• 沒有找到結果。

Syllable relationship between children and caretakers

Chapter 4 Result and analysis

4.4 Syllable relationship between children and caretakers

fourth tended to produce the deletion of initial consonant and the final nasals [n] and [ŋ]. Although the presented results indicated that the four children’s syllable deletion

patterns were exhibited differently, they still tended to drop the segments in their early stages of speech development.

4.4 Syllable relationship between children and caretakers

As previously stated, the results show the unmarked syllable type CV has the highest frequency in the production of the children’s overall production and syllable deletion analysis. This following analysis demonstrates the syllable relationship between the child and his/her caretaker and whether the child’s syllable productions would be affected by the caretaker’s input. The percentages of the overall syllable production of each caretaker-child are shown in the following figures.

Figure 4.9 Percentages of syllable types of child #1 and caretaker #1

According to statistical analysis, there is a positive correlation between the

V CV VG GV VN CVG CVN CGV GVG GVN CGV

syllables of child #1 and the syllables of caretaker #1, r =.96, p<.05. The percentages are statistically significant. Based on Figure 4.9, CV is the most frequently used syllable type produced by child #1 and caretaker #1; the frequencies of CV were reported as 35.7 % and 31.6 % respectively. CVG ranks second place in their productions, with a usage of 13.7% and 18.6% of utterances. The ranking of frequencies of syllable types is thus CV > CVG > CVN. However, for the least frequently used types, VG and VN occur less frequently in speech productions. When caretaker #1 used certain syllable types more often, child #1 would be affected and produce certain syllable types more frequently. Thus, their results show that the syllable patterns of child #1 have been largely influenced by her caretaker.

The following Figure 4.10 presented the comparison of syllable patterns between child #2 and caretaker #2.

Figure 4.10 Percentages of syllable types of child #2 and caretaker #2

V CV VG GV VN CVG CVN CGV GVG GVNCGV

According to the statistical analysis, the syllable types of child #2 and caretaker #2 are significantly correlated, r =.94, p<.05. The percentages are proven to be statistically significant. As displayed in Figure 4.10, CV is the most frequently used syllable type produced by child #2 and caretaker #2; the frequencies of CV are reported as 44.4 % and 33.4 % respectively. Additionally, CVG ranks second place in both their productions, with a usage of 13.2% and 19.6% of utterances. The most frequently used syllable types are CV, and followed by CVG. The least frequently used types, VG and VN, appear least frequently in word productions. When caretaker #2 used certain syllable types more often, child #2 would be affected and produce certain syllable types more frequently. Thus, their results show that syllable patterns of child

#2 have been largely influenced by caretaker #2.

The following Figure 4.11 presents the comparison of syllable patterns between child #3 and caretaker #3.

Figure 4.11 Percentages of syllable types of child #3 and caretaker #3

V CV VG GV VN CVG CVN CGV GV

According to the statistical analysis, the syllable types of child #3 and caretaker

#3 are significantly correlated, r =.98, p<.05. The percentages are proven to be statistically significant. As demonstrated in Figure 4.11, CV is obviously the most frequently used syllable type produced by child #3 and caretaker #3; the frequency of CV is reported as 51.0 % and 39.5 % respectively. Besides, CVG ranks second place, with a usage of 12.8% and 15.7% of the utterances. The most frequently used syllable types are CV, followed by CVG. For the least frequently used types, both VG and VN appeared least frequently in their productions. As caretaker #3 used certain syllable types more often, child #3 would be influenced and produce certain syllable types more frequently. Consequently, the findings show that the syllable patterns of child #3 still have been largely influenced by caretaker #3.

The following Figure 4.12 presents the comparison of syllable patterns between child #4 and caretaker #4.

Figure 4.12 Percentages of syllable types of child #4 and caretaker #4

V CV VG GV VN CVG CVN CGV GVG GVNCGV

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

According to the statistical analysis, there is a positive correlation between the syllable patterns of child #4 and the syllable patterns of caretaker #4, r =.88, p<.05.

The above percentages are proven to be statistically significant. Based on figure 4.12, CV is the most frequently used syllable type produced by this child-caretaker pair; the frequencies of CV are reported as 49.6 % and 36.6 % respectively. However, the second ranking and third ranking were CGV and CVG for child #4 while the ranking was CVG and CVN for caretaker #4. Furthermore, for the less frequently used types, GVN, VN and CGVN occurred less frequently in the production of child #4; VG and VN appeared less frequently in the production of caretaker #4. This child-caretaker syllable patterns are slightly different, but their patterns are still positively correlated.

