• 沒有找到結果。

The Effect of Different Techniques of Vocabulary Instruction on Fifth Graders’ Vocabulary Learning and Retention

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "The Effect of Different Techniques of Vocabulary Instruction on Fifth Graders’ Vocabulary Learning and Retention"

Copied!
16
0
0

加載中.... (立即查看全文)

全文

(1)

The Effect of Different Techniques of Vocabulary Instruction on

Fifth Graders’ Vocabulary Learning and Retention

Tsae-Tzy Wang(王彩姿) & Jia-Wen Yu(游佳文)

National Pingtung University of Education

Abstract

The purpose of the present study aims to investigate the effect of different techniques of vocabulary instruction on EFL elementary school students’ vocabulary learning and retention so as to improve their learning and retention of English vocabulary.

Ninety-two fifth graders from an elementary school in Pingtung City participated in this study and all of them received the three techniques of vocabulary instruction, which were visual aids, contextual inference, and semantic mapping. The researchers of the present study designed a pretest to divide the subjects into high-, mid-, and low-level students. They also gave the subjects a new word test to determine the thirty target words for later instruction. To evaluate the subjects’ performance, they employed self-made tests for both the immediate and the retained recall tests to find out how well the three different techniques of vocabulary instruction facilitated the students’ vocabulary learning and retention. Besides, they gave the students a questionnaire to understand the students’ beliefs about the three different techniques of vocabulary instruction. The students’ scores on the recall tests were analyzed using the descriptive statistics, repeated measures, and pairwise comparisons of SPSS and the students’ responses to the questionnaire were interpreted in terms of percentage.

The results of recall tests showed that visual aids facilitated the students’ vocabulary learning most, followed by semantic mapping and contextual inference, irrespective of proficiency levels of the students. However, semantic mapping assisted the high- and low-level students in retaining the new words best, whereas visual aids helped the mid-level students most. Moreover, the results of the questionnaire indicated that most students considered both visual aids and semantic mapping helpful for their vocabulary learning.

(2)

INTRODUCTION

Second language learners realize that their limited repertoire of vocabulary knowledge greatly affect their ability to convey their intended meaning in

communications using the target language (Read, 2004). Moreover, Gass and Selinker (2008) found that lexical errors were the most common errors committed by second language learners based on the large corpora of errors. It seemed that vocabulary learning was vital to success in second language learning.

Yet, in Taiwan where the first language is not English, children begin to be exposed to English vocabulary at a later age. They are also less likely to be exposed to extensive oral English at home and in their neighborhood, and therefore their English vocabulary development may be affected by their lack of opportunity. As Oxford and Crookall (1990) pointed out, “ courses on reading, writing, speaking, listening, grammar, and culture are common in L2 programs, but very few

vocabulary courses exist” (cited in Sanaoui, 1995, p.25). Moreover, Moody (1982) found that very few textbooks bother to teach vocabulary systematically. In other words, most authors of textbooks deemphasize the role of vocabulary in English language learning, which poses more obstacles for students learning English vocabulary.

With the intention of improving Taiwanese learners’ vocabulary learning, the researchers would like to find out which technique of vocabulary instruction is the most effective, especially for EFL children. Moreover, the researchers would like to investigate the effectiveness of the three different techniques of vocabulary

instruction for different English proficiency levels. Furthermore, the researchers are eager to find out the retention effects of the different techniques of vocabulary instruction on these students.

Accordingly, the researchers propose four research questions and list them as follows:

1. Of the three techniques of vocabulary instruction used in this study, which one would help students most in vocabulary learning?

2. Is students’ performance on recall tests consistent with their evaluations of the usefulness of the three different techniques of vocabulary instruction?

3. Which technique is the most effective for high-, mid- and low-level students respectively?

4. Do these three different techniques of vocabulary instruction have the same retention effect on students of different proficiencies?

LITERATURE REVIEW

Factors Affecting Vocabulary Learning and Retention

Many researchers proposed various factors affecting vocabulary learning which can be organized and classified into three main categories─ teachers’ teaching methods, students’ responsibility for their own learning, and the nature of foreign words.

First, Schmitt (2000) maintained that teaching the pairs of antonyms (e.g.,

deep/shallow, long/short, and rich/poor) posed great learning difficulties for learners,

(3)

meanings, such a teaching method could not help students distinguish one word (long) from the other (short) and make correct connections between form and meanings.

