• 沒有找到結果。

Chapter 8 Music-to-Language Mapping: Tone and Markedness . 119

8.6 L I -to-L O Mapping: Type 3b Operation

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

127

(209) LI-to-LO production grammar: faithful output Linguistic input: yin55 shi33 ‘diet’ 飲食 Final output: yin55 shi35

ASSOC-TERM ID-T

 a. yin55 shi35 * *

b. yin33 shi55 * *!*

c. yin35 shi55 * *!*

In tableau (209), the terms are unassociated at either lexical or postlexical level, and the constraint ASSOC-TERM is violated by each of the candidates. The candidate in (209a) wins over the others by one less violation of ID-T.

8.6 L

I

-to-L

O

Mapping: Type 3b Operation

In the type 3b operation, the final output is unfaithful to the linguistic input. The markedness constraints rule out the faithful output and select the output that conforms to surface well-formedness. Three constraints are relevant for this mapping, as in (210-212).

(210) OCP-LOW:

Assign one violation mark for every pairs of low tones.

(211) TONOTACTICS:

Assign one violation mark for every tone such as 33 and 44 that does not exist in Mandarin.

(212)ID-T-R:

Assign one violation mark for every rightmost tone that is not identical to its correspondent in the input.

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

128

OCP-LOW prohibits consecutive low tones, and forces one of the tones to change.

ID-T-R preserves the right tone and allows the left tone to change. The constraint TONOTACTICS bans any tone that does not exist in Mandarin, and must be ranked higher than ID-T. The enriched constraint ranking is posited in (213).

(213) LI-to-LO constraint ranking (enriched):

ASSOC-TERM,TONOTACTICS,OCP-LOW >>ID-T, ID-T-R;

Tableau (214) shows how this constraint works.

(214) LI-to-LO production grammar: unfaithful output

Initial output/Postlexical input: li11 li11 ‘flourish’ 離離 Final output: li35 li11

ASSOC-TERM TONOTACTICS OCP-LOW ID-T ID-T-R

 a. li35 li11 * *

b. li11 li11 * *!

c. li11 li35 * * *!

d. li33 li11 * *! *

The markedness constraints rule out (214b, d) respectively, while (214c) is ruled out by the faithfulness constraint, ID-T-R. As a consequence, (214a) emerges as the optimal output, where li35 li21, is different from the postlexical input, li11 li11.

Another example lies in the phonotactics. For instance, the musical output, mei5

mao5, perceived as mei55 mao55 in the linguistic input, which, in the failure of

lexical term association, is faithfully mapped as the initial output, which in turns serves as the postlexical input. In the failure of postlexical term association, the postlexical input, mei55 mao55, is eventually mei35 mao55, which is an unfaithful output. This unfaithful mapping is attributed to the fact that the syllable-tone

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

129

combination of mei55 is illegal in the Mandarin output. The constraint PHONOTACTICS, as in (215), is thus needed for the following discussions.

(215) PHONOTACTICS:

Assign one violation mark for every illegal syllable-tone combination in the output.

PHONOTACTICS should be undominated, and the partial constraint ranking is expended as (216).

(216) LI-to-LO constraint ranking (enriched):

ASSOC-TERM,TONOTACTICS, PHONOTACTICS,OCP-LOW >>ID-T, ID-T-R

Tableau (217) demonstrates the constraint interaction.

(217) LI-to-LO production grammar:

Initial output/Postlexical input: mei55 mao55 ‘eyebrows’ 眉毛 Final output: mei35 mao55

ASSOC- TERM TONOTACTICS PHONOTACTICS ID-T

 a. mei35 mao55 * *

b. mei55 mao55 * *!

Since the children fail to associate the term to their mental lexicon, the constraint ASSOC-term is violated by both candidates. The syllable-tone combination of mei55 in (217b) violates PHONOTACTICS. (217a) thus surfaces and an unfaithful output is selected.

