• 沒有找到結果。

Visualized Representation of Visual Search Pattern for a Visuospatial Attention Test

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Visualized Representation of Visual Search Pattern for a Visuospatial Attention Test"

Copied!
4
0
0

加載中.... (立即查看全文)

全文

(1)

Visual search is a common scanning procedure that people use to locate objects in real life. During a typical experiment with the visual search task, a participant is re-quired to search for a specific target in a visual scene; the number of targets and nontargets (distractors) or feature dimensions (e.g., colors) may be manipulated to evaluate reaction times and performance on the visual search task. Visual search is often affected by neurological deficits and, therefore, requires clinical attention for problem identifi-cation and further management of symptoms. However, most lab instruments for visual search studies may be in-appropriate for clinical evaluation. Therefore, we modi-fied a popular assessment instrument, the cancellation task, from a paper-and-pencil test to a computer-based test (Wang, Huang, & Huang, 2006) for education and clinical administration. To better express the spatiotempo-ral process during the assessment, we introduced a pattern identification mechanism for visualized representation of visual search patterns.

Previous studies have indicated that the features shared by the target and the distractors, as well as the number of search items, will affect reaction times and the perfor-mance of visual search (Hogeboom & van Leeuwen, 1997; Müller & Found, 1996; Scharroo, Stalmeier, & Boselie, 1994; Treisman, 1991; Wolfe, 2001). Treisman and Gelade (1980) proposed the feature integration theory, in which two types of visual search tasks are distinguished: feature search and conjunction search. Feature search, in which a single feature, such as size, color, orientation, motion, cur-vature, or depth, solely defines the target, can be executed

easily. Feature search is performed in parallel in the pre-attentive stage, and reaction time will not be affected by the number of distractors. In contrast, conjunction search is a more complex task requiring the examination of tar-gets and nontartar-gets that share the same features. As the number of distractors increases, participants require more time to complete the conjunction search task.

Visual search tasks have been used to study the deploy-ment of visual attention in a visual scene (Bundesen, 1990; Bundesen, Habekost, & Kyllingsbaek, 2005; Johnson & Proctor, 2004). An individual’s intention controls the al-location of attention and guides the attentional focus and the processing of selected information consecutively. Pos-ner and Rafal (1987) reported that visual-scanning deficits can disrupt visual attention. Shifts in visual attention (i.e., the orienting process) are usually accompanied by motor behavior (overt movements of the head and eyes to align the fovea with the spatial locus of the target) and can be ob-served by means of reaction time for motor output. There-fore, visual attention can be observed through people’s motor behavior when they perform a visuospatial task.

In the studies of visuospatial attention, an individual has to search for a specific target in a search scene, which involves the spatial processing with the temporal compo-nent of the task (Robinson & Kertzman, 1990; Snowden, Willey, & Muir, 2001). When an individual searches for a target scattered in a visual scene but fails to point out the target and its location, it is likely that the person has deficits in visuospatial attention. The cancellation test is one of the most popular tools for assessing visuospatial

383 Copyright 2008 Psychonomic Society, Inc.

Visualized representation of visual search

patterns for a visuospatial attention test

HO-CHUAN HUANG

National Kaohsiung University of Applied Sciences, Taiwan

AND TSUI-YING WANG

National Cheng Kung University, Taiwan

Cancellation tests have been widely used in clinical practice and in research to evaluate visuospatial attention, visual scanning patterns, and neglect problems. The aim of the present work is to present a visualized interface for the visuospatial attentional assessment system that can be employed to monitor and analyze attention per-formance and the search strategies used during visuospatial processing of target cancellation. We introduce a pattern identification mechanism for visual search patterns and report our findings from examining the visual search performance and patterns. We also present a comparison of results across various cancellation tests and age groups. The present study demonstrates that our system can obtain more processing data about spatiotem-poral features of visual search than can conventional tests.

Behavior Research Methods 2008, 40 (2), 383-390

doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.2.383

(2)

384 HUANG AND WANG

but also the cancellation process. However, the system did not support a visualized interface for analyzing the visual search path and attentional center, which could increase our understanding of attentional deployment.

In the present article, we present a new visuospatial at-tentional assessment system for assessing and obtaining more detailed information of attentional processes and performance. We use the cancellation test to assess atten-tion performance, the efficiency of attenatten-tional shifts, and the strategies of visuospatial search in a visual scene. A visualized tool is introduced that presents the geographi-cal location of the attentional center for visuospatial atten-tional processes. In addition, a pattern identification algo-rithm is proposed for automatically identifying the type of visual search pattern. With the assistance of computerized tools, we can understand individual discrepancies in the process and strategy of visuospatial search better.

