• 沒有找到結果。

Our subjects were third-year senior high students. They were at the low-intermediate proficiency level based on their performance on the entrance exam.

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share " Our subjects were third-year senior high students. They were at the low-intermediate proficiency level based on their performance on the entrance exam. "

Copied!
24
0
0

加載中.... (立即查看全文)

全文

(1)

CHAPTER THREE RESEARCH DESIGN AND RESULTS

This chapter introduces the research design and then reports the results. The information about the subjects and their listening learning background will be presented in section 3.1. The instruments will be introduced in section 3.2. The procedures including the pilot study, formal testing, data analysis and scoring will be described in section 3.3. The results are reported in section 3.4. Finally, a summary of this chapter will be given in section 3.5.

3.1 Subjects

Our subjects were third-year senior high students. They were at the low-intermediate proficiency level based on their performance on the entrance exam.

To find out the effective and less effective listeners in the study, 106 third-year senior high students from the three classes (68 males and 38 females) taught by the researcher were asked to take a listening comprehension test (LCT) (see Appendix C for the subjects’ LCT performance)

1

. Based on their performance on the LCT, the subjects whose scores ranked at the top 30% (similar to the study of Wang, 2002) of all were grouped as the “HP” (i.e., high proficiency) group. Their scores ranged from 68 to 90. Those who ranked at the bottom 30% of all were considered the “LP” (i.e., low proficiency) group. Their scores were from 18-50. Finally, 65 students (46 males, 19 females) were selected to participate in the present study, including 32 students in the HP group and 33 students in the LP group.

The following personal information of the subjects, as shown in Table 3-1, was obtained through the Personal Information Sheet (see Appendix D):

1

The mean of the LCT of the 106 students was 59.26. The highest score was 90 and the lowest score

was 18.

(2)

Table 3-1 Subjects’ Background Information

Group HP LP

Gender M:19 F:13 M:27 F:6

English-learning starting age

10.66 11.70

Option n percentage Option n percentage

Yes 2 6% Yes 0 0%

Experience of living in English-speaking

countries

No 30 94% No 33 100%

Very good 0 0% Very good 0 0%

Good 2 6% Good 0 0%

So so 24 75% So so 7 21%

Poor 6 19% Poor 16 49%

Self-rating of listening ability

Very poor 0 0% Very poor 10 30%

agree 23 72% agree 9 27%

Opinions about incorporating listening tests into

exams

disagree 9 28% disagree 24 73%

General English

32 100% General

English

33 100%

Listening training courses taken

before Listening and Conversation

10 32% Listening

and Conversation

7 21%

(3)

Applied Foreign Language

27 84% Applied

Foreign Language

16 48%

Others 2 6% Others 2 6%

General English

10 31% General

English

15 45%

Listening and Conversation

4 13% Listening

and Conversation

5 15%

Applied Foreign Language

22 69% Applied

Foreign Language

15 45%

Useful listening training courses

Others 2 6% Others 1 3%

As shown in Table 3-1, the HP subjects started learning English one year earlier than

the LP group. Two subjects of the HP group had the experience living in

English-speaking countries, while none of the LP group reported this experience. As

for their self-rating of the listening ability, 94% of the HP group and all the subjects of

the LP group considered their listening ability “so so” or worse, showing that the

subjects were generally not confident in their listening. Interestingly, most of the HP

group (72%) strongly agreed to incorporate listening tests into the college entrance

examination while most of the LP group (73%) disagreed. Moreover, besides General

English, most of the HP group (84%) took Applied Foreign Language and a high

percentage of them (22 out of 27) agreed its usefulness. However, only half of the LP

group (48%) took Applied Foreign Language but the majority of them (15 out of 16)

reported that it was a useful course.

(4)

3.2 Instruments

Two instruments were used in the study (i.e., a listening comprehension test and a questionnaire). I will describe them in detail in the following sections.

3.2.1 Listening Comprehension Test

To answer the five research questions stated in Chapter One, a listening comprehension test chosen from the intermediate Listening Comprehension Test (LCT) of the General English Proficiency Test (GEPT) was used. The test was published in 2001 by the Language Training & Testing Center.

