• 沒有找到結果。

Influence of the Assistance from NESTs on Students’ Different Listening

Chapter 4 Results and Discussion

4.2 Influence of the Assistance from NESTs on Students’ Different Listening

Comprehension Skills

Question 2: Do the effects of having NESTs as English Teaching Assistants vary by

different listening comprehension skills?

On the whole, participants with more than three years of NESTs’ assistance

perform better throughout the entire test. That is, the average scores participants with

more than three years of NESTs’ assistance received in every part of the test were fairly

higher than those with less than a year NESTs’ assistance. Table 10 illustrates the

findings of the descriptive statistic of less-than-a-year NESTs’ assisting group and

more-than-three-year NESTs’ assisting group in each test part. The statistically

significant differences of the listening comprehension performance in each part

between less than a year of NESTs assistance group and more than three of NESTs

assistance group were reported in Table 11. The differences between the groups in Part

One is slightly smaller than the other parts; however, it has almost reached statistically

48

significant differences (t=-1.95, p<.051).

Table 10. Descriptive Statistic of Less-than-a-year NESTs Assistance Group and More-than-three-year NESTs Assistant Group in Each Test Part

Part Group N Mean S.D. Min. Max.

49

50

Part Six Less-than-a-year NESTs Assistance

278 1.58 1.67 0 5

More-than-three-year NESTs Assistance

304 2.18 1.77 0 5

51

Table 11. Difference of Less-than-a-year NESTs Assistance Group and More-than-three-year NESTs Assistant Group in Each Test Part

Part Mean

Generally speaking, the listening competence that participants should perform

throughout the present communicative test matches the two major communicative

listening sub-skills that were proposed by Weir (1993) in the taxonomy of

communicative listening sub-skills, including (1) direct meaning comprehension, such

as listening for the main idea(s) or important information, being able to distinguish from

supporting detail, or being able to listen for specifics, and being able to include a recall

of important details, and (2) inferred meaning comprehension, such as deducing the

meaning of unfamiliar lexical items from context, recognizing the communicative

function of utterances, relating utterances to their social and situational contexts.

52

In Part One, according to the rubric of the listening test, participants should be able

to deduct the meaning of unfamiliar lexical items from context (Weir, 1993). Once the

students heard the keywords such as lion, monkey, dog, they could draw lines from the

names of the children to the corresponding figures on the picture. Most of the keywords

for the correct answers were already learned by the participants and the directions to

the right answers are rather straightforward. It should, however, be noted that even

though the listening comprehension test used in the present study was adapted from

standardized tests and the vocabulary had been tailored into elementary level following

the elementary vocabulary list proposed by the MOE, some of the keywords to the

correct answers remained unchanged such as “crocodile” and “lizard” in order to reflect

the authenticity of communicating in English. After all, one cannot expect to understand

every single word in an authentic conversation or listening passage (Morrow, 1979).

Participants with the more years of NESTs’ assistance achieved higher mean

scores in Part One than their counterparts. In order to get the correct answer, participants

should be able to perform inferred meaning comprehension, namely deducting the

meaning of unfamiliar lexical items from context (Weir, 1993). For example, they

should be able to know the boy who was drawing a crocodile is next to the girl who is

drawing a monkey. Even though the participants might not know the word “crocodile,”

the participants should be able to understand the context that “the boy is next to the girl.”

53

When analyzing the classroom observation reports, we also found that NESTs would

use English to give instructions such as “Turn in your paper,” and “Time’s up,” or

“Markers down,” and so on. Although students might not understand the exact meaning,

they were able to infer from the context (Buck, 2001). In the class observation report,

students also pointed out that when they did not understand what the NESTs were

talking about, they would just make a guess based on the task they were doing. When

they took the correct action, the NESTs would smile and praise. As a result, NESTs

seemed to be good at getting learners to participate and arouse their willingness to illicit

or guess meaning from the context (Benke & Medgyes, 2005). It seems that NESTs do

facilitate L2 learners to comprehend the target language in real-life situations (Rogers

& Medley, 1988) for they had built up the ability to deduce the meaning judging from

the given context or situation.