While caretaker #4 used certain syllable types more often, child #4 would be affected and produce certain syllable types more frequently. Hence, their results show that syllable patterns of child #4 have largely been influenced by caretaker #4.

To sum up, for the four child-caretaker pairs, the results are significantly correlated respectively. The child’s syllable pattern is positively correlated with his/her caretaker’s syllable pattern; thus, we may deduce that young children’s phonological systems are influenced by the caretakers’ outputs.

5.1 Summary of the findings

In this study, syllable acquisition by four Mandarin-speaking children aged between 0;10 and 2;6 is observed. Two aspects are included: the overall syllable types and patterns of syllable deletion. We calculated the frequencies of each syllable type in overall syllable production and deleted syllable production, and analyzed the deletion pattern of each syllable type. There were a total number of 16515 syllable tokens analyzed in the study. The findings are summarized in the following Table 5.1.

Table 5.1 Ranking of used syllable types patterns of each child

Overall Syllable type frequency Monosyllable: CV > CVG > CGV Disyllable: CV > CVG > CGV Multisyllable: CV > CVG > CVN Syllable deletion frequency Monosyllable: CV > CGV > GV

Disyllable: CV > CGV > V Multisyllable: CV > V > CGV

Patterns of syllable deletion Postnuclear glide [w] > Final nasals [n],[ŋ] >

Initial consonant > Postnuclear glide [j] >

Prenuclear glide [w] > Prenuclear glide [j]

Child-caretaker syllable relationship Child #1 – Caretaker #1: positively correlated Child #2 – Caretaker #2: positively correlated Child #3 – Caretaker #3: positively correlated Child #4 – Caretaker #4: positively correlated

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

(1) Frequency of overall syllable types: For four children’s overall syllable production, CV has the greatest frequency of occurrence. The frequency rankings for monosyllables and disyllables are CV > CVG > CGV, with two open syllables and one closed syllable; and the ranking for multisyllables is CV > CVG > CVN, with one open syllable and two closed syllables. The syllable patterns in monosyllables were similar with the patterns in disyllables. The third ranking in multisyllables was different from the rankings in monosylables and disyllables. Generally, four children frequently use CV and CVG syllable types in the productions. The results of overall syllable types present that the children preferred unmarked syllable type, especially CV at an early age.

(2) Frequency of syllable deletion: the frequency ranking of syllable deletion is CV >

CGV > GV in monosyllabic words, and the frequency ranking is CV > CGV > V in disyllabic words. Moreover, the frequency ranking is CV > V > CGV in multisyllabic words. This indicates that open syllable types are preferred, but closed syllable types occur less frequently. Furthermore, CV has the highest frequency of occurrences in children’s syllable deletion production due to the fact that the children tend to omit the more complex and difficult syllable types to simpler syllable types.

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

(3) Patterns of syllable deletion: the syllable deletion patterns show that the children are likely to drop segments in initial-, middle-, or final-syllable position. They tend to produce the form with the deletion of postnuclear glide [w] and final nasal [n]/ [ŋ]. The deletion of posnuclear glide [w] has the highest percentage of all deletion categories with 27.5%, and is followed by nasals in the syllable-final position, which accounted for 25.7%. It is indicated that children frequently omit the segments in the syllable-final position with the glide [w] and the nasals. The deletion of prenuclear glides [j] and [w] has the least percentage of all deletion categories with 7.8% and 9.6% respectively. According to the analysis of individual differences, child #1 has the greater tendency to reduce the consonants in the syllable-initial positions, with a usage of 44.9%. However, Child #2, child

#3 and child #4 are strongly likely to omit the postnuclear glide [w]. The pattern of postnuclear glide [w] has the highest frequency with around 30% of all categories.

(4) Child-caretaker syllable relationship: the statistical analysis shows that there is a positive correlation between the child’s syllable types and his/ her caretaker’s syllable types. For children’s syllable pattern, CV is obviously the most frequently used syllable type, and is followed by CVG. For caretakers’ syllable patterns, CV, CVG and CVN have higher frequencies. As the caretaker uses certain syllable

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

types more often, the child will produce those types more often. That is, this indicates that child’s syllable patterns may be influenced by his/her caretaker’s linguistic production.