Second, Sanaoui (1995) claimed that learners who had a structured learning approach were more successful in L2 vocabulary learning than those who had an unstructured learning approach. This means that good learners planned for their vocabulary learning, used a variety of vocabulary learning strategies, found the semantic relationships between newly learned and previously learned L2 words, self-created opportunities for learning L2 words outside classrooms, and often reviewed and practiced learned L2 words; however, poor learners generally lacked active management of their L2 vocabulary learning.

Third, the foreign language vocabulary itself affects the success in foreign words learning. Ellis and Beaton (1993) proposed that the less the overlap between the native and foreign language, the harder it would be for the FL learners to learn the foreign language vocabulary. For example, English is an intonation language, whereas Mandarin Chinese is a tone language. Hence, when English speakers learn Mandarin Chinese, they would encounter great difficulties in perceiving and distinguishing the five distinctive tones with the same segmental value but contrasting pitches or tones (Nation, 1990). That is to say, if the form ma was pronounced with a falling pitch, it means ‘scold’, but when the same form is pronounced with a rising pitch, the meaning is ‘hemp’ (O’Grady, Dobrovolsky & Katamba, 1997).

As for the retention effect of foreign language words, some researchers suggested the following influential factors. First, Schouten-van Parreren (1989) proposed that learners who were engaged in activities which involved a deep processing of words retained the words well. Similarly, Cameron (2001) contended that “the immediate translation of a new word takes away from learners any need or motivation to think about the meaning of the foreign language word or to hold the new word in mind” (p.85). Second, Gairns and Redman (1986) claimed that items which occurred most frequently were easily retained and retrieved, as these items were stored at the top of the memory system.

The Visual Aids on Vocabulary Learning

Gairns and Redman (1986) claimed that the most general visual aids that were displayed in the language classrooms included flashcards, photographs, blackboard drawings, wall charts, and realia and that other forms of visual aids such as mimes and gestures were often used to supplement other ways of conveying meanings. Goodman (1987) proposed that for the most part, visuals were more easily

understood by learners of all ages in that a picture seemed worth a thousand words and avoided lengthy and difficult word definitions. Mayer and Sims (1994) also claimed that when learners made connections between verbal and visual

representations of words by the use of visual aids, such learning was enhanced on the basis of dual coding theory, proposed by Paivio.

The Contextual Inference on Vocabulary Learning

(4)

that context clues were signals to help readers to infer the appropriate meaning of an unknown word contained in the text and these clues included definitions, examples, restatements, punctuations like dashes which were followed by a more detailed clarification for a preceding word or phrase, and demonstrative articles such as this and that which provided additional information for the antecedent.

Carter (1987) proposed that “the more advanced learners are, the more likely they are to benefit from learning words in context” (p. 169). Moreover, Decarrico (2001) maintained that the context itself must be rich enough to allow readers to make correct guesses of the word meaning. In this sense, a single context was often not enough for readers to think up the right meaning of an unknown word contained in the context; therefore, repeated encounters with the new word in diverse contexts

were needed.

The Semantic Mapping on Vocabulary Learning

Aitchison (1987) proposed that the human mental lexicon was believed to be a network where semantically related words were associated with each other. One particular form of semantically related instruction is semantic mapping. According to Blachowicz and Fisher (2000), “semantic mapping is a technique that graphically represents the relationships between words and requires students to identify and understand the relations between words” (p.505). Nichols and Rupley (2004) also proposed that even in a whole class setting, the teacher could allow students to brainstorm words that came to mind when they heard the new vocabulary word. For high-level students, the brainstorming activity provides the opportunity to activate their background knowledge around the new term and to make associations between the new word and already known words. For low-level students, it provides them with an opportunity to begin to reconstruct their own conceptual system in which the new term will be an additional component.

Toms-Bronowski (1983) conducted an experimental study with fourth to sixth graders to investigate the results of vocabulary learning through semantic mapping, semantic feature analysis, and contextual analysis and found that students in the semantic groups outperformed those in the contextual group on the immediate and retention tests of words (cited in Heimlich and Pittelman, 1986).

METHODOLOGY

Experimental Design

Having Considered the fact that there were three intact groups as subjects and three different techniques of vocabulary instruction in this experimental study, a 3х3 Latin Squares (LS-3) design was used to allow the three intact groups to receive the three different techniques of vocabulary instruction at different time periods.