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

130

8.7 Summary

This chapter has addressed a third type of operation in the music-to-language mapping. The perception grammar that maps MO to LI is the same as that in type 1 and type 2, as discussed in Chapter 7. This third type of operation further consists of two subtypes, type 3a and type 3b, in both of which term association fails at the lexical and postlexical levels. The mapping from the postlexical input to the final output is keyed to the interactions of faithfulness and markedness constraints. I have posited a set of constraints, which are ranked as in (218).

(218) LI-to-LO production grammar: term association

ASSOC-TERM TONOTACTICS OCP-LOW PHONOTACTICS

ID-T ID-T-R

The MO-to-LI perception grammar values the faithfulness relation between the musical pitch and the linguistic tone. The LI-to-LO production grammar requires term association at the lexical and postlexical levels. If term association fails, the final linguistic output will be faithful to the postlexical input; however, when surface markedness constraints are violated, an unfaithful output will emerge.

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

131

Chapter 9

Conclusion and Further Issue

9.1 Conclusion

This study probes into the connection between language and music through the perception and production grammar in English and Mandarin songs. The interaction between language and music is revealed in three aspects, namely, segment, rhythm, and tone.

Chapters 3 and 4 compare the linguistic mapping and the language-to-music mapping in onset and coda clusters through Mandarin accented English. The linguistic mapping in onset clusters and coda clusters involves the interaction between segmental faithfulness and markedness, as well as prosodic alignment, which yields two output variants: the monosyllabic LO1and the disyllabic LO2. At this stage, the SSP is not crucial in either constraint ranking. Consider the Hasse diagrams reproduced in (219).

(219)LI-to-LO mapping in onset and coda clusters (a) LI-to-LO1 mapping: ()

ALIGN-E (LEX, ) DEP-V MAX-C

SSP

*CC

(b) LI-to-LO2 mapping: ()

ALIGN-E(LEX, ) *CC MAX-C

SSP

DEP-V

(220a) exhibits the LO-to-MI perception grammar in onset clusters, while (220b) presents that in coda clusters.

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

132

(220)

(a) LO-to-MI mapping in onset clusters NOSTRAY MAX-C

DEP-V

ALIGN-R(♩, )

NOSHARE-B NOSHARE-σ

(b) LO-to-MI mapping in coda clusters NOSTRAY MAX-C NOSHARE-σ & *CCCODA

DEP-V

SSP

ALIGN-R(♩, )

NOSHARE-B NOSHARE-σ

In (220b), the conjoined constraint, NOSHARE-σ & *CCCODA,is dominant and SSP is observed to affect the insertion of vowels in singing.

The MI-to-MO production grammar in onset and coda clusters requires faithfulness of syllable-beat association and the removal of prosodic structure in the musical output as reproduced in the Hasse diagram in (221).

(221) MI-to-MO production grammar

ID-ASSOC *PROSST

MAX-PROSST

Chapters 5 and 6 examine the language-to-music mapping with the focus on the composing of Mandarin children’s song. In Chapter 5, I have examined how the foot and IP structures affect the assignment of musical beats. The foot is formed based on the syntactic structures, in which immediate constituent (IC) and branching direction

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

133

are keyed to deciding the output. The constraint ranking for foot formation is reproduced in the Hasse diagram in (222).

(222) LI-to-LO production grammar

NOSTRADDLE-FT PARSE-ALIGN-E(IC,FT)*MONO-FT

FTBIN

The LO-to-MI perception grammar of the syllable-to-beat assignment of foot is exhibited in Hasse diagram reproduced in (223).

(223) LO-to-MI perception grammar

NOSTRAY NOSTRADDLE-B(FT) NOSHARE-(B5)

NOSPLIT-IC(B) NOSHARE-B NOSHARE-σ

As in (223) and in (220) proposed previously, both NOSHARE-B and NOSHARE-σ are ranked at the bottom, as a syllable is often linked to multiple musical beats to prevent stray elements and vice versa.