Visual Search Pattern and Attentional Center During a visual search task with more than one target, all the stimuli marked by the participant can be linked quentially to form a visual scanpath. A scanpath is the se-rial deployment or shifts of the attentional spotlight across displayed visual information mapped in iconic memory (Posner & Peterson, 1990). We define a scanpath as “the total of the cancellation sequences (i.e., links of marked stimuli) based on the temporal order of stimuli marked during the visual search process.” In other words, a scan-path shows the attentional shifts for stimuli marked from one location to the next across the visual scene. With the assistance of the CACTS scanpath analysis, we were able to monitor the attentional shifts of participants in order to understand their deployment of visuospatial attention.

A visual scanpath can be as simple as a straight line or as complex as a visual search graph. We classified a visual search graph into three search pattern types: a hori-zontal, a vertical, and a mixed search pattern. To avoid trivial processes in identifying search patterns by human-made detection, we established a pattern identification algorithm with an evaluation measure, the search pattern index, for automatically identifying the visual search tern (Figure 1). Two expressions relevant to the search pat-tern index are

line(i j ) 



Xj Xi

2



Yj Yi

2 (1) and

slope(ij)  | Sy/Sx|, (2)

where Sx  (Xj  Xi) and Sy  (Yj  Yi); (Xi, Yi) and

(Xj, Yj) are the coordinates of the stimuli i and j,

respec-tively; line(ij) is the distance between two marked stim-uli i and j; slope(ij) is the slope of the straight line (i.e., line(ij)) and is used to identify the type of the straight line (horizontal or vertical). Two variables, H-sum and V-sum, are used to calculate the total length of paths for horizontal and vertical lines, respectively. If the value of slope(ij) is less than 1 or the value of Sy is zero, the line will be

horizontal, and the length of line segment line(ij) will be accumulated into the H-sum variable. On the other hand, if the value of slope(ij) is larger than 1 or the value of Sx

attention (Byrd, Touradji, Tang, & Manly, 2004; Lowery, Ragland, Gur, Gur, & Moberg, 2004). Traditional paper-and-pencil cancellation tests are used for clinical settings and research to evaluate visuospatial attention, visual-scanning patterns, and neglect problems (Halligan, Burn, Marshall, & Wade, 1992; Weintraub & Mesulam, 1985). The formats of cancellation tests vary widely in terms of complexity, with respect to stimulus shapes, set sizes, and array layouts (Friedman, 1992; Halligan et al., 1992; Wil-son, Cockburn, & Halligan, 1987).

Despite the extensive application of cancellation tasks in research, there are relatively few reports on the spatial aspects of cancellation performance in healthy popula-tions. Some studies have examined error distributions for the left and right halves of cancellation forms, find-ing no significant difference for small samples of healthy adults (Gauthier, Dehaut, & Joanette, 1989; Weintraub & Mesulam, 1987, 1988). Geldmacher and colleagues stud-ied verbal and nonverbal cancellation performance in a large sample of healthy adults and reported conflicting findings for visuospatial attention bias. They suggested that the reading habit may be an interacting factor to be considered for the directional bias (Geldmacher & Alhaj, 1999; Geldmacher, Doty, & Heilman, 1994).

In other experiments, cancellation tests have been used as a tool for understanding visual exploratory performance during the target-searching process. It has been assumed that the extent of the disorganization of cancellations may relate to visual inattention. Healthy participants usually cancel targets in organized patterns, generally horizontal (e.g., left to right) or vertical (e.g., top down) (Donnelly et al., 1999; Gauthier et al., 1989; Weintraub & Mesulam, 1988). How-ever, Mark, Woods, Ball, Roth, and Mennemeier (2004) reported no significant correlations between omissions and search organization variables for cancellation performance. They used a small camera to record the test performances of 18 stroke patients and conducted a frame-by-frame analysis on the searching path, with marked stimuli identified by Cartesian (x, y) coordinates. Difficulties arise when one at-tempts to replicate the study with a large sample through such an all-by-hand method. We suggest that further exami-nation of the topography of target searching is necessary to rule out visual attention problems and that an automated procedure is required.