More specifically, when teaching vocabulary from 1 to 10, the researchers of this study used Visual Aids in Class 1, Contextual Inference in Class 2, and

Semantic Mapping in Class 3; when teaching vocabulary from 11 to 20, the

researchers employed Contextual Inference in Class 1, Semantic Mapping in Class 2, and Visual Aids in Class 3; and when teaching vocabulary from 21 to 30, the

(5)

Subjects

The subjects of this study were students from three classes of fifth grade of an elementary school in Ping-Tung City. The total number of the students is 92 in this study: 29 students from Class 1, 32 students from Class 2, and 31 students from Class 3. Students in this elementary school started learning English when they were at grade three, and thus they had learned English for more than two years. After reviewing the school’s textbooks used for fourth and fifth graders, the researchers of this study found that students only knew a small amount of vocabulary and some simple sentence patterns using the present tense; however, students did not have the same English proficiency level.

Instruments and Materials

Before the pilot study, the researchers of this study gave students a pretest to divide them into three different proficiency groups and a new word test to pick out thirty target words for later instruction.

The pretest was composed of twenty lexical items. It is worth mentioning that eighty percent of the lexical items were extracted from those words learned at this elementary school and twenty percent of the lexical items were from other versions of textbooks such as Kang-Hsuan, Nan-I, and Joy so as to improve the

discriminability of the pretest.

The new word test was designed to pick out thirty target words for vocabulary instruction. To select thirty target words which are neither too easy nor too difficult for fifth graders, two times the number of the target words, that is, sixty words were temporarily selected. Excluding those words that had been taught during the

previous semesters, the researchers selected sixty words from the two thousand basic English words to be the testing items of the new word test. All items were expected to be unfamiliar to most subjects. Boote (2006) proposed that if less than forty percent of children knew what a given word meant, then we might not want to spend time teaching it. Similarly, if more than eighty percent of children knew what a word meant, we might not want to spend time teaching it, either. In other words, the words worth teaching to the whole class then were the words that scored between forty and eighty percent. Accordingly, the researchers computed the percentage of each of the sixty words. After deleting those which were either too easy or too difficult, thirty words were selected for the 92 fifth graders as to-be-learned words.

Regarding the immediate recall and retained recall tests which were used in the formal study, each immediate recall test containing ten lexical items was conducted to understand the extent to which the three different techniques of

vocabulary instruction facilitate vocabulary learning for Taiwanese fifth graders. As for the delayed test, it was conducted to know which technique would lead to a better retention effect of new words and contained the same testing items as in the immediate recall test.

Procedures

Through the new word test, the researchers came up with the thirty

(6)

interfering variable.

In the pilot study, the participants were also fifth graders from three other classes (Classes 4, 5, and 6) of the same elementary school. Through the pilot study, the researchers found that the level of difficulty of the three sets of to-be-learned words was not significantly different in that F (2,89)=1.545, p=.219>.05 (see Table 1).

Table 1

The Results of One-Way ANOVA for the Immediate Recall Test in the Pilot Study

Source SS df MS F Sig.

Between Groups 627.373 2 313.686 1.545 .219

Within Groups 18068.280 89 203.014

Total 18695.652 91

According to the experimental design, three conditions of vocabulary instruction in the formal study are as follows.

Procedure of condition 1 (visual aids)

One of the researchers in this study asked students to repeat twice the new English words after her while looking at the researcher-made flashcards. Then, after shuffling the flashcards, she requested them to read out each new word by

themselves. To have a solid form-image association of each word to be taught, she engaged students in the activity where one student acted out the meaning of a new word in front of the whole class and other students guessed which word was being performed. After this activity, students reviewed all the new words by looking at the flashcards. At last, she gave students an immediate recall test to find out students’ performance using the technique of visual aids. Two weeks later, the same set of words was tested again in the delayed test.