Chapter 6 continues to examine the mapping of rhythmic structures focalizing on the intonational phrase (IP). The IPs in the lyric output are defined by the punctuation marks, which is captured by ALIGN-R(PM, IP). The mapping of the IP to the musical input is crucial to the measure-to-IP alignment and the IP-final musical lengthening.

The LO-to-MI perception grammar is governed by two sets of constraints as reproduced

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

134

in the Hasse diagrams in (224) and (225).

(224) LO-to-MI perception grammar: measure-to-IP alignment ALIGN-R(SONG,IP) ALIGN-L(MN, IP)

ALIGN-R(M2N, IP)

ALIGN-L(IP, MN) ALIGN-R(IP,M2N)

(225) LO-to-MI perception grammar: musical beat assignment NOSTRAY *n-1  n]IP NOSHARE-(B5) NOSPLIT-IC(B)

NOSPLIT-IC(B)

The mapping between the musical input (MI) and the musical output (MO) in Chapter 5-6 is governed by three constraints, ID-ASSOC,*PROSST and MAX-PROSST. As previously posited in Chapter 3-4, the musical output preserves the musical beat association but removes the structure of the prosodic structures.

In Chapter 7-8, the music-to-language mapping is examined through children’s perception of the singing of Mandarin songs. Singing words or phrases are mapped to the linguistic input forms with similar tonal values through the perception grammar.

The perception grammar is accounted by the correspondence of musical pitch, register, and distance in the MO-to-LI mapping, which is governed by a set of constraints reproduced in the Hasse diagram in (226).

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

135

(226) MO-to-LI perception grammar: pitch-tone correspondence ID-REG ID-CTR NoDIST=1 MAX-DIST

NoDIST=4

The children associate what they have heard to their mental lexicon through the LI-to-LO production grammar. Four types of operations are observed. In the type 1 and type 2 operations, term association is achieved at some points and the final outputs are actual words or phrases as governed by the constraint ranking reproduced in the Hasse diagram in (227).

(227) LI-to-LO production grammar: term association ASSOC-TERM TONOTACTICS

ID-T

The top-ranked ASSOC-TERM selects the associated term whose tone may be different from the linguistic input.

In the type 3a and 3b operations, term association fails at both lexical and postlexical level. If term association fails, the final linguistic output will be faithful to the postlexical input; however, when surface markedness constraints are violated, an unfaithful output will emerge. The relevant Hasse diagram is reproduced in (228).

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

136

(228) LI-to-LO production grammar: term association

ASSOC-TERM TONOTACTICS OCP-LOW PHONOTACTICS

ID-T ID-T-R

Taken together, Chapters 3 to 8 demonstrate segmental changes, rhythmic alignment, beat assignment, and linguistic tonal changes in the mapping between language and music. From the perspective of segment and rhythm, the language-to-music mapping shows that the perception grammar plays an important role. Tonal changes in the music-to-language mapping also support that there is an independent perception grammar.

9.2 Further issue

In Chapters 7 and 8, I have examined how disyllabic musical pitches are transformed into linguistic tones. Each syllable is associated with one musical beat with equal musical prominence. This study has also observed the musical pitch to linguistic tone mapping in multisyllabic phrases and sentences, where syllables are mapped to accented (>) or unaccented musical beats as exemplified in (229).

(229) ‘His name is Jesus’

> (musical prominence)

ta5 ming7 jiao6 ye5-su3 (musical pitch)

ta55 ming53 jiao53 ye55 su55 (linguistic tone)

he name call Jesus

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

137

In (229), the linguistic final output, ta55, jiao53, and ye55-su55 are terms that can be associated by the children, whereas, ming53 is an unassociated term. Since ming53 is the unassociated term, its linguistic tonal output is not influenced by lexicon association. However, why is ming7 perceived as ming53 instead of ming55, which is faithful to the singing output? It is observed that some of the children perceive the accented (>) singing output as the linguistic tone, 53. Future studies can examine how the pitches of the accented musical beats are perceived into linguistic tones.