In Part Three, participants should also be able to perform inferring meaning

comprehension. To be more specific, Part Three required test-takers to relate utterances

to their social and situational contexts (Weir, 1993). Once the participants heard the

question words such as what, how many, which, they should choose the correct answer

judging from the given situational context in the listening test. The data showed the

participants with more-than-three-year of NESTs’ assistance had done better on this

part than the participants with less-than-a-year of NESTs’ assistance. This might

54

address that participants from more-than-three-year of NESTs’ assistance group had

gradually developed the tendency of listening carefully as soon as the NESTs started

asking questions, for it was important to hear the very first word of a question due to

the longer length of interacting with the NESTs than their counterparts. Some

researchers also found that NESTs focused more on listening and speaking (Lin, 2008;

Medgyes, 1999). Such finding is in consistent with the results of the present study and

classroom observation. As the classroom observation reports stated, NESTs usually

started the class with greetings and daily routine questions. The NEST asked the

students about the day and a student replied but not in the way the NEST wanted. The

teacher corrected the student by restating the correct answer. Upon hearing the NEST’s

answer, the students laughed. Although the laughter was a small gesture, it showed that

the students were becoming aware of the NEST’s expected answers. The NEST did not

correct the student by pointing out the misunderstanding of the question, neither did the

NEST ask the student to refer to the textbook for what they have already learned. The

NEST we observed was being friendly and encouraging to get learners to speak, which

is in line with the finding of the previous study (Benke & Medgyes, 2005). The students

were experiencing the language which is used for real communication by native

speakers (Rogers & Medley, 1988). Therefore, students with more years of NESTs were

better at performing inferred meaning in the listening test as they had more chances to

55

practice with the NESTs class.

In Part Two, Four, Five and Six, although most of the keywords were learned

(foods, color, etc.), participants could not get the correct answer before listening to the

entire conversation which takes time and patience. Participants were expected to: (1)

listen for specifics, including a recall of important details in Part Two (2) listen for the

main idea(s) or important information; and identification of supporting detail or

examples in Part Four and Six (3) determine a speaker’s attitude or intention towards a

listener or a topic in Part Five base on the rubric of the listening test (Weir, 1993). The

results showed the participants with more-than-three-year of NESTs’ assistance had

done better on all the above-mentioned parts than the participants with less-than-a-year

of NESTs’ assistance. It might suggest that with the presence of the NESTs, students

have more opportunities to encounter authentic English in real life such as listening to

class instructions and listening to longer spoken content (Benke & Medgyes, 2005).

During the classroom observation analysis, we found that participants with fewer

years of NESTs’ assistance tended to be quieter and less active; they were not interested

nor participative in the activities or games that the NESTs prepared. Most of the

participants sat quietly and listened with hardly any facial expression. Furthermore,

they only spoke when they were asked to. When they did speak, they usually spoke

very quietly and uttered very simple answers. They relied more on the translation of the

56

NNESTs as they were more aware of the learning difficulties and needs of the students

(Florence Ma, 2012). On the other hand, participants with more years of NESTs’

assistance seemed much more involved in the classroom. There were more responses

from the participants to NESTs while engaging in classroom tasks. For example, some

participants would respond to the NEST and also help with classroom management.

And the participants would all keep quiet and pay attention to the instructions. In

addition, NESTs often took time to have small chats with the students and showed their

care. From the learners’ response, it was observed that the students were very happy to

share and tell the NEST about themselves. For example, one NEST asked about the

learners’ dance performance and congratulated them on their good work before she

began her class. There were many other small chats during the class time where the

NESTs would ask learners about their tests, performances and school life in general. It

seemed that NEST would make an effort to become more interactive with the students

by asking about their lives and provide supports such as words in English when needed

(Florence Ma, 2012; Poon & Higginbottom, 2000). The learners, on the other hand,

were not shy to respond and report about their current situation in English.

Previous studies have found that learners that had been previously taught by

NNESTs showed more positive attitudes towards NNESTs (Moussu, 2010). According

to Moussu (2010), variables such as teacher-contact time, students’ first language, class

57

subject, and teachers’ countries of origin significantly affected learners’ attitude

towards their teachers; thus, learners’ preference influences their relationship with the

NESTs. Contrary to the studies that have found in the past on how NESTs were more

distant from the learners than the local teachers, this study found that the NESTs made

efforts in becoming closer to learners by chatting more with them. From the learners’

responses, it seemed that the strategy was quite effective as the learners were found to

appreciate the time interacting with NESTs even though they could not understand fully

what the NESTs were talking about but they would try hard to get the main idea from

the context by making a guess or discussing with other learners. It is believed that

NESTs do facilitate L2 learners to comprehend the target language in real life situations

(Rogers & Medley, 1988) and improve learners’ listening comprehension by providing

both top-down, such as training learners to get the key message of a given task in the

classroom, and bottom-up process opportunities, such as providing vocabulary supports

when interacting with them. In short, it seems that participants with more years of