The test consisted of three parts and each part had 15 questions, as shown in Table 3-2:

Table 3-2 Structure of the LCT

Part Test Format Text Type Question Number

A picture Statement Qs 1~15

B non-picture Statement Qs 16~30

C non-picture Dialogue Qs 31~45

That is to say, the subjects needed to answer 45 questions in total. All the

questions were in the multiple choice format. The subjects had to choose one best

answer out of four options. The tape was played only once. In Part A, the subjects had

to choose best answers according to the pictures given in the test. For each picture in

Part A, the subjects would need to answer one to three questions, as shown in Table

3-3:

(5)

Table 3-3 An Example of Part A (Look)

(Listen) Look at the picture. Who is taller?

(Listen) A. Julie is taller than Tom.

B. Tom is taller than Julie.

C. Jane is taller than Tom.

D. Julie is taller than Jane.

The best answer is B. Please fill in the correct blanks in black in the answer sheet.

In Part B, the subjects needed to respond to single sentences. Each question in Part B was either an interrogative question or a statement, as in Table 3-4:

Table 3-4 An Example of Part B (Listen) Where’s your brother now?

(Read) A. He’s 20 years old.

B. He’s a doctor.

C. He’s thirsty.

D. He’s in New York.

The best answer is D. Please fill in the correct blanks in black in the answer sheet.

In Part C, the subjects had to answer questions related to short dialogues, as in Table 3-5:

Table 3-5 An Example of Part C

(Listen) (Man) How do you go to school every day?

(Woman) Usually on foot. Sometimes by bus.

Question: How does the woman go to school?

(Read) A. She always walks to school.

B. She usually takes a bus.

(6)

C. She either walks or takes a bus.

D. She usually goes on foot, never by bus.

The best answer is C. Please fill in the correct blanks in black in the answer sheet.

In Part C, each dialogue was read by a male and a female speaker. After listening to the dialogue, a related question would be asked. Then listeners would have to choose the best answer to it.

3.2.2 Questionnaire

To answer the third, fourth, and fifth research questions, a questionnaire (see Appendix E) used to investigate the subjects’ use of listening strategy was designed according to the previous studies (Lee, 1997; Oxford, 1992/1993; Teng, 1996; Wang, 2002). There were 24 items in the Likert-scale format and one open-ended question.

Each of the 24 items represented one substrategy. The open-ended question aimed to elicit the subjects’ other strategies. Altogether six strategies were chosen from two previous studies (Teng, 1996; Wang, 2002) and some were later modified by the researcher. To be specific, the six strategies belonged to two general categories: direct and indirect strategies, as can be seen in Table 3-6:

Table 3-6 Structure of the Questionnaire

Listening Strategy Example Question number

Memory I will relate the content to my own experiences and common sense.

Qs 1~4

Cognitive I predict the content and then confirm it.

Qs 5-8 Direct

Compensation I predict the content by pause, intonation, or mood.

Qs 9-12

(7)

Metacognitive I seek for the ways to learn listening and the chances to practice listening actively.

Qs 13-16

Affective While listening, I encourage and assure myself by telling myself “I am right.” or “I know that.”, etc

Qs 17-20 Indirect

Social I try to find some friends or classmates to practice listening.

Qs 21-24

The direct strategies dealt with language, including memory, cognitive, and compensation strategies, while the indirect strategies were about the “general management of learning” (cited in Oxford, 1990, p. 15), containing metacognitive, affective, and social strategies

2

. Each strategy in the present study consisted of four substrategies. Among the 24 substrategies, nine substrategies

3

were similar to Teng’s (1996) and modified later. The remaining 15 substrategies were taken from Wang (2002) with some modifications.

Oxford’s classification (1990) was adopted in the present questionnaire, because it was a popular system of strategy use and has been adopted by many studies. It provided a more elaborate and comprehensive typology and took more consideration into the different aspects of learning strategies. I agreed with Oxford (1990) that

“…the learner is a ‘whole person’ who uses intellectual, social, emotional, and physical resources and is therefore not merely a cognitive/metacognitive information-processing machine” (cited in Oxford, 1992/1993, p.20). However, the

2

According to Oxford (1990), memory strategies are used for remembering and retrieving new information, cognitive strategies for understanding and producing the language, compensation strategies for using the language despite knowledge gaps, metacognitive strategies for coordinating the learning process, affective strategies for regulating emotions, and social strategies for learning with others.