Procedure of condition 2 (contextual inference)

The researcher first distributed to all students a learning sheet listing three related example sentences for each target word. Chinese definitions of other non-target words expected to be unfamiliar to students were also given in the learning sheet. The researcher first read out the unfamiliar non-target words and asked students to repeat after her. And then, students listened to the three related example sentences read out slowly by the researcher while looking at their own learning sheet. After introducing the example sentences, the researcher encouraged students to make guesses at the target word by looking at the meanings of its

surrounding words. She gave feedback to their intelligent guesses and then provided them with the most appropriate meaning of the target word. To review the newly learned word, students along with the researcher read out the related example sentences again. Finally, an immediate recall test was given to evaluate students’ learning of the new words using the technique of contextual inference. Two weeks later, they would take a delayed test with the same testing items.

(7)

The researcher told students the Chinese definitions of the new words after writing them on the blackboard. Then, using each new word as a stimulus, the researcher asked students to brainstorm three related words by virtue of their common sense to build a semantic mapping of the new word. After constructing the semantic maps of the words to be learned, students reviewed the semantic

relationships of the new word to other related words in each semantic web. To give students more practice with the newly learned words, she asked them to participate in the drill where they memorized and read out the entire semantic map of each new word after she gradually erased one word after another. Finally, she gave them an immediate recall test to probe the effectiveness of the technique of semantic

mapping. Besides, they would have a delayed test after two weeks.

Data Analysis

There were two types of data (i.e., the quantitative and qualitative data) collected in this study. The quantitative data were dealt with using the statistical software SPSS to show the effects of the three different techniques of vocabulary instruction. To find out which technique of vocabulary instruction would help students most in vocabulary learning, the statistical analysis of one-way ANOVA repeated measures was employed to compare the main effects of the three different techniques of vocabulary instruction. Moreover, to investigate which technique would be most effective for low-, mid-, and high-level students, the two-way (3Х3) ANOVA mixed design was used to analyze the data. Besides, to discover the retention effects of the three different techniques of vocabulary instruction on students of different proficiencies, the two-way (3Х3) ANOVA mixed design was run again. Regarding the qualitative data, the results of the questionnaire were analyzed in terms of percentage.

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

The Effect of the Three Different Techniques of Vocabulary Instruction With regard to the first research question ‘Of the three techniques of

vocabulary instruction used in this study, which one would help students most in vocabulary learning?’ the researchers would like to know the main effects of the

three different techniques of vocabulary instruction. Table 2 showed that the three different techniques of vocabulary instruction were significantly different, as

F(2,182)=12.297, p=.000<.05.

Table 2

The Summary of One-Way ANOVA Repeated-Measures Design

Source SS df MS F Sig.

Technique 6182.609 2 3091.304 12.297 .000

Subject 110702.536 91 1216.511

Error 45750.725 182 251.378

(8)

When the three different techniques of vocabulary instruction were

significantly different, it was necessary to further understand which technique was the most effective for EFL elementary school students. The results showed that visual aids had the highest mean score of 90.5435, semantic mapping resulted in the second high mean score of 87.9348, and contextual inference received the lowest mean score of 79.4565, as illustrated in Table 3. As a result, visual aids facilitated fifth graders’ vocabulary learning most; on the contrary, the contextual inference was regarded as the least effective for the fifth graders. It is conceivable that with limited linguistic knowledge, it would be difficult for the fifth graders to benefit from the technique of contextual inference in learning new words. Carter (1987) also maintained that learning words from contexts was subject to the relatively high proficiency level of learners.

Table 3

Descriptive Statistics of the Three Different Techniques of Vocabulary Instruction

Technique N Mean Std. Deviation

Visual Aids 92 90.5435 19.96503

Contextual Inference 92 79.4565 29.32816

Semantic Mapping 92 87.9348 21.45980

Furthermore, after doing the pairwise comparison of the three different techniques of vocabulary instruction, the researchers discovered that the technique of contextual inference was significantly different from the other two techniques of vocabulary instruction in terms of effectiveness, since the mean difference between visual aids and contextual inference was 11.087, which was statistically significant (p=.000<.05) and the mean difference between the semantic mapping and contextual inference was 8.478, which achieved the statistical significance as well (p=.001<.05), as demonstrated in Table 4. The results of this study also showed that there was no significant difference between visual aids and semantic mapping for EFL elementary school students, for the mean difference=2.609, p=.234.

Table 4

Pairwise Comparisons of the Three Different Techniques of Vocabulary Instruction

95% Confidence Interval for Difference (I) Technique (J) Technique Mean Difference (I-J) Std.

