• 沒有找到結果。

Our study showed that the stroke educational program indeed could improve

college students’ knowledge towards stroke pathophysiology, risk factors, warning

symptoms and action to take when a stroke was suspected, same as other studies.4-5, 12

Regardless of the type of assessment questionnaire, mean score of both classes

improved immediately after the educational program. Overall, such educational

program design successfully conveyed message to the students.

seemed extremely well in class A (close-ended questionnaire): all students in class A

could recognize three or more risk factors and at least one warning symptom. A

study in Brazil found that up to 38% of respondents were unable to identify any risk

factors and 41% any symptoms.13 Müller-Nordhorn J et al. reported that 32% of

respondents in Berlin were not able to name one stroke risk factor.5 In northwest

India, Pandian et al concluded that 23% of the subjects did not know a single warning

symptom, and 21% couldn’t identify even a single risk factor.4 However, the

aforementioned studies all used open-ended questionnaire for assessment. If we

checked the results of class B (open-ended questionnaire) for equal comparison, the

awareness/knowledge level of our subjects was still higher than that of other studies.

There was only 5% of students couldn’t name a single risk factor and 24% did not

know any warning symptom. Any attempt on cross-study comparisons has to be

done carefully since studies generally adopted different questionnaire or survey

methods.14

The original awareness status of students was presented in the pretest results, and

such information is important for goal setting of future educational program/campaign.

Reconsidering what we have done, it seems better to design the program/campaign

based on pretest results. For instance, “genetic factors” was included in the

nonmodifiable risk factors in ASA/AHA 2006 guideline,2 but we did not introduce it

during the educational program. The recognition rate of it then dropped after the

program, somehow to our surprise. Another example was cigarette smoking: its

recognition rate was the lowest in both classes during pretest. This may imply that

college students were not alert to smoking behavior in their daily life. Besides, 4%

of students in class B replied that “bloodletting” as an emergency treatment to stroke

and which is merely a myth.15 There were also students in class B replying weather,

stress, mood, alcohol intake and family history as risk factors, yet whether they are

risk factors were still controversial.16-30 All information collected during pretest

could help identify the deficiency or myths the students have and permit more

targeted education efforts.

Mean score of male students was not significantly different from that of female

students except in the posttest of class B (p<0.05). However, sample size was too

small (34 male and 22 female in class A; 12 male and 41 female in class B) for us to

assert that gender was a factor that has influence on the learning performance.

The coding rule may affect the evaluation of educational effects, so does the

questionnaire design. The overall average score of class A was higher than that of

class B. This might due to the fact that close-ended questionnaire somehow

reminded students of the message introduced on the slides. Moreover, we did not

so was to avoid underestimating the learning effect. If we coded these two risk

factors as correct, the mean score of posttest would be lower than present data since

the recognition rate of them dropped after the intervention. As a matter of fact, these

two risk factors were listed on the “how to prevent a stroke” section of the slide

instead of “risk factors” one. And since we did not measure students’ attitude

towards how to prevent stroke, we may not assert that students were not aware that

they can reduce risk of stroke through dietary control and physical exercise. This

was why we kept them neutral to not dilute the education effect of the other listed risk

factors.

Mean score of both classes declined in 12 weeks follow-up. This result implied

that education effect faded away with time as expected. Another study showed the

same tendency.9 How to maintain students’ knowledge of stroke was therefore

crucial, otherwise such education attempt was just a waste of time and resources. It

seems that we have to take action to reinforce students’ awareness. Observing the

world of advertising, there appears to be three underlying principles for getting

messages out to people, and ensuring messages to be remembered. They are (1)

consistency, (2) simplicity, and (3) repetition.31 In our educational program design,

we did arrange and list the key points of risk factors and warning symptoms on the

slide to simplify the message. The trade-off between simple and comprehensive

message came into question.32-33 To evaluate the repetition effect or how long

should the interval be still await further investigation.