3

The nine substrategies included CP2, CP3, MT3, MT4, AF3, AF4, SC1, SC3, and SC4.

(8)

total number of strategies in the classification of Oxford (1990) was too big for our subjects, and it might decrease their willingness and patience in filling out the questionnaire. Therefore, the total number of strategies was reduced and the statements were also modified. By making these changes, it is hoped that some insights about strategy use could be obtained.

3.3 Procedures

In this section, I will introduce the pilot study, formal experiment, and data analysis and scoring respectively.

3.3.1 Pilot Study

Before conducting the experiment, the questionnaire was proofread by a language teacher to see if the wording was understandable for the subjects. Then, 10 third-year senior high school students (6 males and 4 females) from one class taught by one of my colleagues were asked to take the LCT first. After they finished the LCT, they were given the answers and asked if the LCT was too difficult for them. They said it was OK. Then, they were asked to fill in the personal information sheet and questionnaire. One question about the listening situation in the questionnaire was raised by a subject. He was later told that the listening situation referred to a general listening situation, not just to a listening test. Then the subjects were asked if there were any more questions they did not understand clearly. No further questions were raised. It took them about 30 minutes to finish the LCT, five minutes to fill in the personal information sheet and 30 minutes to fill in the questionnaire respectively.

Therefore, the question concerning the listening situation was revised. Moreover, to provide the subjects with sufficient time for listening to directions, taking tests, and filling in questionnaires, the formal test was decided to be given in the English

Composition class which contained two consecutive periods.

(9)

3.3.2 Formal Testing

The formal test took the subjects two continual periods to complete it. In the first period, the researcher spent 5 minutes explaining to the students the purpose and the procedures of the study. To motivate them, the subjects were told that the listening test was taken from the intermediate listening test of the GEPT and that taking the test could help them know more about their listening ability. Moreover, they were told that their responses would have nothing to do with their academic scores. After making sure they did not have any questions, the listening test was given. Then, the subjects were asked to fill in the Personal Information Sheet (see Appendix D).

The questionnaire was given in the second period. Five minutes were used to give the subjects directions. The following things were stressed. First, the questionnaire aimed at exploring the subjects’ strategies used in general listening situations. Second, the subjects were welcome to ask any question about the questionnaire. Third, the study attempted to explore the subjects’ “real” listening strategies, not those they thought the teacher or their classmates would use. It took the subjects about 15 minutes to finish the questionnaire.

3.3.3 Data Analysis and Scoring

The present study mainly used a quantitative method to explore the issues because of its highest degree of structures, simplified data manipulation, and a far more manageable analysis (O’Malley & Chamot, 1990). Both the listening test and the questionnaire were analyzed by the SPSS package (Statistical Packages for the Social Science) Version 11.5. For the convenience of comparing the results, two points were given for each question in the LCT, so the full marks were 90. As for the scoring of the questionnaire, listeners scored five points for choosing “always” in the likert scale, four points for “usually,” three points for “sometimes,” two points for

“seldom,” and one point for “never.”

(10)

The descriptive statistics, paired-samples t-test, independent-samples t-test, multiple regression and stepwise regression were all used. To address the first and second research questions, the descriptive statistics was first used to find out the mean, and standard deviation of the listening performance of the three parts in the LCT.

Then the listening performance was computed according to the test format and text type. The paired-samples t-test was then used to examine the test effects to see if there was a significant difference between the subjects’ performance on the tests with pictures and the ones without pictures. The same method was applied to examine the text effects.

To answer the third research question, the descriptive statistics was used to find out the mean, standard deviation, and ranking of the listening strategy use of the HP and LP groups. After that, the independent-samples t-test was used to see if there was a significant difference between the listening strategy use of the two groups.

Finally, to answer the fourth and fifth research questions, the multiple regression and stepwise regression were used to find out the relationship between the Test Format/text type and listening strategies. The questionnaire responses were later analyzed and summarized.

