• 沒有找到結果。

on incidental learning of vocabulary. After the results and discussions of the study

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "on incidental learning of vocabulary. After the results and discussions of the study "

Copied!
8
0
0

加載中.... (立即查看全文)

全文

(1)

CHAPTER FIVE

CONCLUSIONS

This study investigates effects of exposure frequency and contextual situations

on incidental learning of vocabulary. After the results and discussions of the study

have been presented in Chapter Four, this chapter covers a summary of major findings,

pedagogical implications for the EFL context in Taiwan, limitations of this study and

suggestions for future research.

5.1 Summary of the Findings

EFL senior high school students on a controlled setup in this study can acquire at

least receptive knowledge of vocabulary incidentally by reading texts for meaning and

demonstrate impressive retention for at least one month after receiving repeated

written exposures to the target words. In terms of the strength of word gains and

retention, word exposure frequency still plays an important role. As the number of

word exposures increased, there is a growing sign of word learning among the

learners. When words occur six times in the designated passages, vocabulary

acquisition is significantly observed; however, when words occur less than six times,

word gains are not significant and sometimes are even unpredictable. Those

unpredictable gains may result from the contamination of guesswork in the data,

(2)

forgetting as well as unlearning of words, and temporary declining performance

during lexical restructuring. Meanwhile, similar learning percentage of words under

four and six exposures is found between the present study and Rott (1999). From the

similarity, the study proposes four ranges of learning percentage for words under four

and six exposures and suggests contextual situations might be one of the factors

affecting how large these ranges are. In addition, although there is no

overwhelmingly significant difference in vocabulary learning between the two

contextual situations within each exposure frequency, it is speculated that reading

passages with inferential clues have the potential to facilitate learners at the formal

operational stage in vocabulary learning more and to reduce the rate of word meaning

forgetting to a greater degree. The advantages of inferential clues over explicit ones

are due to the possibility that processing words embedded in the former requires more

mental effort while target words embedded in the latter tend to be ignored. Finally,

although there is no statistically significant interaction between exposure frequency

and contextual situations, it is speculated that an interrelationship still exists between

these two factors in facilitating incidental vocabulary learning.

5.2 Pedagogical Implications for the EFL Context in Taiwan

Most Taiwanese students are used to learning new words by asking for an

accurate definition first. Even though the unknown words are well-defined in the

(3)

context, they would rather interrupt their reading either by looking them up in the

dictionary or by waiting for teachers’ explanations before they restore their confidence

in finishing the reading. Without doing so, they seem to be afraid that they can

neither fully comprehend the text nor increase their vocabulary size. Although

Goodman (1982) states “efficient reading does not result from precise perception and

identification of all elements, but from skill in selecting the fewest, most productive

cues necessary to produce guesses which are right the first time” (p. 34), it is still

common for teachers to preteach vocabulary in a reading text because they know it is

students’ familiar learning pattern and believe in the efficiency of such learning.

However, senior high school students should not abandon their inferring ability

to learn vocabulary because the study reveals that it is possible for them to learn

words incidentally from reading short and comprehensible texts. Interestingly, clues

requiring more inferring ability tend to facilitate students’ vocabulary acquisition

better than those surrounded with explicit definitions or synonyms. In other words,

giving students word definitions directly may deprive them of the opportunities to

deeply process the word meaning and lead to their inattentive browses at the target

words. Therefore, teachers should encourage students to infer word meaning from

context and design tasks that can enhance students’ mental processing of target words.

Considering EFL learners might have difficulty making use of contextual clues

(4)

especially in the early stage, teachers can guide them to start from words with familiar

concepts (Pulido, 2003; Ryder, 1986). Scholars also caution that authentic context

does not reliably assist learners in acquiring word meaning (Beck, McKeown, &

McCaslin, 1983; Laufer, 1997) and that incidental word learning will not happen if

learners can not infer word meanings from texts beyond their comprehensibility

(Ghadirian, 2002; Nation, 2001; Nation & Waring, 1997; Tran, 2006), so the

importance of text selections in promoting incidental learning can never be neglected

(Cho & Krashen, 1994). Additionally, when selecting textbooks for teaching context

skills, teachers can bear Slawson’s (1991) suggestions in mind: (a) introductions to

context units need better pictures to elicit students’ preexisting competence, (b) the

classification of contextual cuing systems should be expanded to a broader extent so

that students could develop their own cues, (c) processes and strategies for deriving

word meanings should be emphasized, (d) better guided practice should be provided

to serve as a bridge to content area materials, (e) questions for practice should be

generative and enhance students’ involvement with the text, and (f) context skills

should be integrated to help learners build a strong vocabulary and then improve

reading comprehension.

In addition, it is advisable for textbook writers to pay attention to the effects of

the repetitions of the target words and the suitability of contextual clues on learners’

(5)

incidental vocabulary learning. If clues for the target words are inferable, six

encounters of the words at least may beget significant vocabulary learning.

Moreover, the strength of learning will increase if contextual clues are mentally

demanding enough for the intended readers. Therefore, when selecting target words

for language use, writers can take these two factors into consideration.