Computerized technologies have been widely used in different assessment tools. Both Donnelly et al. (1999) and Potter et al. (2000) used computers to record and analyze spatiotemporal performance, such as premovement time (initial movement), movement time (time between two successively marked stimuli), drawing time (completing a cancellation), and pause time (the preparatory process). These computer- assisted assessment tools more sensitively measured visuospatial attention performance than did tra-ditional assessment tools. Wang et al. (2006) constructed the computer- assisted cancellation test system (CACTS) to investigate the performance of visuospatial attention in a population of schoolchildren. We used CACTS to analyze visual attention performance in terms of visual search pat-tern, moving time, pause time, and performance, which allowed us to observe not only the correct/error responses,

(3)

VISUOSPATIAL ATTENTION REPRESENTATION 389 1999) and intrinsic motor behavior (Schwartz, Adair, Na, Williamson, & Heilman, 1997) may have an interaction effect on the left-advantage phenomenon. Further inves-tigations that consist of samples with a wider educational background, various neurological conditions, and a com-bination of other assessments with CACTS would provide more evidence for understanding the visuospatial perfor-mance of an individual.

CONCLUSION

Many traditional assessment tools are being replaced by computer-assisted assessments because they are time-saving, less biased, more precise, and more efficient to process and because they have more storage capacity. In the present study, we found that a computerized assess-ment system obtained more detailed information than a traditional paper-and-pencil assessment did about an indi-vidual’s attentional processes and performance. A visual-ized tool was presented to locate the attentional center and monitor the visual scanpath in order to understand shifts in visual attention.

We found that the CACTS offered preliminary data about visuospatial attention and a visual scanpath for the target search task, which may have implications for the understanding of visual reading patterns on a screen. We also found that the visuospatial attention performance of the participants was as good as was expected.

AUTHOR NOTE

This work was supported in part by Grant NSC93-2614-S-151-001 from the National Science Council (NSC), Taiwan. We thank the NSC for their financial support and all the research members involved in this project. We also thank the reviewers for the valuable comments and suggestions. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to T.-Y. Wang, Department of Occupational Therapy, National Cheng Kung University, 1 University Road, Tainan 701, Taiwan (e-mail: michwang@mail.ncku.edu.tw).

REFERENCES

Bundesen, C. (1990). A theory of visual attention. Psychological

Re-view, 97, 523-547.

Bundesen, C., Habekost, T., & Kyllingsbaek, S. (2005). A neural theory of visual attention: Bridging cognition and neurophysiology.

Psychological Review, 112, 291-328.

Byrd, D. A., Touradji, P., Tang, M. X., & Manly, J. J. (2004). Can-cellation test performance in African American, Hispanic, and White elderly. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 401-411.

Chokron, S., & Imbert, M. (1993). Influence of reading habits on line bisection. Cognitive Brain Research, 1, 219-222.

Curran, T., Hills, A., Patterson, M. B., & Strauss, M. E. (2001). Effects of aging on visuospatial attention: An ERP study.

Neuropsy-chologia, 39, 288-301.

Dawes, S., & Senior, G. (2001, October-November). Australian

nor-mative data and clinical utility of the Mesulam and Weintraub Can-cellation Test. Paper presented at the 21st Annual Conference of the

National Academy of Neuropsychology, San Francisco.

Dawson, D. R., & Tanner-Cohen, C. (1997). Visual scanning patterns in an adult Chinese population: Preliminary normative data.

Occupa-tional Therapy Journal of Research, 17, 264-279.

Donnelly, N., Guest, R., Fairhurst, M., Potter, J., Deighton, A., & Patel, M. (1999). Developing algorithms to enhance the sensitiv-ity of cancellation tests of visuospatial neglect. Behavior Research

Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 31, 668-673.

Woodward (1972) predicted that visual search patterns would change because, he suggested, the distal/proximal factor would affect not only the total time of the visual search, but also the orientation of the searching behav-ior. In the present study, the distances between lines and columns were constant in the structured array but varied significantly in the random array. Given that the distance between two stimuli influences the visual search orien-tation, an individual tends to search for the closest next target. This explains why some participants in this study changed their search patterns between the two layouts.

Wang, Huang, and Yang (2004) reported that a comput-erized version of the cancellation test took longer than the paper-and-pencil version to complete and was more diffi-cult (i.e., the error rate was higher) for a population 50 years of age and older. This agrees with the comparison of our computerized version findings with those for the paper-and-pencil-version in Dawes and Senior (2001). Previous studies (Dawes & Senior, 2001; Lowery et al., 2004) have shown no association between gender and completion time or performance, which is consistent with our CACTS re-sults. Researchers also have suggested that age is related to completion time on a cancellation task. When this study is compared with our previous studies on schoolchildren (Wang et al., 2006) and the elderly (Wang et al., 2004), the completion times are significantly different ( p .001), ranking the lowest in college students (n  149; Mstructured  85.55 sec, SD  32.25; Mrandom  81.52 sec, SD  33.21) and then the elderly (n  33; Mstructured  132.18 sec, SD  79.99; Mrandom  127.88 sec, SD  63.72) and the highest in schoolchildren (n  82; Mstructured  187.93 sec, SD  79.74; Mrandom  158.90 sec, SD  62.7). We found that completion time was associated with age-related processing speed. Information-processing speed increases as a child matures, reaching its apex in early adulthood, and gradually declines with age (Kail & Bisanz, 1982; Salthouse & Kail, 1983). Previous studies on visual attention have suggested that the process of attentional shifts and cognitive attention toward various attention tasks in visual performance may be affected by age differences (Curran, Hills, Patterson, & Strauss, 2001; Geldmacher & Riedel, 1999; Kail, 1991; Scialfa, Jenkins, Hamaluk, & Skaloud, 2000).