3.4 Results

In this section, the results of the listening comprehension test will be reported first, followed by the results of the questionnaire.

3.4.1 Listening Comprehension Test

The LCT performance of the subjects are listed in Table 3-7:

(11)

Table 3-7 Subjects’ Performance on the LCT HP

(n=32)

LP (n=33) Group

Mean S.D. Mean S.D.

Part A 26.88 2.59 16.85 3.71

Part B 26.63 2.46 12.30 4.10

Part C 23.25 3.76 12.55 3.95

Note: Range: 1~30

It was found that the HP group performed best on Part A (M: 26.88), followed by Part B (M: 26.63), and then Part C (M: 23.25). The LP group performed best on Part A (M:

16.85), followed by Part C (M: 12.55), and then Part B (M: 12.30). That is to say, both groups performed best on Part A, and worse on Part B or Part C.

3.4.2 Questionnaire

This section will present the subjects’ questionnaire responses. Since there are 24 items and one open-ended question in the questionnaire, the results of the use of the 24 substrategies will be reported first, followed by the use of other strategies.

3.4.2.1 The Use of the Listening Strategies

As can be seen in Table 3-8, the HP group used listening strategies more frequently than the LP group:

Table 3-8 Average Strategy Use Frequency of the Subjects Group Mean S.D. t-value

HP 2.93 .46 LP 2.68 .36

2.475*

Note: p<.05*

The average strategy use frequency of the HP group was 2.93, while that of the LP

(12)

group was 2.68. The independent-samples t-test results showed that there was a significant difference at the .05 level between the two groups. That is, the HP group used listening strategies significantly more frequently than the LP group.

As for the details in the use of the six strategies, the following six tables give related information. First, let us look at the subjects’ use of memory strategy, as can be seen in Table 3-9:

Table 3-9 Subjects’ Use of the Memory Strategy

Substrategy Group Never Seldom Sometimes Usually Always Mean HP 2

(6%)

5 (16%)

6 (19%)

12 (38%)

7 (22%)

3.53 MM1

LP 5 (15%)

5 (15%)

12 (36%)

8 (24%)

3 (9%)

2.97

HP 2 (6%)

4 (13%)

14 (44%)

7 (22%)

5 (16%)

3.28 MM2

LP 3 (9%)

8 (24%)

11 (33%)

9 (27%)

2

(6%) 2.97 HP 4

(13%)

8 (25%)

7 (22%)

7 (22%)

6 (19%)

3.09 MM3

LP 2 (6%)

11 (33%)

10 (30%)

6 (18%)

4

(12%) 2.97 HP 8

(25%)

6 (19%)

11 (34%)

5 (16%)

2 (6%)

2.59 MM4

LP 7 (21%)

13 (39%)

9 (27%)

3 (9%)

1

(3%) 2.33

As shown above, the HP group used MM1 more frequently than the LP group. The

(13)

mean of the average use frequency for the HP group was 3.53, while that for the LP group was 2.97. However, there was no significant difference between the two groups.

It is clear that most subjects of the HP group chose “Always” (22%) and “Usually”

(38%). They accounted for 60% of the whole group. Most subjects of the LP group chose “Usually” (24%) and “Sometimes” (36%), and they also accounted for 60% of the group.

With regard to MM2, the HP group also used more frequently than the LP group.

The mean of the average use frequency for the HP group was 3.28, while that for the LP group was 2.97. But, the difference was not significant. Most subjects of the HP group chose “Always” (16%), “Usually” (22%), and “Sometimes” (44%). The LP group mostly chose “Usually” (27%), “Sometimes” (33%) and “Seldom” (24%).

Similarly, the HP group used MM3 more frequently than the LP group. The mean of the average use frequency for the HP group was 3.09, while that for the LP group was 2.97. Again, there was no significant difference between the two groups. More subjects in the HP group (19%) chose “Always” than those in the LP group (12%) did, and also more subjects in the HP group (22%) chose “Usually” than those in the LP group (18%) did.

