• 沒有找到結果。

Chapter 6 Conclusion

6.2 Limitations and suggestions

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

minimization, in prohibitions. Additionally, bald and minimization are two primary modifications in both control acts after examining the data at each of child’s four ages.

The mother’s power status is higher than that of the child’s, so it is appropriate for her to choose bald. The use of minimization obviously belongs to politeness. Therefore, politeness is embedded in the Mandarin mother’s regulatory language, and the maternal input will help the child to ultimately acquire pragmatic competence in politeness. This study also discovered two major kinds of semantic combinations. One is minimization and repetition. The other is minimization and justification. Finally, the results of contents for control acts show that competent action often occurs in

directives. As for prohibitions, appropriate behavior and caretaking are most related.

Based on our findings, the Mandarin mother seems to favor the direct, imperative form of regulatory language, and semantic bald or minimization forms.

6.2 Limitations and suggestions

Although we have gained some findings about the Mandarin maternal

regulatory language, there are still limitations in this study. First, the results derived from this study may be influenced by an issue of individual differences, but this study would certainly shed some light on the Mandarin maternal regulatory language.

Second, the child’s cognitive development at four ages all belongs to the

preoperational stage, so the findings may be limited in application. It is suggested that future studies may investigate children at other cognitive stages, in order to see if their parents’ regulatory language changes with the growth of the comprehension abilities of the children. Third, the present study only focused on a boy with his mother. The sample is not big enough, so more research should be done with a larger sample.

Fourth, future research may investigate that either directive or prohibition would be

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

the most effective way to regulate children. Next, some studies also have found variations in the regulatory language of parents when speaking to either their sons or their daughters (e.g., Gleason, Ely, Perlmann, and Narasimhan, 1996), so future research also might examine the maternal regulatory language used with children of different genders. Last, the use of more direct forms may elicit more compliance from children (e.g., Schaffer and Crook, 1980), so future research may investigate the relationship between compliance rate and form.

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

Transcribing symbols

(Adopted from MacWhinney, Brian. 1995. The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.)

xxx unintelligible speech, not treated as a word xx unintelligible speech, treated as a word . period

? question

! exclamation

# pause

### extra long pause between words -: lengthening

+… trailing off

+^ quick uptake

[/] retracing without correction [//] retracing with correction

<> portion of utterances been overlapped [>][<] overlapping utterances +/. interruption

+, self-completion [= text] explanation

[=! text] paralinguistic material

%act: action tier

%sit: situation tier

%com: comments by investigator

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

Aronsson, K., & Thorell, M. (1999). Family politics in children’s play directives.

Journal of Pragmatics, 31, 25-47.

Barber, B. K. (1996). Parental psychological control: Revisiting a neglected construct. Child Development, 67(6), 3296-3319.

Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Developmental Psychology Monograph, 4(1), 1-130.

Baumrind, D. (1991). The influence of parenting style on adolescent competence and substance use. Journal of Early Adolescence, 11(1), 56-95.

Baumrind, D., & Black, A. E. (1967). Socialization practices associated with

dimensions of competence in preschool boys and girls. Child Development, 38, 291-327.

Bellinger, D. (1979). Changes in the explicitness of mothers’ directives as children age. Journal of Child Language, 6, 443-458.

Blum-Kulka, S. (1989). Playing it safe: The role of conventionality in indirectness.

In S. Blum-Kulka, J. House & G. Kasper (Eds.), Cross-cultural pragmatics:

Request and apologies (pp. 37-70). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Blum-Kulka, S., House, J., & Kasper, G. (1989). Cross-cultural pragmatics:

Requests and apologies. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Blum-Kulka, S. (1990). You don’t touch lettuce with your fingers: Parental politeness in family discourse. Journal of Pragmatics, 14, 259-288.

Bond, M. H., Wan, K., Leung, K., & Giacalone, R. A. (1985). How are responses to verbal insult related to cultural collectivism and power distance? Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 16 (1), 111-127.

Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (1987). Politeness: Some universals in language usage.

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

Cheah, C. S. L., & Rubin, K. H. (2003). European American and Mainland Chinese mothers’ socialization beliefs regarding preschoolers’ social skills. Parenting:

Science and Practice, 3, 1-22.

Chen, G. M., & Chung, J. (1994). The impact of Confucianism on organizational communication. Communication Quarterly, 41, 93-105.

Culpeper, J. (1996). Towards an anatomy of impoliteness. Journal of Pragmatics, 25, 349-367.

Damon, W. (2006). Socialization and individuation. In G. Handel (Ed.), Childhood socialization (2th ed., pp. 3-10). New Brunswick, NJ; London: Aldine

Transaction.