Error Sig. Upper

Bound Lower Bound 2 11.087* 2.470 .000 6.180 15.994 1 3 2.609 2.180 .234 -1.721 6.938 1 -11.087* 2.470 .000 -15.994 -6.180 2 3 -8.478* 2.354 .001 -13.154 -3.803 1 -2.609 2.180 .234 -6.938 1.721 3 2 8.478* 2.354 .001 3.803 13.154

Note. (1) technique 1=Visual Aids; technique 2=Contextual Inference; technique 3=Semantic

Mapping

(9)

The Analysis of the Students’ Evaluation of the Usefulness of the Three Different Techniques of Vocabulary Instruction

To answer the second research question ‘Is students’ performance on recall

tests consistent with their evaluations of the usefulness of the three different techniques of vocabulary instruction?’ the researchers analyzed the students’

responses to the question ‘Do you think the technique of vocabulary instruction used

in this class helped you learn the new word more effortlessly?’ For each technique,

the same question was asked repeatedly in the three classes (Classes 1, 2, and 3) receiving the same technique of vocabulary instruction at different points of time.

Regarding visual aids, the results of analysis showed that 17.4 percent of the students rated visual aids as very helpful, 64.1 percent of the students considered visual aids helpful, 14.1 percent of the students saw visual aids as little helpful, and 4.3 percent of the students evaluated visual aids as helpless (see Figure 1). In sum, 81.5 percent of the students believed that visual aids facilitated their learning of new English words. helpful 59 64.1% helpless 4 4.3% little helpful 13 14.1% very helpful 16 17.4% helpless little helpful helpful very helpful

Figure 1. Students’ Evaluation of the Usefulness of Visual Aids

As for contextual inference, the researchers discovered that 7.6 percent of the

(10)

helpless 42 45.7% little helpful 30 32.6% very helpful 7 7.6% helpful 13 14.1% helpless little helpful helpful very helpful

Figure 2. Students’ Evaluation of the Usefulness of Contextual Inference

With regard to the usefulness of semantic mapping, the researchers found that 15.2 percent of the students regarded semantic mapping as very helpful, 46.7 percent of the students felt semantic mapping helpful, 23.9 percent of the students thought of semantic mapping as little helpful, and 14.1 percent of the students assessed

semantic mapping as helpless (see Figure 3). In a word, 61.9 percent of the students thought that semantic mapping improved their learning of English words.

helpful 43 46.7% helpless 13 14.1% little helpful 22 23.9% very helpful 14 15.2% helpless little helpful helpful very helpful

Figure 3. Students’ Evaluation of the Usefulness of Semantic Mapping

(11)

The Effect of the Three Different Techniques of Vocabulary Instruction on Students of Different Proficiencies

To answer the third research question ‘Which technique is the most effective

for high-, mid- and low-level students respectively?’ the researchers employed the

two-way (3x3) ANOVA mixed design to find out the conditions under which high-, mid-, low- students would have a better vocabulary learning. Table 5 demonstrated that the vocabulary learning of high-, mid-, and low-level students were significantly different under the three conditions of vocabulary instruction (F (2,89)=38.028,

p=.000<.05) and that the main effects of the three different techniques of vocabulary

instruction on fifth graders’ vocabulary learning were also significantly different (F (2, 178)=12.711, p=.000<.05). However, there was no interaction effect between proficiency levels and the three different techniques of vocabulary instruction on fifth graders’ vocabulary learning in that F (4,178)=.523, p=.719>.05.

Table 5

The Summary of Two-Way ANOVA Mixed Design

Source SS df MS F Sig. (Between-Subjects Effects) Level 17003.536 2 8501.768 38.028 .000 Error 19897.309 89 223.565 (Within-Subjects Effects) Technique 6458.318 2 3229.159 12.711 .000 Technique * Level 531.541 4 132.885 .523 .719 Error 45219.183 178 254.040 Total 89109.887 275

Among the three techniques of vocabulary instruction, the technique of visual aids had the highest means (the unweighted mean=89.326), followed by semantic mapping (the unweighted mean=86.591) and contextual inference (the unweighted mean=77.797), as shown in Table 6.