NESTs’ assistance were more likely to develop a tolerance for long unknown

information, which is similar to the types of conversations participants encountered in

the present communicative listening test. This might be the reason why participants

with more-than-three-year of NESTs’ assistance performed significantly better than

their counterparts did in different Parts of the listening test in this study. Through

58

continuous exposure to listening training, NESTs might have helped training students’

ability to concentrate and tolerate unfamiliar words and sentences in English learning

especially listening comprehension (Buck, 1995; Field, 2010; Goh, 2002; Rubin, 2004;

Vandergrift, 2007).

59

Chapter 5

Conclusion

5.1 Summary and Implication of the Findings

The present study mainly examined the effectiveness of NESTs’ input on younger

learners in Taiwan by applying a communicative listening test. The participants were

recruited from different parts of Taiwan including the North, the East, the Middle, the

South, and the offshore islands in order to reflect the overall picture in Taiwan. The

scores of the listening test were collected and analyzed statistically.

In general, NESTs had a positive effect on students’ English listening ability,

which was shown in the improvement of participants’ listening comprehension test

scores throughout the six areas investigated in the present study. Such result is in line

with the previously reviewed studies that having NESTs and NNESTs to co-teach can

make a better learning environment thus improves students’ English learning. It seems

that the benefits gained from the assistance of NESTs, especially in terms of improving

learner’s listening comprehension skills, may address that such assistance in whichever

co-teaching model is beneficial to students in Taiwan. Furthermore, the participants

with three years or more years of NESTs assistance not only showed improvement in

the mean scores of the entire listening test, but also outperformed their counterparts in

every part of the listening test. Such finding might suggest that NESTs facilitate L2

60

learners to comprehend the target language in real-life situations and help trained

students’ ability to concentrate and tolerate unfamiliar words and sentences in English

learning. Bearing the findings of the annual reports presented by the co-teaching

programs, and studies conducted by researchers (e.g. Herbert & Wu, 2009; Lin, 2017;

Tsai, 2005; Wu, 2015) and the present study in mind, it seems that NESTs not only

motivated students (Gardner, 1985; Dörnyei, 2005), lessened their learning anxiety, but

also improved their listening abilities (Wu, 2015). Thus, the impact NESTs make on the

learning of English for Taiwanese students cannot be neglected.

5.2 Limitations and Suggestions for Future Study

Although the present study has shown findings with a positive effect on students’

listening comprehension with the assistance of NESTs, especially for those with more

than three years of instruction of NESTs, there are limitations that should be considered.

First of all, we cannot ignore other potential factors that might also affect students’

listening comprehension achievement, such as test-taker variables (Farhady, 1982)

including their experience with test types, their weak and strong areas in language skills,

different resources and exposure of English in urban and rural areas (Chang, 2007).

However, since the goal of the present study is to gain a general understanding of the

students’ listening ability under the guidance of NESTs in a co-teaching module, the

large sample size of this study and the statistical analysis of the data might be sufficient

61

to provide a general pattern of students’ listening performance regardless of other

possible influential factors. Second, the main focus of the present study is analyzed

quantitatively. Although statistical data could provide a general picture of how NESTs

help improve students’ listening ability in English, qualitative data, such as interviews,

classroom observations, questionnaires, might give us a more holistic view of the

impact of the NESTs.

Considering the limitations of the present study, suggestions for future research

are addressed as follows. First of all, the listening test could be shortened in order to

rule out the potential attention span issue. For example, the instruction of the tests could

be either in just Chinese or English. Even though the actual testing parts took up around

20 minutes, the participants were still under the pressure of “taking a test” when

listening to the instruction. In order to exclude the potential attention span issue, it is

suggested that we should take as many test-taker variables as possible into

consideration when administering a test. Secondly, qualitative data should be collected

in each of the areas through interviews and classroom observations to explore how

specifically NESTs influence students in terms of improving their English listening

ability. Last but not least, observations on NESTs themselves could be explored

including their gender, characteristics, and their beliefs of education so that more can

62

be understood in terms of the type of NESTs that contribute most to English learning

in Taiwan. In other words, ETAs could be the subject of future study.

63

Reference

Anderson, J. R. (1985). A series of books in psychology. Cognitive psychology and its

implications (2nd ed.). New York, NY, US: W H Freeman/Times Books/ Henry

Holt & Co.