Darling, N. (1999). Parenting style and its correlates. Eric Digest, EDO-PS-99-3.

Dorfman, P. W., & Howell, J. P. (1988). Dimensions of national culture and effective leadership patterns: Hofstede revisited, Advances in International Comparative Management, 3, 127-150.

Dubin, E. R., & Dubin, R. (1963). The authority inception period in socialization.

Child Development, 34, 885-898.

Dunn, J. (1988). The beginnings of social understanding. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Ervin-Tripp, S. M. (1976). Is Sybil there? The structure of some American English directives. Language in Society, 5, 25-66.

Ervin-Tripp, S. M. (1977). Wait for me, roller skate. In S. Ervin-Tripp, & C.

Mitchell-Kernan (Eds.), Child discourse (pp. 165-188). New York: Academic Press.

Ervin-Tripp, S. M. (1988). Control exchange code. Berkeley: University of

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

Ervin-Tripp, S. M., Guo, J., & Lampert, M. (1990). Politeness and persuasion in children’s control acts. Journal of Pragmatics, 14, 195-219.

Forbes, G., Zhang, X., Doroszewicz, K., & Haas, K. (2009). Relationships between individualism-collectivism, gender, and direct or indirect Aggression: A study in China, Poland, and the U.S. Aggressive Behavior, 35, 24–30.

Fukushima, S. (2000). Request and culture: Politeness in British English and Japanese. Berlin: Peter Lang.

Gao, H. (1999). Features of request strategies in Chinese. Lund Universality, Dept.

of Linguistics. Working Papers, 47, 73-86.

Garvey, C. (1975). Requests and responses in children’s speech. Journal of Child Language, 2, 41-63.

Gleason, J. B. (1988). Language and socialization. In F. S. Kessel (Ed.), The development of language and language researchers: Essays in honor of roger Brown. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Gleason, J. B., Ely, R., Perlmann, R. Y., & Narasimhan, B. (1996). Patterns of

prohibition in parent-child discourse. In D. I. Slobin, J. Gerhardt, A. Kyratzis & J.

Guo (Eds.), Social interaction, social context, and language: Essays in honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp (pp. 205-217). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Gralinski, J. H., & Kopp, C. B. (1993). Everyday rules for behavior: Mothers’

requests to young children. Developmental Psychology, 29, 573-584.

Halle, T., & Shatz, M. (1994). Mothers’ social regulatory language to young children in family settings. First Language, 14, 83-104.

Hays, J., Power, T. G., & Olvera, N. (2001). Effects of maternal socialization strategies on children’s nutrition knowledge and behavior. Applied

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

Hetherington, E. M., & Parke, R. D. (1975). Child psychology: A contemporary viewpoint. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Hoffman, M. L. (1977). Moral internalization: Current theory and research. In L.

Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp.85-133). New York: Academic Press.

Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture's consequences: International differences in work-related values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Hofstede, G., & Bond, M. H. (1988). The Confucius connection: From cultural roots to economic growth. Organizational Dynamics, 16(4), 4–21.

Hong, W. (1998). Request patterns in Chinese and German: A cross-cultural study.

Munich: Lincom Europa.

Huitt, W., & Hummel, J. (2003). Piaget's theory of cognitive development.

Educational Psychology Interactive. Valdosta, GA: Valdosta State University.

Retrieved April 20, 2010 from

http://www.edpsycinteractive.org/topics/cogsys/piaget.html

James, S. L. (1978). Effects of listener age and situation on the politeness of children's directives. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 7, 307-317.

Junefelt, K., & Tulviste, T. (1997). Regulation and praise in American, Estonian, and Swedish mother-child interaction. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 4 (1), 24-33.

Kuczynski, L., Kochanska, G., Radke-Yarrow, M., & Girnius-Brown, O. (1987). A developmental interpretation of young children’s noncompliance.

Developmental Psychology, 23(6), 799-806.

Kuczynski, L., & Kochanska, G. (1990). Development of children’s noncompliance strategies from toddlerhood to age 5. Developmental Psychology, 26 (3),

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

Kuczynski, L., & Kochanska, G. (1995). Function and content of maternal demands:

Developmental significance of early demands for competent action. Child Development, 66, 616- 628.

Lakoff, R. (1973). The logic of politeness; or, minding your p’s and q’s. Chicago Linguistic Society, 9, 292-305.

Lampert, M. D., & Ervin-Tripp, S. M. (1993).Structured coding for the study of language and social interaction. In J. A. Edwards, & M. D. Lampert (Eds.), Talking data: Transcription and coding in discourse research (pp. 169-206).

Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Lytton, H. (1980). Parent-child interaction: The socialization process observed in twin and singleton families. New York: Plenum.

