• 沒有找到結果。

Chapter Ⅲ METHOD

3.6. Physiology signals analysis

3.6.4 Data analysis

PAHSE 1

To measure the motivation score by

physiology signals and compare the motivation,

affective experiences and cognitive load of learners in digital-game based learning and traditional static e-learning environments. The attention in ARCS model was measured by NeuroSky, the affective experiences was measured by emWave and the cognitive load was measured by eye-tracker. The physiology signals variables in PAHSE1 were all shown in Table 11. Learner attention is recognized by the Neurosky system, it was used to detect neuron electric

triggering activity, and it has the earphone appearance. According NeuroSky proprietary Attention & Meditation eSense algorithms, NeuroSky can report the attention score each second.

The range of attention score is 1 to 100 (1=very low attention level and 100=very high attention level). Learner affective experience was recognized by emWave system, which uses human pulse physiological signals to identify Coherence score every 5 seconds. Coherence score have 0, 1 and 2 (0 = negative emotion, 1 = peaceful and 2 = positive emotion).

Cognitive load was recognized by eye tracker. To summarize the cognitive load between Digital Game-Based Learning and traditional static e-learning, 5 eye-movement measures were used, including total fixation duration (TFD), number of fixations (NF), and average fixation duration (AFD), percentage of viewing time (PVT) and frequency of saccade path (FSP).

Table 11.Physiology signals variables in PHASE 1

Physiology signals variables Definition Reference

Eye movement variables:

Total fixation duration (TFD) Number of fixations (NF) Average fixation duration (AFD) Percentage of viewing time (PVT) Frequency of saccade path (FSP)

Cognitive load

Sum of durations of all fixation points. (millisecond, ms) Sum of number of all fixation points. (piece)

Average duration of a fixation point. (millisecond, ms) Total fixation duration divided by the total time. (%) Times of saccades divided by total time tracked. (piece)

When above

Positive emotion occurrence times divided by the total emotion state occurrence times. (%)

The range of attention score is 1 to 100; 1=very low

When AT gets bigger, learning

The attention data, emotion data and eye-movement data were exported to Excel, and SPSS was then applied for further statistical analyses. In phase 1, the descriptive statistics and one-way ANOVA were performed to find differences in attention, emotion and eye movements between the different learning groups (DGBL vs. Static e-learning). In addition, post-test scores were analyzed by ANCOVA, in which instructional strategy (DGBL vs. static e-learning ) was the between-groups factor, and pre-test scores were treated as covariates in order to control for the effects of pre-existing between-group differences on subsequent analysis.

PAHSE 2

Before analyzing the problem solving strategy in each group, several LookZones were defined as follows. In post-test problem 1 (Figure 17), LookZone “Title” referred to the area of problem statement. For options, LookZone “correct” referred to the area of option 2 and 3, LookZone “incorrect” included option 1 and 5. For key factor, LookZone 4 and F are the key here to solve this problem. In post-test problem 2 (Figure 18), LookZone “Title” referred to the area of problem statement. For options, LookZone “correct” referred to the area of option 3, LookZone “incorrect” included option 1, 2, 4 and 5. For key factor, LookZone N, W, F and K are the key here to solve this problem.

4

F

Correct Incorrect

Incorrect Text

Correct

Figure 17.The definition scheme for LookZones in problem1

LookZone “Title” referred to the area of problem statement. For options, LookZone “correct”

referred to the area of option 2 and 3, LookZone “incorrect” included option 1 and 5. For key factor, LookZone “4” and “F” are the key here to solve this problem.

Correct Text

Incorrect

Figure 18.The definition scheme for LookZones in problem 2

LookZone “Title” referred to the area of problem statement. For options, LookZone “correct”

referred to the area of option 3, LookZone “incorrect” included option 1, 2, 4 and 5. For key factor, LookZone N, W, F and K are the key here to solve this problem.

Table 12.Physiology signals variables in PHASE 2

Eye-movement measure Definition

Percentage of time spent in zone (PTSZ) Total time in a look-zone, such as a text or picture zone, divided by total time tracked. (%)

Fixation count (FC) Number of fixation points in a zone. (piece)

Percentage of total fixation (PTF) Fixation count divided by number of fixations. (%)

Percentage of time fixated related to total fixation duration (PTFRTFD)

Total fixation duration in a zone divided by total fixation duration. (%)

Hot Zone image In the image, the mapped color varied from individuals’ total fixation duration at each pixel on the screen. The darker the red color was, the longer the total fixation duration accumulated on a pixel.