Finally, like the approaches to vocabulary development proposed in many studies

(Brabham & Villaume, 2002; Horst, Cobb, & Meara, 1998; Jenkins, Matlock &

Slocum, 1989; Nation, 1982; Nation, 2001; Oxford & Scarcella, 1994; Paribakht &

Wesche, 1997; Prince, 1996; Pressley, 2002; Tekman & Daloglu, 2006; Tran, 2006;

Zahar, Cobb, & Spada, 2001), effective learning cannot just have one orientation.

Although the findings of the study support the advantages of incidental learning of

vocabulary, the aim of the study does not ask teachers to devote all the class time to

having students acquire words incidentally and to totally discard other teaching

methods such as direct instruction in helping students build massive vocabulary.

Instead, it still treats incidental learning as one of the methods complementary to the

entire language course. What needs to be stressed is that we should tell students they

have the potential to acquire words from exposures and have them experience how to

pick up words by reading in class, for studies indicate that one can improve the skill

of incidental word learning over time and practice or instruction of the necessary skill

(6)

will be expected to transfer to students’ ability to handle unknown words from context

(Buikema & Graves, 1993; Fraser, 1999; Goerss, Beck, & McKeown, 1999; Jenkins,

Matlock, & Slocum, 1989; Swanborn & de Glopper, 1999). After students accept

incidental learning as a means of enlarging their vocabulary and know how to acquire

language this way, they will enjoy the benefits of applying such experience in playing

Goodman’s (1982) ‘psycholinguistic guessing game’ when learning independently.

5.3 Limitations of the Study

Although words exposed six times are acquired significantly better than those

under any other exposure frequency, there are few significant differences among

words learned under exposures less than six and the two contextual situations also

make unsatisfactory statistical differences. Such nonsignificant results might be due

to the insufficient number of words used for each exposure frequency. Considering

the limited class time during the treatment sessions, the study could only test three

target words for each exposure frequency. In addition, to arrange a schedule for each

exposure frequency to have equal intervals, the study gave participants treatments

twice in a day most of the time during the sessions. Such intensive treatments in a

day might make participants tired of the treatments and less patient while they were

working on the required tasks. If more words had been used in each exposure

frequency and a less intensive treatment schedule had been implemented, there might

(7)

have been more significant differences found among words learned under exposures

less than six and between the two contextual situations.

5.4 Suggestions for Future Research

The study only adopts three target words without any typographical input

enhancement

6

in each exposure frequency so if future studies can cover more words

and make these words more visually salient, maybe there will be more significant

differences found between word exposures fewer than six times and between

contextual situations. In addition, although the types of contextual situations within

each exposure frequency in the study do not constitute a significantly strong factor in

obtaining incidental increments of word knowledge, clue characteristics are still worth

investigating so other types of contextual situations can be tested in a similar

experiment design to see if the results will be different. Moreover, this study has

focused on how freshmen in a senior high school in northern Taiwan learn words

incidentally so future studies can recruit a different target pool of participants or

participants across different age groups and proficiency levels to determine whether

the conclusions here can be generalized to other students and to see if there is any

other significant difference. Additionally, since the study conducts the experiment

6

Kim (2006) defines typographical input enhancement as “use of an attention-getting

or flagging device to highlight unfamiliar words in the text” (p. 347).

(8)

by asking students to read short unrelated passages of contrived texts with easy

content comprehensibility, such a design, as discussed in Rott (1999), might lack

intensive learner-text interaction during word inferencing process that could possibly

engender the delayed effect of word gain found in Knight (1994). Therefore, it is

worth investigating whether learners have the same or even better performance when

reading an authentic literary text with a richer content and a related theme such as a

novel or a follow-up news report in the future. Furthermore, the present study only

measures receptive word knowledge in incidental learning through reading without

involving any oral input or language production, so an identical study on listening or

on productive vocabulary knowledge can be replicated. Finally, even though the

study fails to discover an statistically significant interaction between exposure

frequency and contextual situations in incidental learning of vocabulary, future

research can delve into experiments using higher exposure frequency to see if such

interactions exist.

參考文獻

相關文件

Comparison of B2 auto with B2 150 x B1 100 constrains signal frequency dependence, independent of foreground projections If dust, expect little cross-correlation. If

Achievement growth in children with learning difficulties in mathematics: Findings of a two-year longitudinal study... Designing vocabulary instructio n

Basing on the observation and assessment results, this study analyzes and discusses the effects and problems of learning the polynomial derivatives on different level students

The difference resulted from the co- existence of two kinds of words in Buddhist scriptures a foreign words in which di- syllabic words are dominant, and most of them are the

• elearning pilot scheme (Four True Light Schools): WIFI construction, iPad procurement, elearning school visit and teacher training, English starts the elearning lesson.. 2012 •

DVDs, Podcasts, language teaching software, video games, and even foreign- language music and music videos can provide positive and fun associations with the language for

• involves teaching how to connect the sounds with letters or groups of letters (e.g., the sound /k/ can be represented by c, k, ck or ch spellings) and teaching students to

Microphone and 600 ohm line conduits shall be mechanically and electrically connected to receptacle boxes and electrically grounded to the audio system ground point.. Lines in