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

138

Bibliography

English References

Bermúdez-Otero, R. (1999). Constraint interaction in language change: quantity in

English and Germanic (doctoral dissertation). University of Manchester, UK.

Bermúdez-Otero, R. (2007). Diachronic phonology. In P. de Lacy (Ed.), The

Cambridge handbook of phonology (pp. 497-517). Cambridge, Mass.: Cambridge

University Press.

Boersma, P. (2001). Phonology-semantics interaction in OT, and its acquisition. In R.

Kirchner, W. Wikeley and J. Pater (Ed.), Papers in experimental and theoretical

linguistics, 6, 24-35. Edmonton: University of Alberta.

Booij, G. E. & Rubach, J. (1987). Postcyclic versus postlexical rules in lexical phonology. Linguistic Inquiry, 18 (1), 1-44.

Broselow, E., and Finer, D. (1991). Parameter setting gin second language phonology and syntax. Second Language Research, 7, 35-59.

Carr, P. (1993). Phonology. New York: St. Martins Press.

Chao, Y. R. (1930). A system of tone letters. Le Maître Phonétique, 45, 24-27.

Chen, M. (1984). Unfolding Latent Principles of Literary Taste: Poetry as a Window onto Language. Tsinghua Journal of Chinese Studies, 16, 203-240.

Downing, B. (1970). Syntactic Structure and Phonological Phrasing in English (doctoral dissertation). University of Texas at Austin, Austin.

Downing, B. (1973). Parenthesization Rules and Obligatory Phrasing. Papers in

Linguistic, 6, 108-128.

Goldsmith, J. A. (1976). Autosegmental Phonology (doctoral dissertation). MIT, Cambridge, Mass.

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

139

Green, T. (1993). The conspiracy of completeness. Paper presented at Rutgers Optimality Workshop1.

Halle, J. & Lerdahl F. (1993). A generative textsetting model. Current Musicology, 55, 3-23.

Hayes, B. (2005). Textsetting as constraint conflict. In J.-L. Aroui and A. Arleo (Eds.),

Towards a typology of poetic form (pp. 43-61). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Halliday, M. A. K. (1967). Intonation and grammar in British English. The Hague:

Mouton.

Hsiao, Y. E. (1991). Syntax, rhythm and tone: a triangular relationship. Taipei: The Crane Publishing.

Hsiao, Y. E. (2006). The metrical structure of Changhua folk verse. Concentric: Studies

in Linguistics, 32 (1), 1-29.

Hsiao, Y. E. (2007). The metrical structure of Taiwanese nursery rhymes: a corpus study.

In D. L. Hoover and S. Lattig (Eds.), Stylistics: prospect & retrospect (pp. 85-106).

Amsterdam: Rodopi.

Hsiao, Y. E. (2015). Tonal chain shifts in Taiwanese: a comparative markedness approach. In Y. E. Hsiao, L.-H. Wee (Eds.), Capturing phonological shades within

and across languages (pp. 142-165). Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars

Publishing.

Inkelas, S. (1989). Prosodic constituency in the lexicon (doctoral dissertation). Stanford University, Stanford.

Ito, J., Kubozono, H., Mester, A. & Tanaka, S. (2019). Kattobase: the linguistic structure of Japanese baseball chants. In K. Hout, A. Mai, A. McCollum, S. Rose, and M. Zaslansky (Eds.), Proceedings of the 2018 Annual Meetings on Phonology.

Tokyo, Japan: Kurosio Publishing.

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

140

Jespersen, O. (1904). Lehrbuch der phonetik. Leipzig: Teubner.

Kager, R. (1999). Optimality theory. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Kenstowicz, M. (2003). The role of perception in loanword phonology. A review of Les emprunts linguistiques d’origine europénne en Fon by Flavien Gbéto Köln:

Rüdiger Köppe Verlag. 2000. Studies in African Linguistics, 32, 95-112.