We found that most participants started their spatial search from the left side and used a left-to-right search pattern on both arrays. The participants made fewer er-rors on the left side of the screen than on the right, which implied a left-side advantage, findings consistent with those in previous studies on a Chinese population (Daw-son & Tanner-Cohen, 1997; Wang et al., 2006). Our re-sults also supported the findings of several related studies using diverse methods to demonstrate a right-hemisphere dominance in visual search performance (Geldmacher et al., 1994; Mesulam, 1985; Vingiano, 1991; Weintraub & Mesulam, 1988). Vingiano found that nonverbal stimuli were associated with right-side inattention, and with a left-ward rather than a rightleft-ward performance bias on timed cancellation tasks in English-reading college students. Other studies also have suggested that the reading direc-tions of some languages (e.g., Hebrew and Arabic lan-guages; Chokron & Imbert, 1993; Geldmacher & Alhaj,

(4)

390 HUANG AND WANG

Potter, J., Deighton, T., Patel, M., Fairhurst, M., Guest, R., & Donnelly, N. (2000). Computer recording of standard tests of visual neglect in stroke patients. Clinical Rehabilitation, 14, 441-446. Robinson, D. L., & Kertzman, C. (1990). Visuospatial attention:

Ef-fects of age, gender, and spatial reference. Neuropsychologia, 28, 291-301.

Salthouse, T. A., & Kail, R. (1983). Memory development through the lifespan: The role of processing rate. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 5, pp. 89-116). New York: Academic Press.

Scharroo, J., Stalmeier, P. F., & Boselie, F. (1994). Visual search and segregation as a function of display complexity. Journal of General

Psychology, 121, 5-17.

Schwartz, R. L., Adair, J. C., Na, D., Williamson, D. J., & Heilman, K. M. (1997). Spatial bias: Attentional and intentional influence in normal subjects. Neurology, 48, 234-242.

Scialfa, C. T., Jenkins, L., Hamaluk, E., & Skaloud, P. (2000). Aging and the development of automaticity in conjunction search.

Journals of Gerontology, 55, P27-P46.

Snowden, R. J., Willey, J., & Muir, J. L. (2001). Visuospatial atten-tion: The role of target contrast and task difficulty when assessing the effects of cues. Perception, 30, 983-991.

Treisman, A. M. (1991). Search, similarity, and integration of features between and within dimensions. Journal of Experimental Psychology:

Human Perception & Performance, 17, 652-676.

Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97-136.

Vingiano, W. (1991). Pseudoneglect on a cancellation task.

Interna-tional Journal of Neuroscience, 58, 63-67.

Wang, T. Y., Huang, H. C., & Huang, H. S. (2006). Design and imple-mentation of cancellation tasks for visual search strategies and visual attention in schoolchildren. Computers & Education, 47, 1-16. Wang, T. Y., Huang, H. C., & Yang, H. C. (2004, December). Visual

at-tention performance on computerized and paper–pencil cancellation test measures. Paper presented at the Biomedical Engineering Society

Annual Symposium, Tainan, Taiwan.

Weintraub, S., & Mesulam, M. M. (1985). Mental state assessment of young and elderly adults in behavioral neurology. In M. M. Mesulam (Ed.), Principles of behavioral neurology (pp. 71-123). Philadelphia: Davis.

Weintraub, S., & Mesulam, M. M. (1987). Right cerebral dominance in spatial attention: Further evidence based on ipsilateral neglect.

Ar-chives of Neurology, 44, 621-625.

Weintraub, S., & Mesulam, M. M. (1988). Visual hemispatial inat-tention: Stimulus parameters and exploratory strategies. Journal of

Neurology, Neurosurgery, & Psychiatry, 51, 1481-1488.

Wilson, B., Cockburn, J., & Halligan, P. (1987). Development of a behavioral test of visuospatial neglect. Archives of Physical Medicine

& Rehabilitation, 68, 98-102.