Gaps usually exist between knowing and doing. There were several developed

models about individual health behavior discussing how to stimulate positive behavior

changes, such as Health Belief Model (HBM), Theory of Reasoned Action (TRA),

and the Transtheoretical Model (TTM).34 Individual perception/awareness precedes

their behavioral change. Our study provides evidence that through such educational

program, we may improve students’ perception/awareness. Based on the finding of

this pilot study, we may further apply aforementioned model to enhance behavioral

change.

This study has limitations. We took convenient samples instead of randomized

ones. The results may only be representative for participants with college education

in rural area. Also, due to insufficient samples, we didn’t adjust the results by

individual characteristics other than genders to avoid statistical bias. Care must be

taken to generalize these results to other populations.

In conclusion, there were still gaps of stroke knowledge among college students

to fill. A well-designed educational program, which takes into account the primary

awareness status of the targeted audience, may be an effective way to bridge the gap.

Therefore, conducting a large scale research to identify the deficiency of stroke

knowledge of targeted population for further educational program/campaign design

seems worthwhile.

Reference

1. 2006 National Health Insurance Annual Statistics Reports. 2006. Available at

www.doh.gov.tw/statistic/data/全民健康保險統計年報/95 醫療統計年報 /01/950128.xls/. Assessed Jun 08, 2008.

2. Goldstein LB, Adams R, Alberts MJ, Appel LJ, Brass LM, Bushnell CD,

Culebras A, Degraba TJ, Gorelick PB, Guyton JR, Hart RG, Howard G,

Kelly-Hayes M, Nixon JV, Sacco RL. Primary prevention of ischemic stroke: a

guideline from the American Heart Association/American Stroke Association

Stroke Council: cosponsored by the Atherosclerotic Peripheral Vascular Disease

Interdisciplinary Working Group; Cardiovascular Nursing Council; Clinical

Cardiology Council; Nutrition, Physical Activity, and Metabolism Council; and

the Quality of Care and Outcomes Research Interdisciplinary Working Group: the

American Academy of Neurology affirms the value of this guideline. Stroke. 2006;

37: 1583-633.

3. Reeves MJ, Rafferty AP, Aranha AAR, Theisen V. Changes in Knowledge of

stroke risk factors and warning signs among Michigan adults. Cerebrovasc Dis.

2008; 25: 385-91.

4. Pandian JD, Jaison A, Deepak SS, Kalra G, Shamsher S, Lincoln DJ, Abraham G.

Public awareness of warning symptoms, risk factors, and treatment of stroke in

northwest India. Stroke. 2005; 36: 644-48.

5. Müller-Nordhorn J, Nolte CH, Rossnagel K, Jungehülsing GJ, Reich A, Roll S,

Villringer A, Willich SN. Knowledge about risk factors for stroke: a

population-based survey with 28,090 participants. Stroke. 2006; 37: 946-50.

6. Gupta A, Thomas P. Knowledge of stroke symptoms and risk factors among

at-risk elderly patients in the UK. Int J Clin Pract. 2002; 56: 634-7.

7. Sug Yoon S, Heller RF, Levi C, Wiggers J, Fitzgerald PE. Knowledge of stroke

risk factors, warning symptoms, and treatment among an Australian urban

population. Stroke. 2001; 32:1926-30.

8. Kraywindel K, Heidrich J, Heuschmann PU, Wagner M, Berger K. Stroke risk

perception among participants of a stroke awareness campaign. BMC Public

Health. 2007; 7:39.

9. Miller ET, King KA, Miller R, Kleindorfer D. FAST Stroke Prevention

Educational Program for Middle School Students: pilot study results. J Neurosci

Nurs. 2007; 39: 236-42.

10. Morgenstern LB, Gonzales NR, Maddox KE, Brown DL, Karim AP, Espinosa N,

Moyé LA, Pary JK, Grotta JC, Lisabeth LD, Conley KM. A randomized,

controlled trial to teach middle school children to recognize stroke and call 911:

the kids identifying and defeating stroke project. Stroke. 2007; 38: 2972-78.