Sequential analysis Sequential analysis can effectively infer the overall behavioral path patterns during learners’ thinking.

Meanwhile, to analyze the attention distributions on different medium components (look-zones) on post-test problem, 4 eye movement measures were used, including percentage of time spent in zone (PTSZ), fixation count (FC), percentage of total fixations (PTF), percentage of time fixated related to total fixation duration (PTFRTFD). In order to describe an entire picture of learners’ visual attention on the post-test questions, we analyzed the Hot Zone images produced by DataViewer programming. In the image, the mapped color varied from individuals’

total fixation duration at each pixel on the screen. The darker the red color was, the longer the total fixation duration accumulated on a pixel. Therefore, the red or orange spots (i.e., Hot Zones) represented locations where the information had been processed longer and more deeply by participants while green colors represented locations where the information had been minimally processed. In addition, sequential analysis can effectively infer the overall behavioral path

patterns during learners’ thinking. We can observe the problem solving logic of leaners.

In phase 2, the one-way ANOVA were performed to find differences in eye movements between the different learning style group (Active vs. Reflective), learning environment group (DGBL vs. Static e-learning), different learners’ major group (Non-science vs. Science) and different post-test performance group (High vs. Low). In order to further analyze the scan patterns of participants in both post-test performance groups, two lag sequential analyses were conducted for each group on the sequences of LookZones fixated of all participants.

Chapter Ⅳ DATA ANALYSIS

For verifying the hypotheses in this study, the results were showed in chapter 4. In section 4.1, physiology signals for learning motivation, affective experiences and cognitive load were represented. In section 4.2, this paper found that DGBL group have better academic achievement but have no significant differences. In section 4.3, the result showed that high working memory capacity learning style group knew better where to look the key factors. In section 4.4, the result showed that DGBL group knew better where to look the key factors. In section 4.5, that result showed that Science major group couldn’t know better where to look the key factors, but Non-science majors’ learners need more clues to solve problem. In section 4.6, we found that successful problem solvers are able to recognize and concentrate on relevant cues. In section 4.7, the result showed that Successful problem solvers inspected the factors in a different pattern from unsuccessful problem solvers. In section 4.8, we found that physiology signals during learning couldn’t explain learners’ learning style. In section 4.9, Pearson correlation coefficient analysis was used and found that earning style (active vs. reflective) could be measure by eye movement variables when learners solving problem. In section 4.10, the summary of hypotheses verified was showed in table. Finally, discussion was summarized in section 4.11.

4.1 DGBL group have more cognitive load than traditional static e-learning group

4.1.1 Physiology signals representation for learning motivation, affective experiences and cognitive load

To compare how Digital Game-Based Learning affect learning motivation, affective experiences and cognitive load, the attention in ARCS model was measured by attention score

(AT), the affective experiences was measured by occupied percentage of positive (PE) and the cognitive load was measured by 5 eye-movement measures including total fixation duration (TFD), number of fixations (NF), and average fixation duration (AFD), percentage of viewing time (PVT) and frequency of saccade path (FSP). A one-way ANOVA was used to examine the differences between traditional static e-learning and DGBL. The results are shown in Table 13.

Table 13.ANOVA Analysis of physiology signals between Static and DGBL Learner

Static (n=16) DGBL (n=16) ANOVA

AT = attention score, PE = occupied percentage of positive, TFD = total fixation duration, NF = number of fixations, AFD = average fixation duration, PVT = percentage of viewing time, FSP = frequency of saccade path.

*** p < .001

A one-way ANOVA showed that there were significant differences in total fixation duration (TFD), number of fixations (NF), and average fixation duration (AFD) and percentage of viewing time (PVT) across the two learning environment. (FTFD (1,30) = 45.297 , p<0.001 ; FNF

(1,30) = 105.966 , p<0.001 ; FAFD (1,30) = 61.247 , p<0.001 ; FPVT(1,30) = 58.333 , p<0.001 .) For attention score (AT), occupied percentage of positive (PE), frequency of saccade path (FSP) and sum of saccade paths (SSP), we did not find significant differences.

4.1.2 Finding

Hypotheses 1 is rejected

According attention (AT) score, DGBL group had better attention score than traditional static e-learning group, but we did not find significant differences. Huang et al. (2010) observed ARCS scores by questionnaire and found that learners started out with a successful motivational processing that consisted of a high attention level. In addition, Tüzün, Yılmaz-Soylu, Karakuş, İnal, and Kızılkaya (2009) found that students demonstrated statistically significant higher intrinsic motivations and statistically significant lower extrinsic motivations learning in the game-based environment. This suggests that many studies proved that learners got better attention and learning motivation in DGBL environment by questionnaire, but we couldn’t prove this argument by the proof of affective computing technique.