Wolfe, J. M. (2001). Asymmetries in visual search: An introduction.

Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 381-389.

Woodward, R. M. (1972). Proximity and direction of arrangement in numeric displays. Human Factors, 14, 337-343.

(Manuscript received March 29, 2007; revision accepted for publication September 11, 2007.) Friedman, P. J. (1992). The star cancellation test in acute stroke.

Clini-cal Rehabilitation, 6, 23-30.

Gauthier, L., Dehaut, F., & Joanette, Y. (1989). The bells test: A quantitative and qualitative test for visual neglect. International

Jour-nal of Clinical Neurosychology, 11, 49-54.

Geldmacher, D. S., & Alhaj, M. (1999). Spatial aspects of letter cancellation performance in Arabic readers. International Journal of

Neuroscience, 97, 29-39.

Geldmacher, D. S., Doty, L., & Heilman, K. M. (1994). Spatial performance bias in healthy elderly subjects on a letter cancellation task. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, & Behavioral Neurology,

7, 275-280.

Geldmacher, D. S., & Riedel, T. M. (1999). Age effects on random-array letter cancellation tests. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, &

Behavioral Neurology, 12, 28-34.

Halligan, P. W., Burn, J. P., Marshall, J. C., & Wade, D. T. (1992). Visuo-spatial neglect: Qualitative differences and laterality of cere-bral lesion. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, & Psychiatry, 55, 1060-1068.

Hills, E. C., & Geldmacher, D. S. (1998). The effect of character and array type on visual spatial search quality following traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 12, 69-76.

Hogeboom, M., & van Leeuwen, C. (1997). Visual search strategy and perceptual organization covary with individual preference and structural complexity. Acta Psychologica, 95, 141-164.

Huang, H. C., & Wang, T. Y. (2005, July). Toward a graphical analysis

tool for computer-assisted assessment of visual search patterns. Paper

presented at the Fifth IEEE International Conference on Advanced Learning Technologies, Kaohsiung, Taiwan.

Johnson, A., & Proctor, R. W. (2004). Attention: Theory and practice. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Kail, R. (1991). Developmental change in speed of processing during childhood and adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 490-501. Kail, R., & Bisanz, J. (1982). Information processing and cognitive

development. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development

and behavior (pp. 45-81). New York: Academic Press.

Lowery, N., Ragland, D., Gur, R. C., Gur, R. E., & Moberg, P. J. (2004). Normative data for the symbol cancellation test in young healthy adults. Applied Neuropsychology, 11, 216-219.

Mark, V. W., & Monson, N. (1997). Two-dimensional cancellation ne-glect: A review and suggested method of analysis. Cortex, 33, 553-562. Mark, V. W., Woods, A. J., Ball, K. K., Roth, D. L., & Mennemeier, M.

(2004). Disorganized search on cancellation is not a consequence of neglect. Neurology, 63, 78-84.

Mesulam, M. M. (1985). Attention, confusional states, and neglect. In M. M. Mesulam (Ed.), Principles of behavioral neurology (pp. 125-168). Philadelphia: Davis.

Müller, H. J., & Found, A. (1996). Visual search for conjunctions of motion and form: Display density and asymmetry reversal. Journal

of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 22,

122-132.

Posner, M. I., & Peterson, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25-42.

Posner, M. I., & Rafal, R. D. (1987). Cognitive theories of attention and the rehabilitation of attentional deficits. In M. Meier, A. Benton, & L. Diller (Eds.), Neuropsychological rehabilitation (pp. 182-201). New York: Guilford.

參考文獻

相關文件

• A language in ZPP has two Monte Carlo algorithms, one with no false positives and the other with no

This paper presents (i) a review of item selection algorithms from Robbins–Monro to Fred Lord; (ii) the establishment of a large sample foundation for Fred Lord’s maximum

Then, we tested the influence of θ for the rate of convergence of Algorithm 4.1, by using this algorithm with α = 15 and four different θ to solve a test ex- ample generated as

which can be used (i) to test specific assumptions about the distribution of speed and accuracy in a population of test takers and (ii) to iteratively build a structural

By correcting for the speed of individual test takers, it is possible to reveal systematic differences between the items in a test, which were modeled by item discrimination and

We explicitly saw the dimensional reason for the occurrence of the magnetic catalysis on the basis of the scaling argument. However, the precise form of gap depends

Miroslav Fiedler, Praha, Algebraic connectivity of graphs, Czechoslovak Mathematical Journal 23 (98) 1973,

Otherwise, if a principle of conduct passes only the universal test but fails to pass this test, then it is an “imperfect duty.” For example, the principle “takes care of