Kiparsky, P. (1982). From cyclic phonology to lexical phonology. In H. van der Hulst, N. Smith (Eds.), The structure of phonological representations (PartI) (pp. 131-175). Dordrecht: Foris Publications.

Kiparsky, Paul. (2010). Reduplication in stratal OT. In L. Uyechi and L.-H. Wee (Eds.),

Reality exploration and discovery: pattern interaction in language & Life (pp.

125-142.).

Leben, W. R. (1973). Suprasegmental phonology (doctoral dissertation). MIT, Cambridge, Mass.

Lerdahl, F. & Jackendoff, R. (1983). A generative rheory of tonal music. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

Lindblom, B. (1978). Final lengthening in speech and music. In E.Gårding, G. Bruce, and R. Bannert (Eds.), Nordic prosody: papers from a symposium (pp.85-102).

Lund: Department of Linguistics, Lund University

Łubowicz, A. (2002). Derived environment effects in optimality theory. Lingua, 112 (4), 243-280

Łubowicz, A. (2005). Locality of conjunction. In J. D. Alderete, C. Hang and A.

Kochetov (Eds.), Proceedings of the 24th

West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (pp. 254-262). Somerville: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.

McCarthy, J. J. and Prince, A. (1995). Faithfulness and reduplicative identity. In: B. Jill, L. Walsh Dickey, and S. Urbanczyk (Eds.), University of Massachusetts

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

141

Occasional Papers in Linguistics 18 (pp.249-384). Amherst, MA: GLSA

Publications.

McCarthy, J. J. and Prince, A. (1999). Faithfulness and identity in prosodic morphology.

In R. Kager, H. van der Hulst, and W. Zonneveld (Eds.), The Prosody–Morphology

Interface (pp. 218-309). Cambridge, Mass.: Cambridge University Press.

[Excerpted in John J. McCarthy (Ed.) Optimality Theory in Phonology: A Reader, Malden, MA & Oxford: Blackwell (2004).]

Mohanan, K. P. (1982). Lexical Phonology (doctoral dissertation). MIT, Cambridge, Mass.

Morris, R. E. (2002). Coda obstruents and local constraint conjunction in North-Central Peninsular Spanish. In T. Satterfield, C. Tortora and D. Cresti (Eds.), Current

issues in Romance Languages (pp. 207-224). The Netherlands: John Benjamins

B.V.

Nespor, M., and Irene Vo. (1986). Prosodic phonology (doctoral dissertation).

Dordrecht: Foris.

Prince, A., and Smolensky, P. (1993). Optimality theory: constraint interaction in generative grammar. Manuscript. Rutgers University.

Prince, A. and Smolensky, P. (2004). Optimality theory: constraint interaction and

generative grammar. Blackwell Publishing.

Schellenberg, M. (2009). Singing in a tone language: Shona. In A. Ojo and L. Moshi (Eds.), Proceedings of the 39th

Annual Conference on African Linguistics (pp.

137-144). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.

Schreuder, M. (2006). Prosodic Processes in Language and Music (doctoral dissertation). Research School of Behavioral and Cognitive Neurosciences (BCN), Groningen.

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

142

Selkirk, E. (1980). Prosodic domains in phonology: Sanskrit revisited. In M. Aronoff and M.-L. Kean (Eds.), Juncture (pp. 107-129). Saratoga, CA: Anma Libri.

Selkirk, E. (1984). Phonology and syntax: the relation between sound and structure.

Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

Shih, C. L. (1986). The Prosodic Domain of Tone Sandhi in Chinese. San Diego:

University of California dissertation.

Sievers, E. (1881). Grundzüge der Phonetik. Leipzig, Germany: Breitkopf and Hartel.

Silverman, D. (1992). Multiple scansions in loanword phonology: evidence from Cantonese. Phonology, 9, 289-328.

Smolensky, P. (1995) On the internal structure of the constraint component CON of UG. Handout presented in the University of Arizona.