Hypothesis 2 is rejected

According occupied percentage of positive (PE) score, DGBL group didn’t have better affective experiences than traditional static e-learning group. Clark et al. (2011) found that majority of the eighth and ninth grade students in Taiwan and United States liked their affective experiences using SURGE game. SURGE game is a serious educational game and not a commercial game, so maybe college student couldn’t feel funny like eighth and ninth grade students.

Hypothesis 3 is supported

The cognitive load was measured by 5 eye-movement measures. There were found the significant differences in fixation duration (TFD), number of fixations (NF), and average fixation duration (AFD), percentage of viewing time (PVT). In TFD and NF data, the result showed that DGBL group less than Static e-learning group. But in AFD and PVT data, DGBL group more than Static e-learning group. Because a parts of learner completed all stages less than 10 min, based on the different learning time for each learner, this study used AFD and PVT to measure

the cognitive load. Therefore, this suggests that DGBL group have more cognitive load than traditional static e-learning group and have significant difference.

4.2 DGBL group have better academic achievement but have no significant differences

Force Concept Inventory (FCI) test score were used as the pre-test: 32.08 (SD = 12.64) and 33.13 (SD = 11.51) for the static e-learning group and DGBL group. In addition to the pre-test, a post-test was also conducted, with mean scores of 43.75 (SD = 25.00) and 50.00 (SD = 31.62) for the static e-learning group and DGBL group, respectively. The pre-test and post-test score between two learning environment (static e-learning vs. DGBL) was showed in Figure 19.

Post-test score was analyzed ANCOVA, in which learning environment (static e-learning vs. DGBL) was the between-groups factor, and pre-test scores were treated as covariates in order to control for the effects of pre-existing between-group differences on subsequent analysis.

Before ANCOVA analysis was conducted, the Kolmogorov–Smirnova method was used to test the normality of academic achievement data in this study; a non-significant result (p < .001) indicated that the data were non-normally distributed. Additionally, a test for homogeneity of variance was conducted, with the result revealing no significant effect, F (1, 30) = .091, p = .765;

that is, the data meet the requirement for homogeneity of variance. There were no significant effect be found for learning environment, F (1, 30) = .349, p = .559, and is presented in Table 14.

Pre-test Post-test

Figure 19.The pre-test and post-test measurement between two learning environment

Vogel, Greenwood-Ericksen, Cannon-Bowers, and Bowers (2006) reported that interactive games were more effective than traditional classroom instruction on learners’ academic learning gains and cognitive skill development. As the results in this study, we found that no differences in student learning can be found between learning environments that involve games and those without game elements (Leonard A. Annetta, Minogue, Holmes, & Cheng, 2009; Papastergiou, 2009). Thus, DGBL group have better academic achievement but have no significant differences.

Hypothesis 2 is rejected.

Table 14.Analysis of covariance for academic achievement

SS df MS F p

Covariance Pre-test 174.58 1 174.58 .209 .651

Between-groups Learning environment 291.49 1 291.49 .349 .559

Within-group Error 24200.42 29 834.49

4.3 High working memory capacity learning style group knew better where to look the key factors.

4.3.1 Active vs. reflective

Attention distributions on each LookZone

To analyze the attention distributions on different look-zones between learning style (active vs. reflective), 4 eye movement measures were used, including percentage of time spent in zone (PTSZ), fixation count (FC), percentage of total fixations (PTF), percentage of time fixated related to total fixation duration (PTFRTFD).

In problem 1, one-way ANOVA showed that there were significant differences in LookZone

“Text” (FPTSZ (1,30) = 9.956 , p<.01 ; FPTF (1,30) = 5.148 , p<.05 ; FPTFRTFD (1,30) = 12.124 ,

p<.01), LookZone “4” (F

FC (1,30) = 7.163 , p<.05 ; FPTF (1,30) = 5.987 , p<.05), LookZone “F”

(FPTSZ (1,30) = 5.119 , p<.05 ; FFC (1,30) = 6.409 , p<.05 ; FPTF (1,30) = 8.453 , p<.01 ; FPTFRTFD

(1,30) = 4.816 , p<.05) and LookZone “correct” (FPTSZ (1,30) = 6.762 , p<.05 ; FPTF (1,30) = 8.453 , p<.01 ; FPTFRTFD (1,30) = 4.816 , p<.05). The one-way ANOVA results are shown in Table 15. The significant differences LookZone between active and reflective are shown in Figure 20, the red area represent the LookZone was found significant differences between active and reflective.