11. Yeh BH, Cheng CH, Wang YH, Huang LH, Chan YC, Hu MH, Teng YC, Lin CY,

Lin YC, Li CC, Hsu SH. The northern Taiwan stroke center for prevention and

treatment project 2004. Taipei: Dept. of health (TW), Bureau of Health

Promotion; 2004 Report No.: DOH93-HP-1114. 2004.

12. Stern EB, Berman M, Thomas JJ, Klassen AC. Community education for stroke

awareness: An efficacy study. Stroke. 1999; 30: 720-3.

13. Campos-Sousa RN, Soares VY, Almeida KJ, Carvalho LI, Jacobina KS, Athayde

Netto AE, Macêdo Ede A, Veloso LA, Knowledge of stroke among a brazilian

urban population. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2007; 65: 587-91.

14. Billings-Gagliardi S, Mazor KM. Development and validation of the stroke

action test. Stroke. 2005; 36: 1035-9.

15. O’Connor A. The Claim: Really? Pricking a stroke victim’s fingers can help

delay symptoms. New York Times. 2006 Nov 21; Sect. F:5.

16. Dawson J, Weir C, Wright F, Bryden C, Aslanyan S, Lees K, Bird W, Walters M.

Associations between meteorological variables and acute stroke hospital

admissions in the west of Scotland. Acta Neurol Scand. 2008; 117: 85-9.

17. Ohshige K,. Hori Y, Tochikubo O, Sugiyama M. Influence of weather on

emergency transport events coded as stroke: population-based study in Japan. Int

J Biometeorol. 2006; 50: 305–11.

18. Oberg AL, Ferguson JA, McIntyre LM, Horner RD. Incidence of stroke and

season of the year: evidence of an association. Am J Epidemiol. 2000; 152:

558-64.

19. Feigin VL, Nikitin YP, Bots ML, Vinogradova TE, Grobbee DE. A

population-based study of the associations of stroke occurrence with weather

parameters in Siberia, Russia (1982-92). Eur J Neurol. 2000; 7: 171-8.

20. Low RB, Bielory L, Qureshi AI, Dunn V, Stuhlmiller DF, Dickey DA. The

relation of stroke admissions to recent weather, airborne allergens, air pollution,

seasons, upper respiratory infections, and asthma incidence, September 11, 2001,

and day of the week. Stroke. 2006; 37: 951-7.

21. Field TS, Hill MD. Weather, Chinook, and stroke occurrence. Stroke. 2002; 33:

1751-7.

22. Truelsen T, Nielsen N, Boysen G, Grønbaek M. Self-reported stress and risk of

stroke: the Copenhagen City Heart Study. Stroke. 2003; 34: 856-62.

23. Salaycik KJ, Kelly-Hayes M, Beiser A, Nguyen AH, Brady SM, Kase CS, Wolf

PA. Depressive symptoms and risk of stroke: The Framingham study. Stroke.

2007; 38: 16-21.

24. Carod-Artal FJ. Are mood disorders a stroke risk factor? Stroke. 2007; 38: 1-3.

25. Mukamal KJ, Chung H, Jenny NS, Kuller LH, Longstreth WT Jr, Mittleman MA,

Burke GL, Cushman M, Beauchamp NJ Jr, Siscovick DS. Alcohol use and risk of

ischemic stroke among older adults: the cardiovascular health study. Stroke. 2005;

36: 1830-4.

26. Mukamal KJ, Ascherio A, Mittleman MA, Conigrave KM, Camargo CA Jr,

Kawachi I, Stampfer MJ, Willett WC, Rimm EB. Alcohol and risk for ischemic

stroke in men: the role of drinking patterns and usual beverage. Ann Intern Med.

2005; 142: 11-9.

27. Iso H, Baba S, Mannami T, Sasaki S, Okada K, Konishi M, Tsugane S. Alcohol

consumption and risk of stroke among middle-aged men: the JPHC Study Cohort

I. Stroke. 2004; 35: 1124-9.

28. Reynolds K, Lewis B, Nolen JD, Kinney GL, Sathya B, He J. Alcohol

consumption and risk of stroke: a meta-analysis. JAMA. 2003; 289: 579-88.