Smolensky, P. (1996). On the comprehension/production dilemma in child language.

Linguistic Inquiry, 27, 720-731.

Sundberg, J and Lindblom, B. (1991). Generative theories for describing musical structure. In P. Howell, R. West, and I. Cross (Eds.), Representing musical

structure (pp. 245-272). London: Academic Press.

Wee, L. H. (2002). Of chains and weights. Handout for seminar series Grammaticality of the Fittest, City University of Hong Kong.

Wee, L. H. (2007). Unraveling the relation between Mandarin tones and musical melody.

Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 35, 128-144.

Wong, P. and Diehl, R. (2002). How can the lyrics of a song in a tone language be understood? Psychology of Music, 30, 202-209.

Yip, M. (1989). Hanyu fangyan gaiyao [An introduction to Chinese dialects]. 2nd edn.

Beijing: Wenzi Gaige Chubanshe.

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

143

Yip, M. (1993). Cantonese loanword phonology and optimality theory. Journal of East

Asian Linguistics, 1 (1), 261-291.

Yip, M. (2007). Tone. In P. de Lacy (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of phonology (pp.

229-252). Cambridge, Mass.: Cambridge University Press.

Chinese References

方金雅、蘇姿云 (Fang and Su 2005)。童謠教學對幼兒聲韻覺識影響之研究。高 雄師大學報:教育與社會科學類,19,1-19。

李金青 (Li 2009)。兒歌的韻律研究 (碩士論文),國立臺東大學,臺東。

李紫蓉、楊麗鈴 (2007)。手指遊戲動動兒歌-小猴子。臺北:信誼基金出版社。

李紫蓉、朱雲嵩、林炳興、張硯田、蘇珊 (2011)。早安晚安:給 0~3 歲的自

理兒歌。臺北:信誼基金出版社。

李紫蓉、鄭榮珍、張麗雪、蘇珊、張硯田、陳中申 (2014)。小雨滴。臺北:

信誼基金出版社。

李紫蓉、林世仁、蘇珊 (2017)。1234 動動體操。臺北:信誼基金出版社。

宋凱琳 (Sung 2012)。從優選理論看語言與音樂的互動:從詩經入樂談起。華語 文教學研究

9 (4),17-39。

林清山 (Lin 1968)。男女學前兒童語言發展各變數的比較研究。心理與教育,2,

121-137。

林蕙珊 (Lin 2001)。從「優選理論」來談國語的三聲變調。聲韻論叢,10,497-524。

黃婷 (Huang 2007)。華語手指謠之韻律研究。華語文教學研究

4 (2),29-42。

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

144

劉惠美、曹峰銘 (Liu and Tsao 2010)。華語嬰幼兒溝通發展量表。臺北:

心理。

劉惠美、陳昱君(Liu and Chen 2015)。華語嬰幼兒表達性詞彙的語意內容及

詞類組成之發展。教育心理學報,47 (2),217-242。

2020 Ph.D. in Linguistics, National Chengchi University 2010 M.A. in Linguistics, National Chengchi University 2007 B.A. in Chinese, National Taiwan Normal University

Journal Paper

Ling, Wang-Chen. 2015. Circular tonal chain shifts in Jiaoxian compound words.

Language and Linguistics (語言暨語言學). 16.6. [SSCI & AHCI].

Conference Papers

Ling, Wang-Chen. 2018. The Output-to-output Tonal Correspondence in Language and Music. International Conference on Tone and Intonation (TIE2018).

Ling, Wang-Chen. 2018. The Competition between Contour and Register Correspondence in Music-to-Language Perception: Evidence from Mandarin Child Songs. The 51st International Conference on Sino-Tibetan Languages and Linguistics (ICSTLL51)

Ling, Wang-Chen. 2018. Child perception of Mandarin Songs: An Optimality Theory Analysis. The 7the International Conference on Phonology and Morphology.