The HP group used MM4 more frequently than the LP group. The mean of the average use frequency for the HP group was 2.59, while that for the LP group was 2.33. However, the difference was not significant. In addition, it was found that more subjects in the HP group (6%) chose “Always” than those in the LP group (3%) did, more subjects in the HP group (16%) chose “Usually” than those in LP group (9%) did and also more subjects in the HP group (34%) chose “Sometimes” than those in the LP group (27%) did.

In general, the HP group used 4 substrategies of the memory strategy more

frequently than the LP group, but none of them reached a significant difference.

(14)

Among the four substrategies, the HP group tended to use MM1 most frequently. The LP group tended to use MM1 and MM2 more frequently than the other two substrategies.

The details in the use of the cognitive strategy are shown in Table 3-10:

Table 3-10 Subjects’ Use of the Cognitive Strategy

Substrategy Group Never Seldom Sometimes Usually Always Mean HP 1

(3%)

12 (38%)

6 (19%)

8 (25%)

5 (16%)

3.13 CG1

LP 0 (0%)

8 (24%)

11 (33%)

7 (21%)

7 (21%)

3.39

HP 1 (3%)

5 (16%)

15 (47%)

9 (28%)

2 (6%)

3.19 CG2

LP 3 (9%)

9 (27%)

14 (42%)

5 (15%)

2 (6%)

2.82

HP 7 (22%)

18 (56%)

6 (19%)

1 (3%)

0 (0%)

2.03 CG3

LP 9 (27%)

18 (55%)

2 (6%)

4 (12%)

0 (0%)

2.03

HP 0 (0%)

11 (34%)

11 (34%)

8 (25%)

2 (6%)

3.03 CG4

LP 4 (12%)

8 (24%)

11 (33%)

8 (24%)

2 (6%)

2.88

We can see that the HP group used CG1 less frequently than the LP group. The mean

of the average use frequency for the HP group was 3.13, while that for the LP group

was 3.39. But, there was no significant difference between the two groups. It was

(15)

found that most HP subjects chose “Seldom” (38%), but most LP subjects chose

“sometimes” (33%).

As far as CG2 is concerned, the HP group used it more frequently than the LP group. The mean of the average use frequency for the HP group was 3.19, while that for the LP group was 2.82. However, no significant difference was found. Moreover, most of the HP group chose “Usually” (28%) and “Sometimes” (47%), and most LP subjects chose “Sometimes” (42%) and “Seldom” (27%).

The HP group used CG3 slightly more frequently than the LP group. The mean of the average use frequency for the HP group was 2.03, while that for the LP group was 2.03. No significant difference was found between the two groups. In addition, both groups chose “Always,” “Seldom” and “Never” with a similar rate.

The HP group used CG4 more frequently than the LP group. The mean of the average use frequency for the HP group was 3.03, while that for the LP group was 2.88. Again, there was no significant difference. Besides, the numbers of the subjects in the HP group choosing “Always”, “Usually”, and “Sometimes” were the same as those of the subjects in the LP group.

With regard to the cognitive strategy, the HP group used the substrategies more frequently than the LP group except CG1, though none of them reached a significant difference. Among the four substrategies, the HP group used CG2 most frequently while the LP group used CG1 most frequently.

Next, let us take a look at the use of the compensation strategy, as shown in

Table 3-11:

(16)

Table 3-11 Subjects’ Use of the Compensation Strategy

Substrategy Group Never Seldom Sometimes Usually Always Mean HP 2

(6%)

6 (19%)

10 (31%)

8 (25%)

6 (19%)

3.31 CP1

LP 2 (6%)

7 (21%)

15 (45%)

7 (21%)

2 (6%)

3.00

HP 3 (9%)

6 (19%)

5 (16%)

11 (34%)

7 (22%)

3.41 CP2

LP 2 (6%)

8 (24%)

15 (45%)

6 (18%)

2 (6%)

2.94

HP 4 (13%)

7 (22%)

9 (28%)

8 (25%)

4 (13%)

3.03 CP3

LP 7 (21%)

10 (30%)

10 (30%)

2 (6%)

4 (12%)

2.58

HP 0 (0%)

3 (9%)

4 (13%)

16 (50%)

9 (28%)

3.97 CP4

LP 0 (0%)

2 (6%)

12 (36%)

16 (48%)

3 (9%)

3.61

It was found that the HP group used CP1 more frequently than the LP group. The mean of the average use frequency for the HP group was 3.31, while that for the LP group was 3.00. However, the difference did not reach a significant level. More subjects of the HP group chose “Always” (19%) than those of the LP group (6%).

