarked syllable type (i.e. CV form) has the highest frequency in the analyses of overall syllable types and syllable deletion types respectively. The more marked and complex syllable structures occur less frequently in the young children’s early production.

At early stage in phonological development, since the children’s word productions are immature and unstable, they tend to use many different phonetic forms to reduce a word in speech development. The segments in the syllable-initial,

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

syllable-middle and syllable-final positions can be omitted. We can observe that children tend to delete the postnuclear glide [w] and the final nasal [n]/ [ŋ] in the productions, indicating that the segments in the final positions seem to be more difficult for children to produce. As young children’s phonological systems are immature, the patterns of syllable deletion would occur. Presumably, if their phonological systems become stable and mature, the occurrences of syllable deletion patterns would decrease accordingly.

Concerning the analysis and discussion, the findings can be accounted for markedness theory proposed by Jakobson (1968) and frequency effects (e.g., Ingram, 1988; Zamuner et al., 2005). On the one hand, the children’s most frequently used syllable is CV, and is then followed by CVG. Since these two types are syllables which contain onset consonant and do not contain consonant clusters, they are considered to be simple and unmarked syllable types for the children. The more unmarked syllable types occur more frequently in young children’s speech.

Furthermore, syllable types contain more complex structure, that is, a more marked syllable, may occur less frequently in the children’s words. On the other hand, comparison of frequency effects presented that the child-caretaker has the positive correlation. Children general have the similar syllable patterns with their caretakers, suggesting that the children are sensitive to ambient language at an early age. It may

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

be beneficial for future word to trace patterns of syllable deletion for more participants, for longer duration, and examine the age range of each participant’s syllable production.

Finally, we hope that this study may shed light on several issues to the process and pattern of syllable acquisition and syllable deletion of Taiwan Mandarin in the longitudinal observation.

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

References

Albin, D. D., & Echols, C. H. (1996). Stressed and word-final syllables in infant-directed speech. Infant Behavior and Development, 19(4), 401-418.

Allen, G. D., & Hawkins, S. (1980). Phonological rhythm: Definition and development. Child phonology, 1, 227-256.

Bao, Z. (1996). The syllable in Chinese. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 24(2), 312-353.

Bao, Z. (1990). Fanqie languages and reduplication. Linguistic Inquiry, 317-350.

Bernhardt, B. H., & Stemberger, J. P. (1998). Handbook of phonological development from the perspective of constraint-based nonlinear phonology. Academic press.

Burchfield, L. A., & Bradlow, A. R. (2014). Syllabic reduction in Mandarin and English speech. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 135(6), EL270-EL276.

Carter, A. K., & Gerken, L. (2003). Similarities in weak syllable omissions between children with specific language impairment and normally developing language:

a preliminary report. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36(2), 165-179.

Carter, A., & Gerken, L. (2004). Do children's omissions leave traces?. Journal of child language, 31(03), 561-586.

Cheng, C. C. (1973). A synchronic phonology of Mandarin Chinese (Vol. 4). Walter

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

de Gruyter.

Cheng, C., & Xu, Y. (2009). Extreme reductions: contraction of disyllables into monosyllables in taiwan Mandarin. In INTERSPEECH (pp. 456-459).

Chen, L. M., & Kent, R. D. (2005). Consonant–vowel co-occurrence patterns in Mandarin-learning infants. Journal of Child Language, 32(03), 507-534.

Chen, L. M., & Kent, R. D. (2010). Segmental production in Mandarin-learning infants. Journal of child language, 37(02), 341-371.

Chiang, W.Y. (1992). The prosodic morphology and phonology of affixation in Taiwanese and other Chinese languages. Ph.D. dissertation. University of

Delaware.

Chien, Y. F. (2011). Postnuclear Glides and Coda Nasals in Taiwan Mandarin:

Evidence from Experimental Elicitation of Speech Errors. WECOL 2010, 20.

Davis, B. L., & MacNeilage, P. F. (1990). Acquisition of Correct Vowel ProductionA Quantitative Case Study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 33(1), 16-27.

De Boysson-Bardies, B. (1993). Ontogeny of language-specific syllabic

productions (pp. 353-363). Springer Netherlands.

De Boysson-Bardies, B., & Vihman, M. M. (1991). Adaptation to language: Evidence

from babbling and first words in four languages. Language, 297-319.

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

Demuth, K. (1995). Markedness and the development of prosodic structure. In J.

Beckman (Ed.), Proceedings of the North East Linguistic Society, 25. Amherst, MA: University of Massachusetts, Graduate Linguistics Student Association.

Demuth, K. (2001). Prosodic constraints on morphological development.Language Acquisition and Language Disorders, 24, 3-22.

Demuth, K., & Johnson, M. (2003). Truncation to subminimal words in early French.

Canadian Journal of Linguistics, 48, 211-241.

Dodd, B., Holm, A., Hua, Z., & Crosbie, S. (2003). Phonological development: a normative study of British English‐speaking children. Clinical Linguistics &

Phonetics, 17(8), 617-643.