Arnold, J. (2000). Seeing through Listening Comprehension Exam Anxiety. TESOL

Quarterly, 34(4), 777-786

Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of

Communication Disorders, 36(3), pp.189-208.

Barratt, L., & Kontra, E. H. (2000). Native‐English‐speaking teachers in cultures

other than their own. TESOL Journal, 9(3), 19-23.

Benke, E., & Medgyes, P. (2005). Differences in teaching behaviour between native

and non-native speaker teachers: As seen by the learners. In Llurda E. (Ed.),

Non-native language teachers (pp. 195-215). New York: Springer, Boston, MA.

Bensimon, E. M. (2005). Closing the achievement gap in higher education: An

organizational learning perspective. New Directions for Higher Education,

2005(131), 99-111.

Brindley, G. (1998). Assessing listening abilities. Annual Review of Applied

Linguistics, 18, 171-191.

64

Brindley, G., & Slatyer, H. (2002). Exploring task difficulty in ESL listening

assessment. Language Testing, 19(4), 369-394.

Buck, G. (1991). The testing of listening comprehension: An introspective study1.

Language Testing, 8(1), 67-91.

Buck, G. (1995). How to become a good listening teacher. In D. J. Mendelsohn & J.

Rubin (Eds.), A guide for the teaching of second language listening (pp.

113-131). San Diego, CA: Dominie Press.

Buck, G. (1990). The testing of second language listening comprehension.

Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Lancaster, England.

Buck, G. (2001). Assessing listening. England: Cambridge University Press.

Buckley, F. J. (2000). Team teaching: What, why, and how? Thousand Oaks, CA:

SAGE Publications, Inc.

Burstall, C. (1975). French in the primary school: The British experiment. Canadian

Modern Language Review, 31(5), 388-402.

Candlin, C., Abbs, B., & Edelhoff, C. (1982). Challenges: A multi-media project for

learners of English. Langenscheidt.

Carless, D., & Walker, E. (2006). Effective team teaching between local and

native-speaking English teachers. Language and Education, 20(6), 463-477.

65

Carroll, J. B. (1961). Fundamental considerations in testing for English language

proficiency of foreign students. Testing the English Proficiency Of Foreign

Students, 36.

Carroll, B. J. (1980). Testing communicative performance: An interim study. Oxford:

Pergamon Press.

Chang, A., & Read, J. (2006). The effects of listening support on the listening

performance of EFL learners. TESOL Quarterly, 40(2), 375-397.

Chang, A. (2008). Listening strategies of L2 learners with varied test tasks. TESL

Canada Journal, 26(1), 1-26.

Chang, A. (2009). EFL listeners' task-based strategies and their relationship with

listening performance. TESL-EJ, 13(2), n2.

Chapelle, C. A. (1999). Construct definition and validity inquiry in SLA research.

Interfaces between second language acquisition and language testing research

(pp. 32-70). England: Cambridge University Press

Chern, C. L. (2010). An overview of English language education at primary level in

Taiwan. Proceedings of Second Language Studies: Acquisition, Learning,

Education and Technology Conference, Tokyo. Retrieved from

http://www.gavo.t.u-tokyo.ac.jp/L2WS2010/papers/L2WS2010_S-03.pdf

66

Chien, C. N., & Wei, L. (1998). The strategy use in listening comprehension for EFL

learners in Taiwan. RELC Journal, 29(1), 66-91.

Cook, L., & Friend, M. (1995). Co-teaching: Guidelines for creating effective

practices. Focus on Exceptional Children, 28(3), 1-16.

Dörnyei, Z. (2005). The psychology of the language learner: Individual differences in

second language acquisition. London: Routledge.

Dunkel, P. (1991). Listening in the native and second/foreign language: Toward an

integration of research and practice. TESOL Quarterly, 25(3), 431-457.

Elkhafaifi, H. (2005). Listening comprehension and anxiety in the Arabic language

classroom. Modern Language Journal, 89(2), 206-220.

Farhady, H. (1979). The disjunctive fallacy between discrete-point and integrative

tests. TESOL Quarterly 13(3), 347-357.

Farhady, H. (1982). Measures of language proficiency from the learner's perspective.

TESOL Quarterly, 16(1), 43-59.

Ferris, D. (1998). Students' views of academic aural/oral skills: A comparative needs

analysis. TESOL Quarterly, 32(2), 289-316.

Field, J. (2008). Bricks or mortar: Which parts of the input does a second language

listener rely on?. TESOL Quarterly, 42(3), 411-432.