Ling, Wang-Chen. 2017. The Conformity of Language and Melody in Mandarin Children’s Song. 37th Annual Conference of the Poetics And Linguistics Association. West Chester University, Pennsylvania, USA.

Ling, Wang-Chen. 2016. Text-setting of Mandarin tone in children’s song: a corpus analysis. The 28th annual North American Conference on Chinese Linguistics (NACCL-28). Brigham Young University, Utah, USA.

Ling, Wang-Chen. 2015. Circular Tonal Chain Shifts in Leling Pre-light Syllable Positions, National Conference on Linguistics (NCL 2014). National Chengchi University, Taipei.

Ling, Wang-Chen. 2014. Mandarin clipped words of government committees in Taiwan-An OT analysis. The 22nd Annual Conference of the IACL & the 26th North American Conference on Chinese Linguistics (IACL-22 & NACCL-26).

University of Maryland, Maryland, USA.

International Theoretical Phonology Conference (TPC-4). National Chengchi University, Taipei.

Ling, Wang-Chen. 2012. A corpus-based study on the semantic relatedness of the Mandarin polysemous word xia1, blind. NCL2012. Southern Taiwan University, Tainan.

Ling, Wang-Chen. 2012. The disyllabic tone sandhi in Rizhao. The 20th Annual Conference of the International Association of Chinese Linguistics (IACL-20).

The Hong Kong Polytechnic University, Hong Kong.

Ling, Wang-Chen. 2009. The phonological-musical interaction in Chinese hymns: an OT analysis. Mao Kong Forum—The First International Graduate Student Conference on Modern Phonology. National Chengchi University, Taipei.

Ling, Wang-Chen. 2009. Textsetting strategies in Mandarin hymns. The 9th International Conference on Teaching of Chinese Language. Taipei

Ling, Wang-Chen. 2009. The phonological-musical strategies in textsetting of Chinese Bible verses: a corpus-based analysis. The 42nd International Conference on Sino-Tibetan Languages and Linguistics. Payap University, Chiang Mai, Thailand.

Ling, Wang-Chen. 2009. The Mandarin (reflexive) pronoun of SELF (zi4ji3) in bible:

a corpus-based study. The Fifth Corpus Linguistics Conference. Liverpool University. Liverpool, England.

Ling, Wang-Chen. 2009. Lyric modifications in Mandarin hymns as a case of music template satisfaction. The 2009 Graduate Student Workshop on Phonology.

National Chengchi University, Taipei.

Ling, Wang-Chen & Wang, Li-Ting. 2008. Mandarin-Accented English: A Syllable Template Perspective. 2008 Student Workshop on East Asian Linguistics.

National Tsinghua University, Hsinchu, Taiwan.

Grants and Scholarships

2020 Member of the Phi Tau Phi Scholastic Honor Society of the Republic of China 2019 Graduate student scholarship (Ph.D. program)

2018-2020 Research Assistantship of Ministry of Science and Technology (MOST107-2410-H-004-115-MY2)

2018 Teaching Assistantship of Taiwan Southern Min and Taiwan Culture, NCCU

2018 College of Foreign Languages & Literature, NCCU Travel Grant for attending the 7th International Conference on Phonology and Morphology (ICPM7)

2017 Teaching Assistantship of Language and World Civilization, NCCU

2017 Ministry of Science and Technology (MOST) Travel Grant for attending the 37th Annual Conference of the Poetics And Linguistics Association

2016-2018 Research Assistantship of MOST (MOST 105-2410-H-004-181-MY2) 2016 MOST Travel Grant for attending the 28th North American Conference on

Chinese Linguistics

2014-2016 Research Assistantship of MOST (MOST 103-2410-H-004-086-MY2) 2014 MOST Travel Grant for attending the 22nd Annual Conference of the IACL & the

2014-2016 Research Assistantship of MOST (MOST 103-2410-H-004-086-MY2) 2014 MOST Travel Grant for attending the 22nd Annual Conference of the IACL & the