5.2 Discussion on syllable analysis

This study aims to investigate the patterns of overall syllables types and syllable deletions in Taiwan Mandarin-speaking children. Since young children’s productions of words are immature and unstable, they tend to use many different phonetic forms and drop the segments in the outputs. Syllable acquisition and syllable deletion can be attributed to many factors, including markedness factors, frequency effects, perception and production accounts (e.g., Ingram 1986; Echols, 1993; MacNeilage &

Davis, 1993; Stites et al, 2004; Ota, 2006). In this study, the relationship between syllable type frequency and syllable deletion as well as the child-caretaker syllable relationship are carefully examined.

5.2.1 The overall syllable types

For the four children’s speech productions, CV and CVG types have higher frequencies in overall syllable types while VG and VN have lower frequencies. On the one hand, in terms of the syllable types with higher frequency, CV and CVG syllable types are syllables which consist of onset consonants and do not contain consonant clusters. The results show that the frequently used syllable types generally

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

accord with the unmarked syllable criterion proposed by Jakobson (1968). Although CVG ranks as the second most frequently used type and it is not a complete open syllable, this type with the onset consonant and without the consonant cluster is considered to be a simple syllable type for the children. On the other hand, CV and CVG also have the higher frequencies in the caretakers’ syllable productions. The frequency effect can also account for the results (e.g., Ingram, 1988; Chen & Kent, 2005, 2010; Ota, 2006). That is, a target phonological structure produced by a young child is closely related to the input frequency. It is indicated that the ambient languages may influence young children’s syllable structures.

However, VG and VN have the lowest frequency of the four children’s production. They are closed syllables among the twelve syllable types in Taiwan Mandarin and they do not have the onset consonant. Although they have relatively simple structure compared to CGVG or CGVN, they are the two syllable types without onset consonant and are regarded as the difficult and marked syllable types.

Moreover, in terms of markedness theory, the more marked syllable types would be acquired later because young children would display unmarked linguistic structure first (Jakobson, 1968). Accordingly, because VG and VN are the more marked syllable types, young children would acquire these difficult types during the latter course of development (H. Y. Wang, 2014). Nevertheless, CGVG and CGVN

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

consisting of the consonant cluster do not have the lowest frequency of all utterances.

The reason may be due to the fact that the age ranges during the observation. After the age of two, words learned and produced by the children are more complex so that the frequencies of CGVG and CGVN are not the lowest in the data. Thus, syllable types with the lowest frequency may also be consistent with the constraints mentioned above, including the closed syllable, the syllable without onset, and syllable contained consonant cluster which are more marked syllable types.

5.2.2 The syllable deletion types

According to results of syllable deletion in monosyllables, disyllables and multisyllables, CV, CGV and V are the frequently used syllable types among twelve types. CV still has the highest frequency in syllable deletion words, and is then followed by CGV and V in the production of four children. They are the syllables, which include onset consonant and do not contain consonant clusters and codas.

Among syllable types used by children, it is clearly shown that the unmarked syllable CV is the most frequently used type since young children would tend to omit the segments in complex syllable types to become simple syllable types. As a result, the more unmarked syllable types are likely to be used in children’s early immature speech.

In terms of the syllable deletion forms, the children tend to use a coda-dropping

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

strategy to produce reduced words in phonological development (Levitt & Aydelott Utman, 1992; So & Dodd, 1995; Tsay, 2007). The results show that CV is the most frequently used syllable type produced by four children in overall production and syllable deletion productions. The findings are consistent with the study of McCarthy and Prince (1994), who proposed that children’s early words are governed by highly-ranked No-Coda constraints. For instance, complex target forms are easily to be realized as CV depending on the relative ranking of faithfulness constraints on the realization of segments. Hence, it may be predicted that CV syllable type is the most common outputs of syllable errors in children’s speech.

However, for least frequent syllable types of syllable deletion forms, the frequencies of GVG, GVN, VG and VN have the low frequencies among all the deleted syllable types. The frequencies of GVG and GVN account for less than 2% of the total, being only 0.7% and 1.1% respectively. These less frequently used syllable types are closed syllables and VG and VN and thus have no onset consonant, so these syllable types appear less frequently then open syllables. These results may due to the fact that Mandarin severely limits the possible coda consonants. Some studies of syllable omission in Mandarin have claimed that open syllables are more likely to undergo phonetic reduction as opposed to closed syllables (Cheng & Xu, 2009;

Burchfield & Bradlow, 2014). As a result, the findings are in accordance with

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

previous studies, indicating that languages favoring coda-less syllables may show overall higher rates of phonetic deletion