Table 6

Descriptive Statistics of the Three Different Techniques of Vocabulary Instruction Using the Unequal Sample Sizes of the Three Groups

Technique N (LL:26+ML:39+HL:27) Mean SD Visual Aids 92 89.326 19.96503 Contextual Inference 92 77.797 29.32816 Semantic Mapping 92 86.591 21.45980

Moreover, when compared with each other, the results showed that contextual inference was significantly different from visual aids (the mean difference=11.529,

p=.000<.05) and from semantic mapping (the mean difference=8.794, p=.000<.05);

(12)

Table 7

Pairwise Comparisons of the Three Different Techniques of Vocabulary Instruction

95% Confidence Interval for Difference (I) Technique (J) Technique Mean Difference (I-J) Std.

Error Sig.(a) Upper Bound Lower Bound 2 11.529* 2.521 .000 6.520 16.538 1 3 2.735 2.226 .222 -1.688 7.159 1 -11.529* 2.521 .000 -16.538 -6.520 2 3 -8.794* 2.412 .000 -13.586 -4.002 1 -2.735 2.226 .222 -7.159 1.688 3 2 8.794* 2.412 .000 4.002 13.586

Note. (1) technique 1=Visual Aids; technique 2=Contextual Inference; technique 3=Semantic

Mapping

(2) * The mean difference is significant at the .05 level.

In the following Table, it was found that the high-level students obtained the same highest mean score of 99.2593 under both visual aids and semantic mapping and the second high mean score of 88.5185 under contextual inference. For the mid-level students, they got the highest mean score of 97.1795 under visual aids, the second high mean score of 95.1282 under semantic mapping, and the lowest mean score of 88.7179 under contextual inference. With regard to the low-level students, they received the highest mean score of 71.5385 under visual aids, the second high mean score of 65.3846 under semantic mapping, and the lowest mean score of 56.1538 under contextual inference. In conclusion, low-, mid-, and high-level subjects all benefited from visual aids most, followed by semantic mapping and contextual inference.

Table 8

Descriptive Statistics of Low-, Mid-, High-Level Subjects’ Learning Performance Using the Three Different Techniques of Vocabulary Instruction

Technique Group N Mean SD

Visual Aids LL subjects 26 71.5385 29.07814

ML subjects 39 97.1795 6.46803

HL subjects 27 99.2593 3.84900

Contextual Inference LL subjects 26 56.1538 34.41824

ML subjects 39 88.7179 19.49082

HL subjects 27 88.5185 23.64866

Semantic Mapping LL subjects 26 65.3846 27.45626

ML subjects 39 95.1282 10.48101

HL subjects 27 99.2593 2.66880

The Retention Effect of the Three Different Techniques of Vocabulary Instruction on the Students of Different Proficiencies

Regarding the fourth research question ‘Do these three different techniques of

(13)

proficiencies?’ the researchers employed the two-way ANOVA mixed design to

investigate the degree to which the three different techniques of vocabulary

instruction helped high-, mid-, and low-level students in retaining the newly learned foreign words. The findings were that the main effects of the three different

techniques of vocabulary instruction on fifth graders’ vocabulary retention were not significantly different (F (2, 178)=1.986, p=.140>.05). Additionally, the interaction effect between proficiency levels and the three different techniques of vocabulary instruction on fifth graders’ vocabulary retention was also insignificant, since F (4,178)=.833, p=.506>.05 (see Table 9).

Table 9

The Summary of Two-Way ANOVA Mixed Design

Source SS df MS F Sig. (Between-Subjects Effects) Level 33193.667 2 16596.833 31.748 .000 Error 46525.657 89 522.760 (Within-Subjects Effects) Technique 1514.652 2 757.326 1.986 .140 Technique* Level 1270.288 4 317.572 .833 .506 Error 67880.437 178 381.351 Total 150384.701 275

As for the average retention scores of high-, mid-, and low-level students using the three different techniques of vocabulary instruction, Table 10

demonstrated that twenty seven high-level students had the mean retention score of 85.432, thirty nine mid-level students obtained the mean retention score of 77.863, and twenty six low-level students got the mean retention score of 39.359. If we further compared the mean score obtained in the immediate recall test with that in the retained recall test in the three groups, we observed that for the low-level group, their mean score dropped abruptly from 64.359 in the immediate recall test to 39.359 in the retained recall test (losing 25 points) ; for the mid-level group, their mean score dropped from 93.675 to 77.863 ( losing 15.812 points) ; and for the high-level group, their mean score dropped from 95.679 to 85.432 (losing 10.247 points). This signified that the lower the proficiency level of students, the quicker they tended to forget the words learned two weeks ago.