Also, the HP group chose “Usually” (25%) more than the LP group (21%).

The HP group also used CP2 more frequently than the LP group. The mean of

the average use frequency for the HP group was 3.41, while that for the LP group was

(17)

2.94. The two groups were not significantly different in their strategy use. Most subjects of the HP group chose “Always” (22%) and “Usually” (34%), but most subjects of the LP group chose “Sometimes” (45%) and “Seldom” (24%).

Likewise, the HP group used CP3 more frequently than the LP group. The mean of the average use frequency for the HP group was 3.03, while that for the LP group was 2.58. Again, a significant difference between the two groups was not found.

Besides, most subjects of the HP group chose “Usually” (25%) and “Sometimes”

(28%), and most subjects of the LP group chose “Sometimes” (30%) and “Seldom”

(30%).

The HP group also used CP4 more frequently than the LP group. The mean of the average use frequency for the HP group was 3.97, while that for the LP group was 3.61. However, there was no significant difference between the two groups in this respect. In addition, it was found that most subjects of the HP group chose “Always”

(28%) and “Usually” (50%), and most subjects of the LP group chose “Usually” (48%) and “Sometimes” (36%).

In brief, the HP group used 4 substrategies of the compensation strategy more frequently than the LP group, but none of them reached a significant difference.

Among the four substrategies, both the HP and LP group used CP4 most frequently and used CP3 least frequently.

Now, let us turn to the use of the metacognitive strategy, as shown in Table 3-12:

(18)

Table 3-12 Subjects’ Use of the Metacognitive Strategy

Substrategy Group Never Seldom Sometimes Usually Always Mean HP 0

(0%)

4 (13%)

12 (38%)

12 (38%)

4 (13%)

3.50 MT1

LP 2 (6%)

7 (21%)

15 (45%)

7 (21%)

2 (6%)

3.00

HP 1 (3%)

9 (28%)

6 (19%)

11 (34%)

5 (15%)

3.31 MT2

LP 5 (15%)

9 (27%)

6 (18%)

11 (33%)

2 (6%)

2.88

HP 4 (13%)

11 (34%)

12 (38%)

4 (13%)

1 (3%)

2.59 MT3

LP 4 (12%)

12 (36%)

7 (21%)

0 (0%)

0 (0%)

2.09

HP 0 (0%)

7 (22%)

13 (41%)

10 (31%)

2 (6%)

3.22 MT4

LP 2 (6%)

16 (48%)

13 (39%)

2 (6%)

0 (0%)

2.45

From Table 3-12, it is clear that the HP group used MT1 more frequently than the LP group. The mean of the average use frequency for the HP group was 3.50, while that for the LP group was 3.00. The t-test results showed that a significant difference existed between the two groups (t= 2.177, p<.05). More subjects of the HP group chose “Always” (13%) than those of the LP group (6%) and more subjects of the HP group chose “Usually” (38%) than those of the LP group (21%).

The HP group used MT2 more frequently than the LP group. The mean of the

(19)

average use frequency for the HP group was 3.31, while that for the LP group was 2.88. However, the two means did not reach a significant difference. The HP and LP groups had the same numbers of the subjects that chose “Usually”, “Sometimes” and

“Seldom.”

The HP group used MT3 more frequently than the LP group. The mean of the average use frequency for the HP group was 2.59, while that for the

LP group was 2.09. Besides, a significant difference between the two groups was found (t=2.511, p<.05). Moreover, more subjects of the HP group chose “Always”,

“Usually” and “Sometimes” than the LP group.

The HP group also used MT4 more frequently than the LP group. The mean of the average use frequency for the HP group was 3.22, while that for the LP group was 2.45. The difference between the two means reached a significant level (t=3.883, p<.001). Additionally, it was found that most of the HP group chose “Usually” (31%) and “Sometimes” (41%), but the LP group chose “Sometimes” (39%) and “Seldom”

(48%).