In problem 2, one-way ANOVA showed that there were significant differences in LookZone

“K” (FPTSZ (1,30) = 40.060 , p<.001 ; FFC (1,30) = 105.043 , p<.001 ; FPTF (1,30) = 92.396 ,

p<.001; F

PTFRTFD (1,30) = 40.427 , p<.001). The one-way ANOVA results are shown in Table 16.

The significant differences LookZone between active and reflective are shown in Figure 21, the

4

F

Correct Incorrect

Incorrect Text

Figure 20.Significant differences LookZone between active and reflective group (problem 1).

The dotted area represent the LookZone was found significant differences and reflective see more than active.

Table 15.Visual attention distributions between active and reflective group (problem 1)

LookZone Measures Active (n=16) Reflective (n=16) ANOVA

LookZone: Text = problem description, 4 and F = key factor, correct = correct option 2 and 3, incorrect = incorrect option 1 and 5; Measures: PTSZ = percentage of time spent in zone, FC = fixation count, PTF = percentage of total fixation, PTFRTFD = percentage of time fixated related to total fixation duration.

* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01

Correct Text

Incorrect

Figure 21.Significant differences LookZone between active and reflective group (problem 2).

The dotted area represent the LookZone was found significant differences between active and reflective.

Table 16.Visual attention distributions between active and reflective group (problem 2)

LookZone Measures Active (n=16) Reflective (n=16) ANOVA

Hot Zone image

In problem 1, we found the HotZone image for active group showed that the group paid the more attention on text, especially on factor 4. In problem 2, we found the HotZone image for active group showed that the group paid the less attention on text and option, but paid the more attention on factor K.

Figure 22.HotZone image between active and reflective group (problem 1)

Figure 23.HotZone image for active and reflective (problem 2)

4.3.2 Sensing vs. intuitive

Attention distributions on each LookZone

In problem 1, the one-way ANOVA results are shown in

Table 17. There are no significant differences be found between sensing group and intuitive group. In problem 2, one-way ANOVA showed that that is a significant difference in LookZone

“incorrect” (FPTSZ (1,30) = 40.060 , p<.001 ; FFC (1,30) = 105.043 , p<.001 ; FPTF (1,30) = 92.396 , p<.001; FPTFRTFD (1,30) = 40.427 , p<.001). The one-way ANOVA results are shown in Table 18. The significant differences LookZone between sensing and intuitive are shown in Figure 24, the red area represent the LookZone was found significant differences between sensing and intuitive.

Table 17.Visual attention distributions between sensing and intuitive group (problem 1)

LookZone Measures Sensing (n=17) Intuitive (n=15) ANOVA

Correct Text

Incorrect

Figure 24 Significant differences LookZone between sensing and intuitive group (problem 2).

The dotted area represent the LookZone was found significant differences between sensing and intuitive.

Table 18.Visual attention distributions between sensing and intuitive group (problem 2)

LookZone Measures Sensing (n=17) Intuitive (n=15) ANOVA

LookZone: Text = problem description, N, W, F and K = key factor, correct = correct option 3, incorrect = incorrect option 1, 2, 4 and 5; Measures: PTSZ = percentage of time spent in zone, FC = fixation count, PTF = percentage of total fixation, PTFRTFD = percentage of time fixated related to total fixation duration.

Hot Zone image

In problem 1, we found the HotZone image for sensing group showed that the group paid the more attention on factor 5 and F. In problem 2, we found the HotZone image for intuitive group showed that the group paid the more attention on factor N.

Figure 25.HotZone image between sensing and intuitive group (problem 1)

4.3.3 Sequential vs. global

Attention distributions on each LookZone

To analyze the attention distributions on different look-zones between learning style (active vs. reflective), 4 eye movement measures were used, including percentage of time spent in zone (PTSZ), fixation count (FC), percentage of total fixations (PTF), percentage of time fixated related to total fixation duration (PTFRTFD). There are no significant differences be found between sensing group and intuitive group in problem 1 and 2. The one-way ANOVA results in problem 1 are shown in Table 19 and the results in problem 2 are shown in Table 20.