Duanmu, S. (1990). A formal study of syllable, tone, stress and domain in Chinese languages. Ph.D. dissertation. Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

Duanmu, S. (2000). The phonology of standard Mandarin. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

Duanmu, S. (2007). The phonology of standard Chinese. Oxford, England; Oxford University Press.

Duanmu, S., Kim, H. Y., & Stiennon, N. (2005). Stress and syllable structure in

English: Approaches to phonological variations. Taiwan Journal of

Linguistics,3(2), 45-77.

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

Echols, C. H. (1993). A perceptually-based model of children's earliest productions. Cognition, 46(3), 245-296.

Echols, C. H., & Newport, E. L. (1992). The role of stress and position in determining first words. Language acquisition, 2(3), 189-220.

Fee, J., & Ingram, D. (1982). Reduplication as a strategy of phonological development. Journal of Child Language, 9(01), 41-54.

Fenson, L., Marchman, V., Thal, D., Dale, P., Reznick, S. & Bates, E. (2007).

MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories : user’s guide and

technical manual, 2nd edn. Baltimore : Paul H. Brookes.

Ferguson, C. A., & Farwell, C. B. (1975). Words and sounds in early language acquisition. Language, 419-439.

Fromkin, V. A. (1971). The non-anomalous nature of anomalous utterances. Language, 27-52.

Gerken, L. (1994). A metrical template account of children's weak syllable omissions from multisyllabic words. Journal of child language, 21(03), 565-584.

Hilton, N. H., Schüppert, A., & Gooskens, C. (2011). Syllable reduction and articulation rates in Danish, Norwegian and Swedish. Nordic Journal of Linguistics, 34(02), 215-237.

Ingram, D. (1974). Phonological rules in young children. Journal of child

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

language, 1(01), 49-64.

Ingram, D. (1978). The role of the syllable in phonological development. In A. Bell and J. Hooper (eds.), Syllables and Segments. New York, NY: Elsevier-North Holland press.

Ingram, D. (1989). First language acquisition: Method, description and explanation.

Cambridge University Press.

Jakobson, R. (1968). Child language: aphasia and phonological universals (No. 72).

Walter de Gruyter.

James, D., van Doorn, J., & McLeod, S. (2007). Does the number of syllables in words affect weak syllable deletion in typically developing children aged 3 to 7 years speaking Australian English. In World congress of the International Association of Logopedia and Phoniatrics, Copenhagen, Denmark.

Kehoe, M. M., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (2001). Development of syllable structure in English-speaking children with particular reference to rhymes. Journal of child language, 28(02), 393-432.

Levelt, C. C., Schiller, N. O., & Levelt, W. J. (2000). The acquisition of syllable types. Language acquisition, 8(3), 237-264.

Levitt, A. G., & Aydelott Utman, J. G. (1992). From babbling towards the sound systems of English and French: A longitudinal two-case study. Journal of child

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

language, 19(01), 19-49.

Lewedag, V. L., Oller, D. K. & Lynch, M. P. (1994). Infants’ vocalization patterns across home and laboratory environments. First Language 14, 1(40), 49–65.

Lin, Y. H. (1989). Autosegmental treatment of segmental processes in Chinese phonology. Ph.D. dissertation. University of Texas at Austin.

Lin, Y. H. (2007). The Sounds of Chinese. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Lleó, C. (2000). The interface of phonology and morphology: the emergence of the article in the early acquisition of Spanish and German. Approaches to Bootstrapping: Phonological, Syntactic and Neurophysiological Aspects of

Early language Acquisition, 2, 23-44.

Lleó, C., & Prinz, M. (1996). Consonant clusters in child phonology and the directionality of syllable structure assignment. Journal of child language, 23(01), 31-56.

Macken, M. A. (1978). Permitted complexity in phonological development: One child's acquisition of Spanish consonants. Lingua, 44(2), 219-253.

Macken, M. A. (1992). Where’s phonology. Phonological development: Models, research, implications, 249-269.

MacNeilage, P. F., & Davis, B. L. (1993). Motor explanations of babbling and early

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

speech patterns. In Developmental neurocognition: Speech and face processing in the first year of life (pp. 341-352). Springer Netherlands.

Macrae, T. (2013). Lexical and child-related factors in word variability and accuracy in infants. Clinical linguistics & phonetics, 27(6-7), 497-507.

Matthei, E. H. (1989). Crossing boundaries: More evidence for phonological constraints on early multi-word utterances. Journal of child language, 16(01), 41-54.

McCarthy, J. & A. Prince. (1993). The Emergence of the unmarked optimality in prosodic morphology. Paper presented at NELS 26, UMass, Amherst.

Menn, L. (1983). Development of articulatory, phonetic, and phonological capabilities. Language production, 2, 3-50.

