• 沒有找到結果。

Chapter 6 Conclusion

6.1. Summary of the Major Findings

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

148

Chapter 6 Conclusion

This chapter concludes the thesis based on the findings of the previous chapters.

Afterwards, limitations of this study and suggestions for future studies on agreement are given.

6.1. Summary of the Major Findings

This section summarizes the major findings of this study. Findings of agreement in general are presented first. Then, the influence of the social factor—gender is

depicted.

6.1.1. Agreement in General

To answer the research questions and verify hypotheses of this study, findings for agreement in general can be divided into two parts: categories of agreement, and degrees of agreement.

(1) Categories of Agreement

Research question A: Among the three categories of agreement (namely, head act alone HA, supportive move alone SM, and head act with supportive move HA+SM), which category is more preferred by Mandarin speakers?

Hypothesis A-1: Head act alone (HA) would occur more frequently than supportive moves alone (SM).

Finding A-1: Statistic results show that HA is not used significantly more than SM.

Hypothesis A-1 is thus not verified. One possible reason to explain this phenomenon is that because SM, although indirect, could still be used to effectively express agreement because interlocutors in this study are either close

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

149

friends or couples, who share much background information which can help hearers receive the intention of agreement through inference. Other explanation is that using SM alone can express high involvement and thus establish solidarity.

Hypothesis A-2: Head act alone (HA) would occur more frequently than head act with supportive move (HA+SM).

Finding A-2: Statistic results of this study show that HA is performed significantly more frequently than HA+SM, Thus, Hypothesis A-2 is verified. It means that direct and simple method to make agreement is preferred.

Hypothesis A-3: Head act with supportive moves (HA+SM) emerges more frequently than supportive move alone (SM).

Finding A-3: Statistic results of this study show that SM is used significantly more frequently than HA+SM. Hypothesis A-3 is not verified.

(2) Degrees of Agreement

Research question B: Among the various kinds of agreement by degrees, which one is used more frequently, agreement without contingency (including upgrading and preserving agreement) or agreement with contingency (i.e. downgraded agreement)?

Hypothesis B-1: Agreement without contingency (WOC) would occur more frequently than agreement with contingency (WC).

Finding B-1: Statistical analysis indicates that WOCs are performed significantly more frequently than WCs. Hypothesis B-1 is thus verified. It means that people avoid using downgrading agreement, which carries connotation of disagreement which may bring forth misunderstanding or conflict.

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

150

Hypothesis B-2: Upgrading agreement is more frequently applied than preserving agreement.

Finding B-2: According to statistic results of this study, upgrading agreement is performed significantly more frequently than preserving agreement. Thus, Hypothesis B-2 is verified. A possible reason is that people want to fulfill hearers’ positive face wants so that they frequently maximize agreement.

Besides the findings given above, this study aims at the interaction between categories of agreement and agreement by degrees, which is not investigated before. It is found that in various categories of agreement, Finding B-1 and Finding B-2 are repeated. In other words, when categories of agreement and degrees of agreement are interacted, the concept of degrees of agreement is more important than that of categories of agreement. That is, in every category of agreement, maximization of agreement is enacted frequently.

One of the purposes of this thesis is to bridge the gap for the unexamined pragmatic strategies in agreement. Unlike previous studies of agreement (Pomerantz, 1984; Kotthoff, 1993; Kuo, 1994; Mulkay, 1985; Baym, 1996; Rattai, 2003), in which pragmatic strategies in agreement are not examined, it is found in this study that textual rhetoric strategies are used significantly more than interpersonal rhetoric strategies. Besides, for the use of textual rhetoric strategies, it is found that the strategies performed most frequently are emphasis and elaboration, which means that people’s primary goal is to meet the end of clarity and expressivity of information.

6.1.2. Agreement by Gender

This thesis points out how men and women differ from each other in agreement constructions as well as related pragmatic strategies, which is not specifically pointed out in the previous gender studies on agreement (Kalcik, 1975; Leet-Pellegrini, 1980;

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

151

Edelsky, 1981; Coates, 1989; Holmes, 1995: 60). The following paragraphs show major findings of the influence of gender in the use of agreement.

Research Question C: Is gender an influential factor to the construction of agreement?

Hypothesis C-1: Speaker’s gender is a significant factor to manipulate the construction and pragmatic strategies in the performance of agreement.

Finding C-1: Speaker’s gender is an influential factor in agreement, which can be verified by several findings below. First, for categories of agreement, male speakers apply HA significantly more than SM; whereas, female speakers has no significant difference in the comparison of HA and SM. It means that when making agreement, men may put more emphasis on efficiency of message transmission than women do. By contrast, female speakers frequently use both HA and SM. Female speakers’ use of SMs, which can be applied to show high involvement in conversations, may mean to fulfill the functions of politeness, solidarity, and rapport which are highly revered by women.

Second, for degrees of agreement, female speakers perform WOC much more frequently than male speakers do. It means that women try harder than men in reinforcement of agreement and avoidance of using downgrading agreement.

Third, for pragmatic strategies in agreement, male speakers and female speakers differ from each other on the use of elaboration. Elaboration, which can show high involvement, is a strategy frequently used by women.

Hypothesis C-2: Hearer’s gender is a significant factor to determine people’s ways of construction and choice of pragmatic strategies in agreement.

Finding C-2: Hearer’s gender is verified to be an influential factor in agreement. First, for the categories of agreement, female hearers receive significantly more EPC

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

152

than male hearers do. It means that people may think female hearers like to be agreed through the way of building on each other’s contribution to show high involvement. By contrast, people may think male hearers, putting more

emphasis on the efficiency of message transmission, may not like to be agreed by EPC for consuming time to decode agreement.

Second, when HA and SM are compared, male hearers receive significantly more HA than SM; whereas, female hearers receive similar amounts of HA and SM. Perhaps it is because people think efficiency of information exchange is the first priority for male hearers. And for female hearers, people may think they prefer to be agreed by showing high involvement and elaborating more on their contributions.

Third, for pragmatic strategies to show agreement, female hearers receive significantly more elaboration than male hearers do. It means that people may think that women like to be agreed by flouting the Quantity Maxim in CP (Grice, 1975), which is often used to show politeness.

Hypothesis C-3: When both speaker’s and hearer’s genders are considered, gender is a significant factor to manipulate the construction and pragmatic strategies in the performance of agreement.

Finding C-3: When both speaker’s and hearer’s genders are considered, gender is an influential factor in agreement. First, for categories of agreement, significant difference is only shown in the comparison of SMs used by FF and FM. To be specific, the FF group applies much more SM (especially the subtype EPC) than the FM group does. It means that women, unlike men, are easily influenced by hearer’s gender and change their behavior on the usage of SM.

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

153

Second, for degrees of agreement, in general, FF and FM use WOC significantly more than WC. Additionally, FF is the only one group among the four that use significantly more upgrading agreement than preserving agreement.

In other words, women are the one who try hard to maximize agreement to fulfill hearers’ positive face wants.

Based on the results above, women are the one who makes more effort to make agreement forceful. Furthermore, when interlocutors are both women, this tendency becomes more obvious. Perhaps it is because that for women, the purpose of

communication is to reach consensus, maintain solidarity, and avoid conflict between self and other (Tannen, 1994). Besides, women are more hearer-oriented and more willing to fulfill hearers’ positive face wants (Brown and Levinson, 1978; Woods, 1997). Therefore, women and men differentiate from each other in the way to make agreement.

It is concluded that gender differences occur in the construction and pragmatic strategies of agreement. Thus, in general, gender is an influential factor to the construction of agreement.