29. Kim H, Friedlander Y, Longstreth WT Jr, Edwards KL, Schwartz SM, Siscovick

DS. Family history as a risk factor for stroke in young women. Am J Prev Med.

2004; 27: 391-6.

30. Kubota M, Yamaura A, Ono J, Itani T, Tachi N, Ueda K, Nagata I, Sugimoto S. Is

family history an independent risk factor for stroke? J Neurol Neurosurg

Psychiatry. 1997; 62: 66-70.

31. Hodgson CS. FAST or not to FAST. Stroke. 2007; 38: 2631-2.

32. Kleindorfer KO, Miller R, Moomaw CJ, Alwell K, Broderick JP, Khoury J, Woo

D, Flaherty ML, Zakaria T, Kissela BM. Designing a Message for Public

Education Regarding Stroke: does FAST Capture Enough Stroke? Stroke. 2007;

38: 2864-8.

33. Chiti A, Giorli E, Orlandi G. FAST or Not to FAST: SHOUT-FAST! Stroke. 2008;

39: 40-1.

34. Glanz K, Lewis FM, Rimer BK. Health behavior and health education. 2nd ed.

(CA): Jossey-Bass Publishers; 1997. p.37-139.

*All the numbers in this figure means number of students.

Figure 1. Flow of Students through the Study.

Class A (Close-Ended)

Table 1. Stroke Related Knowledge Named by Students in Class A. (n=56) Frequency (%)

Pretest Posttest 12 week follow-up

p value*

I.Where a stroke occurs-brain 46(82) 56(100) 55(98) 0.47 II Risk factors

Numbness or lack of feeling(on one side of the body)

*p value was from comparison of mean score of pretest between male and female participants by χ2 test.

Table 2. Comparison of Mean Score by Gender for Class A. (n=56)

Mean Score (Standard Deviation)

Gender

Frequency (%)

Pretest Posttest

12 weeks follow-up

Male 34(61) 15.0(1.98) 19.2(0.88) 17.5(1.44) Female 22(39) 14.5(1.57) 19.1(1.17) 17.2(1.37) Total 56(100) 14.8(1.83) 19.2(0.99) 17.4(1.41)

p value* 0.1563 0.4003 0.1840

*Student’s t test results of the equality of mean score between male and female participants.

Comparison of Mean Score (Class A)

8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20

pretest posttest 12 week follow-up Evaluation

Mean Score male

female

Figure 2. Comparison of Mean Score for Class A (tested by close-ended questionnaire)

Table 3. Stroke Related Knowledge Named by Students in Class B. (n=53) Frequency (%)

Pretest Posttest 12 week

follow-up

p value*

I.Where a stroke occurs-brain 42(80) 53(100) 52(98) 0.75 II Risk factors

Cardiopathy 13(25) 47(89) 37(70) 0.47

Cigarette smoking 6(11) 49(92) 34(64) 0.09

Hypertension 27(51) 53(100) 43(81) 0.47

High blood cholesterol level 17(32) 46(87) 20(38) 0.55

Obesity 14(26) 39(74) 34(64) 0.38

feeling(on one side of the body)

21(40) 45(85) 41(77) 0.87

Bloodletting 2(4) 0(0) 0(0) 0.35

*p value was from comparison of mean score of pretest between male and female participants by χ2 test.

Table 4 Comparison of Mean Score by Gender for class B. (n=53)

Mean Score (Standard Deviation)

Gender

Frequency (%)

Pretest Posttest

12 weeks follow-up

Male 12(23) 8.9(1.80) 17.8(1.60) 13.8(2.34) Female 41(77) 9.0(1.75) 18.9(1.13) 15.0(2.55)

Total 53(100) 8.9(1.75) 18.7(1.33) 14.7(2.54)

p value* 0.4034 0.0029 0.0640

*Student’s t test results of the equality of mean score between male and female participants.

Comparison of Mean Score (Class B)

8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20

pretest posttest 12 week follow-up Evaluation

Mean Score male

female

Figure 3. Comparison of Mean Score for Class B (tested by open-ended questionnaire)

相關文件