Table 10

The Average Performance of Low-, Mid-, High-Level Subjects on the Immediate Recall Tests and Retained Recall Tests

Immediate Recall Tests Retained Recall Tests Test

Group N Mean SD Mean SD

(14)

retention score of 41.9231 under semantic mapping, the second high mean retention score of 41.5385 under visual aids, and the lowest mean retention score of 34.6154 under contextual inference; for mid-level students, they had the highest mean

retention score of 82.5641 under visual aids, the second high mean retention score of 75.8974 under contextual inference, and the lowest mean retention score of 75.1282 under semantic mapping; for high-level students, they got the highest mean retention score of 88.8889 under semantic mapping, the second high mean retention score of 85.1852 under visual aids, and the lowest mean retention score of 82.2222 under contextual inference. It was clear that for both low- and high-level students, semantic mapping was the best technique in retaining new foreign words for a period of two weeks, followed by visual aids and contextual inference. However, for mid-level students, the technique of visual aids was more useful for vocabulary retention than contextual inference and semantic mapping.

Table 11

Descriptive Statistics of Low-, Mid-, High-Level Subjects’ Retention Scores using the Three Different Techniques of Vocabulary Instruction

Technique Group N Mean SD

Visual Aids LL subjects 26 41.5385 34.48969

ML subjects 39 82.5641 23.58835

HL subjects 27 85.1852 18.88637

Contextual Inference LL subjects 26 34.6154 37.01143

ML subjects 39 75.8974 25.41425

HL subjects 27 82.2222 25.31848

Semantic Mapping LL subjects 26 41.9231 38.47277

ML subjects 39 75.1282 26.83986

HL subjects 27 88.8889 16.94637

CONCLUSIONS AND SUGGESTIONS

Conclusions

Concerning the first research question ‘Of the three techniques of vocabulary

instruction used in this study, which one would help students most in vocabulary learning? the researchers would like to conclude that based on the students’

performance on the immediate recall tests, visual aids helped students most in vocabulary learning, followed by semantic mapping and contextual inference. Moreover, contextual inference was significantly ineffective compared with visual aids and semantic mapping for fifth graders’ vocabulary learning.

With regard to the second research question ‘Is students’ performance on

recall tests consistent with their evaluations of the usefulness of the three different techniques of vocabulary instruction?’ the researchers would like to conclude that

most students had positive evaluations on visual aids and semantic mapping and somewhat negative evaluations on contextual inference based on their responses to the question ‘Do you think the technique of vocabulary instruction used in this class

helped you learn the new word more effortlessly?’

Regarding the third research question ‘Which technique is the most effective

(15)

conclude that the effectiveness of the three different techniques of vocabulary instruction seemed unrelated to the proficiency levels of the students, since all of the high-, mid-, and low-level students, in learning foreign words, benefited from visual aids most, from semantic mapping secondly, and from contextual inference least.

As for the fourth research question ‘Do these three different techniques of

vocabulary instruction have the same retention effect on students of different proficiencies?’ the researchers would like to conclude that the three different

techniques of vocabulary instruction had different retention effects on students of different proficiencies. More specifically, high- and low-level students, on the retained recall tests, got the highest scores using semantic mapping, followed by visual aids and contextual inference. However, this is not the case for mid-level students. Visual aids not only helped them learn the foreign English words most but also helped them to retain those words for a longer period of time. Moreover,

contextual inference also enhanced mid-level students’ memory of the new words in that they got better scores on the retained recall tests under contextual inference than they did under semantic mapping.

Limitations and Suggestions for Further Study

There are some limitations in this study. First, the subjects are fifth graders from an elementary school in Pingtung City. The proficiency levels of the subjects in this study do not represent the proficiencies of students living in other urban or rural areas. Consequently, the results of this study could not be generalized to all

elementary school students in Taiwan. For further study, the researchers suggest that future researchers recruit students from different regions in Taiwan to minimize the sampling errors. Second, the nature of the thirty target words should be examined more carefully in the future study to avoid the fact that some words might be easier to learn using a certain technique while others might be easier to learn using another, which has nothing to do with the technique itself. Third, the researchers suggest that future researchers use a larger sample size to make the results more convincing, that is, to increase statistical testing power (1-β).