Maccoby, E. E. (1980). Social development: Psychological growth and the parent-child relationship. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization in the context of the family:

Parent-child interaction. In E. M. Hetherington (Vol. Ed.), & P. H. Mussen (Series Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, personality, and social development (4th ed., pp. 1-101). New York: Wiley.

McDonald, L., & Pien, D. (1982). Mother conversational behaviour as a function of interactional intent. Journal of Child Language, 9, 337-358.

McLaughlin, B., Schutz, C., & White, D. (1980). Parental speech to five-year-old children in a game-playing situation. Child Developmental, 51, 580-582.

McLaughlin, B. (1983). Child compliance to parental control techniques.

Developmental Psychology, 19(5), 667-673.

Minton, C., Kagan, J., & LeVine, J. A. (1971). Maternal control and obedience in the

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

Newport, E.L., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L. R. (1977). Mother, I'd rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In C. E. Snow, &

C. A. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to Children: Language input and acquisition (pp.

109-149). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Olsen-Fulero, L. (1982). Style and stability in mother conversational behaviour: A study of individual differences. Journal of Child Language, 9, 543-564.

Perry, D. G., & Perry, L. C. (1983). Social learning, causal attribution, and moral internalization. In J. Bisanz, G. L. Bisanz & R. Kail (Eds.), Learning in

children: Progress in cognitive development research (pp. 105-136). New York:

Springer-Verlag.

Power, T. G., & Manire, S. H. (1992). Child rearing and internalization: A

developmental perspective. In: J. Janssens, & J. Gerris (Eds.), Child rearing:

Influence on prosocial and moral development (pp. 101-123). Amsterdam: Swets

& Zeitlinger.

Rosen, M. (1989). Piagetian theory and cognitive therapy. In: A. Freeman, K.M.

Simon, L.E. Beutler & H. Arkowitz (Eds.), Comprehensive handbook of cognitive therapy (pp. 189–212). New York: Plenum Press.

Rue, Y., & Zhang, G. (2008). Requests strategies: A comparative study in

Mandarin Chinese and Korean. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

Sachs , J. (1980). The role of adult-child play in language development. In K. Rubin (Ed.), Children’s play. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Schaffer, H. R. (2006). The mutuality of parental control in early childhood. In G.

Handel (Ed.), Childhood socialization (2th ed., pp. 45-64). New Brunswick, NJ;

London: Aldine Transaction.

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

play situation. Child Development, 50, 989-998.

Schaffer, H. R., & Crook, C. K. (1980). Child compliance and maternal control techniques. Developmental Psychology, 16(1), 54-61.

Schneiderman, M. H. (1983). “Do what I mean, not what I say!” Changes in mothers’ action-directives to young children. Journal of Child Language, 10, 357-367.

Scollon, R., & Scollon, S. (1981). Narrative, literacy, and face in interethnic communication. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Searle, J. R. (1975). Indirect speech acts. In P. Cole, & J. L. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics: Vol. 3. Speech acts (pp. 59-82). New York: Academic Press.

Searle, J. R. (1976). A classification of illocutionary acts. Language in Society, 5, 1-23.

Shatz, M. (1978). Children’s comprehension of their mothers’ question-directives.

Journal of Child Language, 5, 39-46.

Shatz, M. (1994). A toddler’s life: Becoming a person. New York: Oxford University Press.

Snow, C., Perlmann, R., Gleason, J., & Hooshyar, N. (1990). Developmental perspectives on politeness: Sources of children’s knowledge. Journal of Pragmatics, 14, 289-305.

Triandis, H. C. (1993). Collectivism and individualism as cultural syndromes.

Cross-Cultural Research, 27, 155-180.

Triandis, H. C. (1994). Culture and Social behaviour. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Triandis, H. C., McCusker, C., & Hui, C. H. (1990). Multimethod probes of individualism and collectivism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

Tulviste, T. (1995). Mothers’ regulation of their two-year-olds’ behavior in two settings. In: K. Junefelt (Ed.), Activity theory. Proceedings of the XIVth

Scandinavian Conference of Linguistics and the VIIIth Conference of Nordic and General Linguistics, August 16-21, 1993. Gothenburg Papers in Theoretic

Linguistics 73. Göteborg: Göteborg University Press, 127-137.

Tulviste, T., & Raudsepp, M. (1997). The conversational style of Estonian mothers.

First Language, 17, 151–63.

Wertsch, J. V. (1979). From social interaction to higher psychological processes: A clarification and application of Vygotsky’s theory. Human Development, 22, 1-22.

Yu, C. L. (2004). Infixing with a vengeance: Pingding Mandarin infixation. Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 13, 39-58.

相關文件