In general, the HP group used four substrategies of the metacognitive strategy more frequently than the LP group. Significant differences were found in all substrategies except MT2. Both groups had the same frequency ranking of using the four substrategies, with MT1 being the most frequently used and MT3 the least frequently used.

Table 3-13 shows the subjects’ use of the affective strategy:

(20)

Table 3-13 Subjects’ Use of the Affective Strategy

Substrategy Group Never Seldom Sometimes Usually Always Mean HP 8

(25%)

13 (41%)

8 (25%)

3 (9%)

0 (0%)

2.19 AF1

LP 6 (18%)

12 (36%)

9 (27%)

4 (12%)

2 (6%)

2.52

HP 8 (25%)

9 (28%)

8 (25%)

7 (22%)

0 (0%)

2.44 AF2

LP 6 (18%)

13 (39%)

5 (15%)

8 (24%)

1 (3%)

2.55

HP 12 (38%)

14 (44%)

4 (13%)

2 (6%)

0 (0%)

1.88 AF3

LP 12 (36%)

12 (36%)

7 (21%)

1 (3%)

1 (3%)

2.00

HP 10 (31%)

17 (53%)

4 (13%)

1 (3%)

0 (0%)

1.88 AF4

LP 12 (36%)

11 (33%)

8 (24%)

2 (6%)

0 (0%)

2.00

Clearly, the HP group used AF1 less frequently than the LP group. The mean of the average use frequency for the HP group was 2.19, while that for the LP group was 2.52. No significant difference was found between the two groups. Most of the HP group chose “Seldom” (41%) and most of the LP group chose “Seldom” (36%) as well. The numbers of the subjects of the two groups were similar in this respect.

The HP group also used AF2 less frequently than the LP group. The mean of the

average use frequency for the HP group was 2.44, while that for the LP group was

(21)

2.55. However, there was no significant difference between both groups. Moreover, most of the HP group chose “Usually” (28%) and “Sometimes” (47%). As for the LP group, most of the subjects chose “Sometimes” (42%) and “Seldom” (27%).

The HP group used AF3 less frequently than the LP group. The mean of the average use frequency for the HP group was 1.88, while that for the LP group was 2.00. No significant difference was found between the two groups. In addition, the number of the HP group that chose “Always,” “Seldom” and “Never” was similar to that of the LP group.

The HP group used AF4 less frequently than the LP group. The mean of the average use frequency for the HP group was 1.88, while that for the LP group was 2.00. Again, the two means were not different from each other at a significant level.

Besides, both groups tended to choose “Seldom” and “Never” more.

In a word, different from the above-mentioned strategies, the HP group used four substrategies of the affective strategy less frequently than the LP group, but none of them reached a significant difference. Both groups showed a great similarity in their use of the four substrategies.

Finally, let us look at the social strategy use, as shown in Table 3-14:

(22)

Table 3- 14 Subjects’ Use of the Social Strategy

Substrategy Group Never Seldom Sometimes Usually Always Mean HP 1

(3%)

4 (13%)

8 (25%)

14 (44%)

5 (16%)

3.56 SC1

LP 2 (6%)

5 (15%)

17 (52%)

7 (21%)

2 (6%)

3.06

HP 2 (6%)

11 (34%)

12 (38%)

6 (19%)

1 (3%)

2.78 SC2

LP 3 (9%)

9 (27%)

14 (42%)

7 (21%)

0 (0%)

2.76

HP 6 (19%)

19 (59%)

5 (16%)

2 (6%)

0 (0%)

2.09 SC3

LP 17 (52%)

11 (33%)

5 (15%)

0 (0%)

0 (0%)

1.64

HP 2 (6%)

3 (9%)

10 (31%)

15 (47%)

2 (6%)

3.38 SC4

LP 3 (9%)

7 (21%)

16 (48%)

5 (15%)

2 (6%)

2.88

As shown above, the HP group used SC1 more frequently than the LP group. The mean of the average use frequency for the HP group was 3.56, while that for the LP group was 3.06. The t-test results showed that there was a significant difference between the two groups (t=2.077, p<.05). Besides, the HP group chose “Always”

(16%) and “Usually” (44%) more than the LP group.