Menn, L., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (2009). Phonological development. In J. B. Gleason.

and N. B. Ratner (eds.), The Development of Language. Boston, Ma: Pearson Press.58-103.

Ota, M. (2003). The Development of Prosodic Structure in Early Words. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins.

Ota, M. (2006). Input frequency and word truncation in child Japanese: Structural and lexical effects. Language and Speech, 49(2), 261-294.

Pater, J. (1997). Minimal violation and phonological development. Language

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

Acquisition, 6(3), 201-253.

Prieto, P. (2006). The relevance of metrical information in early prosodic word acquisition: A comparison of Catalan and Spanish. Language and speech,49(2), 231-258.

Prieto, P., Bosch-Baliarda, M., & Saceda-Ulloa, M. (2005). The development of codas in Catalan and Spanish: frequency effects in specific contexts. In Xth international congress for the study of child language. 25-29.

Roark, B., & Demuth, K. (2000). Prosodic constraints and the learner’s environment:

A corpus study. In Proceedings of the 24th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (Vol. 2, pp. 597-608). Somerville, MA:

Cascadilla Press.

Rose, Y. (2000). Headedness and prosodic licensing in the L1 acquisition of phonology. Ph.D. dissertation. McGill University, Montréal

Rose, Y., & Inkelas, S. (2011). The interpretation of phonological patterns in first language acquisition. The Blackwell companion to phonology, 2414, 2438.

Saceda-Ulloa, M. (2005). Adquisición prosódica en español peninsular: La sílaba y la palabra prosódica. M.A. Thesis. Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona.

So, L. K., & Dodd, B. J. (1995). The acquisition of phonology by Cantonese-speaking children. Journal of child language, 22(03), 473-495.

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

Sosa, A. V., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (2012). Lexical and phonological effects in early word production. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,55(2), 596-608.

Spencer, A. (1996). Phonology. Oxford, England: Blackwell.

Stark, R. E. (1980). Stages of speech development in the first year of life. Child phonology, 1, 73-90.

Stemberger, J. P. (1983). Speech errors and theoretical phonology: A review. Indiana University Linguistics Club.

Stites, J., Demuth, K., & Kirk, C. (2004). Markedness vs. frequency effects in coda acquisition. In Proceedings of the 28th annual Boston University conference on language development (pp. 565-576).

Stoel-Gammon, C. (1998). Sounds and words in early language acquisition: The relationship between lexical and phonological development. Exploring the speech-language connection, 8, 25-52.

Stoel-Gammon, C. (2011). Relationships between lexical and phonological development in young children. Journal of child language, 38(01), 1-34.

Tsay, J. (2007). Markedness and Frequency in Phonological Development: A Corpus-based Study. International Workshop on Grammar and Evidence.

Symposium conducted at the meeting of National Chung Cheng University

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

College of Humanities, Room 155.

Tse, A.C.Y. (1991). The acquisition process of Cantonese phonology: A case study.

Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Hong Kong.

Vihman, M. M., & McCune, L. (1994). When is a word a word?. Journal of child language, 21(03), 517-542.

Wan, I. P. (1999). Mandarin Phonology: evidence from speech errors. Ph.D.

dissertation. SUNY-Buffalo.

Wan, I. P. (2002). The status of prenuclear glides in Mandarin syllables: Evidence from psycholinguistics and experimental acoustics. Chinese Phonology 11, special issue. 141-162.

Wan, I. P., & Jaeger, J. J. (2003). The phonological representation of Taiwan Mandarin vowels: A psycholinguistic study. Journal of East Asian Linguistics,12(3), 205-257.

Wan, I. P. (2006). A psycholinguistic study of postnuclear glides and coda nasals in Mandarin. Journal of Language and Linguistics, 5(2), 158-176.

Wang, H. S., & Chang, C. L. (2001). On the status of the prenucleus glide in Mandarin Chinese. Language and Linguistics, 2(2), 243-260.

Wang, H. Y. (2014). Phonological variability in word production in Taiwan Mandarin.

Unpublished master's thesis, National Chengchi University, Taipei.

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

Wang, Z. (1996). Features as phonological primitives: evidence from Beijing Mandarin. In 70th Annual Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America, San Diego.

Wijnen, F., Krikhaar, E., & Den Os, E. (1994). The (non) realization of unstressed elements in children's utterances: Evidence for a rhythmic constraint. Journal of child language, 21(01), 59-83.

Yip, M. (2002). Tone. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Zamuner, T. S., Gerken, L., & Hammond, M. (2005). The acquisition of phonology based on input: A closer look at the relation of cross-linguistic and child language data. Lingua, 115(10), 1403-1426.

Zhu, H. & B. J. Dodd (2000). The phonological acquisition of Putonghua (Modern Standard Chinese). Journal of Child Language, 27, 3-42.

蔡素娟(民 95)。語料庫為本的閩南語兒童韻律習得研究。行政院國家科學委員

會專題研究成果報告(編號:NSC 95-2411-H-194-022-MY3),未出版。