REFERENCES

Aitchison, J. (1987). Words in the Mind: An Introduction to the Mental Lexicon. Oxford and New York: Basil Blackwell.

Blachowicz, C. L. Z., & Fisher, P. (2000). Vocabulary instruction. In M. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of Reading Research,

Volume III. Mahwah, N.J. : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Boote, C. (2006). Vocabulary: Reasons to teach it, an effective teaching method, an words worth teaching. The NERA Journal, 42(2), 24-28.

Cameron, L. (2001). Teaching Language to Young learners. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Carter, R. (1987). Vocabulary: Applied Linguistic Perspectives. London: Allen & Unwin.

Decarrico, J. S. (2001). Vocabulary learning and teaching. In M. Celce-Murcia (Ed.),

Teaching English as a Second or Foreign Language (3rd ed.). Boston, MA:

(16)

Ellis, N. C., & Beaton, A. (1993). Psycholinguistic determinants of foreign language vocabulary learning. Language Learning, 43(4), 559-617.

Gairns, R., & Redman, S. (1986). Working with Words: A Guide to Teaching and

Learning Vocabulary. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Gass, S. M., & Selinker, L. (2008). Second Language Acquisition: An Introductory Course (3rd ed.). New York: Taylor & Francis.

Goodman, L. S. (1987). Training aids. In R. Craig (Ed.), Training and Development (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

Heimlich, J. E., & Pittelman, S. D. (1986). Semantic Mapping: Classroom

Applications. Newark, Delaware: International Reading Association.

Mayer, R. E., & Sims, V. A. (1994). For whom is a picture worth a thousand words? Extensions of a dual-coding theory of multi-media learning. Journal of

Educational Psychology, 86, 389-401.

Moody, R. (1982). Scheduling drills and teaching vocabulary. Hispania, 65(4), 609-614.

Nation, I. S. P. (1990). Teaching and Learning Vocabulary. New York: Heinle & Heinle Publishers.

Nattinger, J. (1988). Some current trends in vocabulary teaching. In R. Carter & M. McCarthy (Eds.), Vocabulary and Language Teaching. London: Longman. Nichols, W. D., & Rupley, W. H. (2004). Matching instructional design with

vocabulary instruction. Reading Horizons, 45(1), 55-71.

O’Grady, W., Dobrovolsky, M., & Katamba, F. (1997). Contemporary Linguistics:

An Introduction. United Kingdom: Copp Clark Pitman Ltd.

Read, J. (2004). Research in teaching vocabulary. Annual Review of Applied

Linguistics, 24, 146-161.

Sanaoui, R. (1995). Adult learners’ approaches to learning vocabulary in second languages. The Modern Language Journal, 79(1), 15-28.

Schmitt, N. (2000). Vocabulary in Language Teaching. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Schouten-van Parreren, C. (1989). Vocabulary learning through reading: Which conditions should be met when presenting words in texts? AILA Review, 6, 75-85.

數據

Figure 1. Students’ Evaluation of the Usefulness of Visual Aids
Figure 2. Students’ Evaluation of the Usefulness of Contextual Inference

參考文獻

相關文件

volume suppressed mass: (TeV) 2 /M P ∼ 10 −4 eV → mm range can be experimentally tested for any number of extra dimensions - Light U(1) gauge bosons: no derivative couplings. =&gt;

For pedagogical purposes, let us start consideration from a simple one-dimensional (1D) system, where electrons are confined to a chain parallel to the x axis. As it is well known

The observed small neutrino masses strongly suggest the presence of super heavy Majorana neutrinos N. Out-of-thermal equilibrium processes may be easily realized around the

incapable to extract any quantities from QCD, nor to tackle the most interesting physics, namely, the spontaneously chiral symmetry breaking and the color confinement.. 

(1) Determine a hypersurface on which matching condition is given.. (2) Determine a

• Formation of massive primordial stars as origin of objects in the early universe. • Supernova explosions might be visible to the most

The difference resulted from the co- existence of two kinds of words in Buddhist scriptures a foreign words in which di- syllabic words are dominant, and most of them are the

• elearning pilot scheme (Four True Light Schools): WIFI construction, iPad procurement, elearning school visit and teacher training, English starts the elearning lesson.. 2012 •