The HP group used SC2 more frequently than the LP group. The mean of the

average use frequency for the HP group was 2.78, while that for the LP group was

(23)

2.76. According to the t-test results, the two means did not reach a significant difference. Besides, both groups chose “Sometimes” and “Seldom” more than other options.

The HP group used SC3 more frequently than the LP group. The mean of the average use frequency for the HP group was 2.09, while that for the LP group was 1.64. The two means differed significantly (t=2.427, p<.05). Moreover, most of the HP group chose “Seldom” (59%), while most of the LP group chose “Never” (52%).

The HP group also used SC4 more frequently than the LP group. The mean of the average use frequency for the HP group was 3.38, while that for the LP group was 2.88. The t-test results showed a significant difference between the two groups (t=2.032, p<.05). Additionally, most of the HP group chose “Usually” (47%) and

“Sometimes” (31%), but most of the LP group chose “Sometimes” (48%) and

“Seldom” (21%).

Briefly speaking, so far as the social strategy is concerned, the HP group used the substrategies significantly more frequently than the LP group except SC2. The two groups had the same frequency ranking of using the four substrategies, with SC1 being the most frequently used and SC3 the least frequently used.

To sum up, the following generalizations are derived from the present findings.

First, the HP group used all strategies more frequently than the LP group except the cognitive strategy (CG1) and the affective strategy (AF). Second, the difference between the HP and LP groups was significant in response to the metacognitive strategy (MT1, MT3, and MT4) and the social strategy (SC1, SC3, and SC4).

3.4.2.2 The Use of Other Strategies

This section reports the results of the last question of the questionnaire, which

asked if there was any other strategy used by the subjects in addition to the above

mentioned ones. It was found that the HP group was more active concerning their

(24)

strategy use. For example, one student of the HP group reported that he would “chat with foreigners” (HP21), another mentioned that he would “use the resources on the Internet” (HP9), three of the HP subjects said that they would “listen to English broadcast or songs,” (HP5, 17, 28) and two mentioned that they would “watch films with subtitles first and then no subtitles” (HP13, 25). On the other hand, two students of the LP group would “watch more films” (LP8, 26), and one mentioned that he would “listen to English songs” (LP12). In fact, all these strategies actually belonged to MT3 (i.e., I seek for the ways to learn listening and the chances to practice listening actively). Therefore, no other strategies were reported in both groups. Nevertheless, we can see that the HP group was more flexible in using strategies and had more positive attitudes toward improving their listening ability.

3.5 Summary of Chapter Three

In this chapter, I have reported the present research design and findings. The

results concerning the subjects’ listening comprehension included the LCT

performance of the subjects. The results regarding the questionnaire were reported in

detail with respect to the strategy use (such as the average use frequency, their use of

the 24 substrategies and other strategies). Further discussions about the results will be

presented in Chapter Four.

參考文獻

相關文件

Then they work in groups of four to design a questionnaire on diets and eating habits based on the information they have collected from the internet and in Part A, and with

The A-Level Biology Curriculum aims to provide learning experiences through which students will acquire or develop the necessary biological knowledge and

volume suppressed mass: (TeV) 2 /M P ∼ 10 −4 eV → mm range can be experimentally tested for any number of extra dimensions - Light U(1) gauge bosons: no derivative couplings. =&gt;

We explicitly saw the dimensional reason for the occurrence of the magnetic catalysis on the basis of the scaling argument. However, the precise form of gap depends

The temperature angular power spectrum of the primary CMB from Planck, showing a precise measurement of seven acoustic peaks, that are well fit by a simple six-parameter

• Formation of massive primordial stars as origin of objects in the early universe. • Supernova explosions might be visible to the most

Miroslav Fiedler, Praha, Algebraic connectivity of graphs, Czechoslovak Mathematical Journal 23 (98) 1973,

• elearning pilot scheme (Four True Light Schools): WIFI construction, iPad procurement, elearning school visit and teacher training, English starts the elearning lesson.. 2012 •