5.2.3 The syllable deletion patterns

In terms of the pattern of syllable deletion, the four children were likely to drop the segments in the syllable-final position than syllable-initial and syllable-middle position. On the one hand, the deletion of the glide [w] in syllable-final position has

the highest percentage of all the deletion categories, and is then followed by the nasal [n] and [ŋ] in syllable-final position whereas the percentage of the prenuclear glide [j]

and prenuclear glide [w] have the least frequencies of all categories. This deletion pattern is consistent with previous studies, demonstrating that young children show limited ability at final consonant production in syllable-final position (Fee & Ingram, 1982; Demuth & Johnson, 2003). Demuth and Johnson (2003) also proposed that because young children’s phonological system is unstable and immature, they are inclined to drop word-final segments and produce the targets as CV words or truncated words. On the other hand, it seemed that the glide [j] in the syllable-final position does not explained by the coda-dropping strategy because the percentage of postnuclear glide [j] has the second last frequency of all categories. But, the reason may be due to the fact that the young children seldom produced the target words which contained the postnuclear glide [j]; hence, the syllable deletion of postnuclear

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

glide [j] occurred less frequently in the analysis.

Nevertheless, when carefully examining each child’s deletion patterns, we can observe that child #1 tended to reduce the segments in the syllable-initial position rather than in the syllable-final position. The deletion of initial consonant had the highest percentage of her all reduction categories, and was then followed by final nasals and the deletion of prenuclear glide [w]. The initial consonant deletion is also in accordance with the studies of Zhu and Dodd (2000) and Tsay (2006), who claimed that young children are likely to omit the syllable-initial consonant in their early phonological acquisition. Young children are expected to delete some components such as syllable-initial consonant at the syllable level. The syllable-initial consonant deletion is very common in young children’s data (Zhu & Dodd, 2000).

In general, although there are individual differences in terms of dropping the segments in positions, young children still have undergone the syllable deletion in their immature productions. Thus, the findings of syllable deletion pattern may indicate that children tend to reduce the consonants and glides in every syllable position.

5.2.4 The syllable relationship between children and caretakers

Based on the results of children’s overall syllable production pattern, CV is obviously the most frequently used syllable type, and is then followed by CVG. For

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

caretakers’ syllable patterns, CV, CVG and CVN have higher frequencies in the results. According to the statistical findings, the syllable types of the four child-caretaker pairs are positively correlated. The two highest ranking of frequencies of syllable types were CV and CVG in the productions of pair #13, pair #2 and pair #3.

VG and VN had the least frequencies in the production of pair #1, pair #2 and pair #3.

Nevertheless, pair #4 presented slightly different patterns from the other pairs. Child

#4 used CV and CGV more frequently but used GVN and CGVN less frequently;

caretaker #4 produced CV and CVG more frequently but VN and VG appeared less frequently. One possible reason for there being slightly different syllable patterns may lies in the individual differences (Levelt et al., 2000). The children may exhibit individual differences in the productions, preferring the different complexity of the syllable types in the syllable-initial or the syllable-final.

Nevertheless, with regard to the syllable patterns of the four children-caretakers, the results are significantly correlated. In general, our findings are congruent with those previous studies (e.g., de Boysson-Bardies & Vihman, 1991; Chen & Kent, 2005; Zamuner et al., 2005; Ota, 2006). The ambient language can be observed from children’s final consonant production in their early stages (de Boysson-Bardies &

Vihman, 1991). Moreover, the finding of Chen and Kent (2005) indicate that young

3 In order to be succinct, the following child-caretaker relationship would be marked as a “pair” (e.g., pair #1 referring to child #1 and caretaker #1).

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

children’s syllable productions are relevant to their caretakers. Children’s phonological productions may be influenced by the overall productivity and the phonetic content of the ambient language. Furthermore, it can be reasoned that a young child produces a target phonological structure which is strongly related to the input frequency (Ota, 2006). To conclude, the findings lend some credence to the accounts that children’s syllable patterns have largely been influenced by their

children’s syllable productions are relevant to their caretakers. Children’s phonological productions may be influenced by the overall productivity and the phonetic content of the ambient language. Furthermore, it can be reasoned that a young child produces a target phonological structure which is strongly related to the input frequency (Ota, 2006). To conclude, the findings lend some credence to the accounts that children’s syllable patterns have largely been influenced by their