Table 19.Visual attention distributions between sequential and global group (problem 1)

LookZone Measures Sequential (n=15) Global (n=17) ANOVA

LookZone: Text = problem description, 4 and F = key factor, correct = correct option 2 and 3, incorrect = incorrect option 1 and 5; Measures: PTSZ = percentage of time spent in zone, FC = fixation count, PTF = percentage of total fixation, PTFRTFD = percentage of time fixated related to total fixation duration.

Table 20.Visual attention distributions between sequential and global group (problem 2)

LookZone: Text = problem description, N, W, F and K = key factor, correct = correct option 3, incorrect = incorrect option 1, 2, 4 and 5; Measures: PTSZ = percentage of time spent in zone, FC = fixation count, PTF = percentage of total fixation, PTFRTFD = percentage of time fixated related to total fixation duration.

Hot Zone image

In problem 1, we found the HotZone image for sequential group showed that the group paid the more attention on factor 4, 5 and F. In problem 2, we found the HotZone image for sequential group showed that the group paid the more attention on factor F.

Figure 27.HotZone image between sequential and global group (problem 1)

Figure 28.HotZone image between sequential and global group (problem 2)

4.3.4 Finding

According the result of visual attention distributions between high working memory capacity (WMC) learning style and low WMC learning style group, hypotheses 1 in PAHSE 2 was verified as follow.

Hypotheses 1 is supported.

The results showed in Table 21. In problem 1, we found that high WMC group paid more visual attention in LookZone “Text”, “4” and “F”, but paid less in LookZone “correct”. In problem 2, low WMC group paid more visual attention in LookZone “K” and “incorrect”. This suggested that high WMC group have more cognitive resources (Daneman & Carpenter, 1980;

Mayer, 2001), so they paid more visual attention not only in key factors but also in text. In problem 2, the key for solving this problem is the relationship between “N” and “W”, but low WMC group paid more visual attention in the factor “K”. For high and low WMC group, the concept of “K” was be comprehended. Thus, this study found that high working memory capacity learning style group knew better where to look the key factors than low working memory capacity learning style group.

Table 21.Visual attention distributions differences between two WMC groups.

Problem LookZone Visual attention distributions

P1 Problem description P2 Key factor everyone know

Incorrect option

High WMC group < Low WMC group High WMC group < Low WMC group

4.4 DGBL group knew better where to look the key factors 4.4.1 Attention distributions on each LookZone

To analyze the attention distributions on different look-zones between learning environment (DGBL vs. Static e-learning), 4 eye movement measures were used, including percentage of time spent in zone (PTSZ), fixation count (FC), percentage of total fixations (PTF), percentage of time fixated related to total fixation duration (PTFRTFD).

In problem 1, A one-way ANOVA showed that there was significant differences in LookZone “correct” (FPTSZ (1,30) = 13.831 , p=.001 ; FFC (1,30) = 8.212 , p<.01 ; FPTF (1,30) = 25.849 , p<.001 ; FPTFRTFD (1,30) = 22.402 , p<.001). The one-way ANOVA results are shown in Table 22. The significant differences LookZone between DGBL and Static e-learning are shown in Figure 29. The red area represent the LookZone was found significant differences between DGBL and Static e-learning.

In problem 2, A one-way ANOVA showed that there was significant differences in LookZone “N” (FPTSZ (1,30) = 11.690 , p<.01 ; FFC (1,30) = 6.955 , p<.05 ; FPTF (1,30) = 14.824 ,

p<.01 ; F

PTFRTFD (1,30) = 16.683 , p<.001). The one-way ANOVA results are shown in Table 23.

The significant differences LookZone between DGBL and Static e-learning are shown in Figure 30. The red area represent the LookZone was found significant differences between DGBL and Static e-learning.

4

F

Correct Incorrect

Incorrect Text

Figure 29.Significant differences LookZone between DGBL and Static e-learning (problem 1)

The dotted area represent the LookZone was found significant differences between DGBL and Static e-learning.

Table 22.Visual attention distributions between two learning environment (problem 1)

LookZone Measures DGBL (n=16) Statice (n=16) ANOVA

LookZone: Text = problem description, 4 and F = key factor, correct = correct option 2 and 3, incorrect = incorrect option 1 and 5; Measures: PTSZ = percentage of time spent in zone, FC = fixation count, PTF = percentage of total fixation, PTFRTFD = percentage of time fixated related to total fixation duration.

*** p < .001

Correct Text

Incorrect

Figure 30Significant differences LookZone between DGBL and Static e-learning (problem 2)

Figure 30Significant differences LookZone between DGBL and Static e-learning (problem 2)