• 沒有找到結果。

Gender differences in online reading engagement, metacognitive strategies, navigation skills and reading literacy

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Gender differences in online reading engagement, metacognitive strategies, navigation skills and reading literacy"

Copied!
20
0
0

加載中.... (立即查看全文)

全文

(1)

Gender differences in online reading

engagement, metacognitive strategies,

navigation skills and reading literacy

J-Y. Wu

Institute of Education, National Chiao Tung University, Taiwan

Abstract This study examined how knowledge of metacognitive strategies and navigation skills mediate the relationship between online reading activities and printed reading assessment (PRA) and electronic reading assessment (ERA) across 19 countries using the PISA (Programme for International Student Assessment) 2009 database. Participants were 34 104 fifteen-year-old students (female: 50.1%). The results showed that information-seeking activity, knowledge of metacognitive strategies and navigations skills positively predicted ERA and PRA. Social reading activities negatively predicted knowledge of metacognitive strategies and PRA but had no effect on ERA and the navigation skills in most countries. Increased information-seeking reading resulted in higher ERA and PRA as demonstrated by navigation skills and knowledge of metacognitive strategies. Gender differences in online reading engagement were not statistically significant in most countries. However, girls performed better in knowledge of metacognitive strategies, navigation skills and PRA but were not significantly better on ERA. Multiple group comparisons of gender indicated that the hypothesized model held for both boys and girls. Besides the infra-structure of information and communications technology as a tool to access the cyber informational space, students should be empowered to use appropriate strategies and navi-gation skills to achieve their goals. Implications for teaching and learning practices are discussed.

Keywords gender difference, metacognitive strategy, navigation skill, reading engagement, PISA, reading literacy.

Since 2000, the Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) has conducted 3-year cycles of assessment of students’ reading, math-ematics and science literacy through the Programme

for International Student Assessment (PISA). The pre-dicative power of PISA scores on academic and career outcomes has been documented (Knighton & Bussiere, 2006; OECD, 2007) and used to inform educational reform.

In 2009, the main focus of PISA was assessment of reading literacy, which is an essential skill for people to acquire new knowledge and survive in an increasingly global, information-based world. Based on the results Accepted: 4 December 2013

Correspondence: Jiun-Yu Wu, National Chiao Tung University, Insti-tute of Education, 1001 Ta-Hsueh Rd., Hsinchu 300, Taiwan. Email: jiunyu.rms@gmail.com

(2)

of previous PISA cycles, girls have been found to dem-onstrate a consistent advantage in reading ability over boys (Chiu & McBride-Chang, 2006; Lietz, 2006); moreover, the overrepresentation of boys in the reading underperformance group has become an issue requir-ing closer attention (OECD, 2011a). In the e-learnrequir-ing era, the concept of reading literacy has evolved and expanded to include the ability to read both paper- and web-based materials. Acknowledging the importance of both forms of reading, PISA administered printed reading assessments (PRAs) along with electronic reading assessments (ERAs) in 2009.

What mechanism will help explain reading literacy in the digital era? Will the gender gap still exist in ERA? And will gender moderate the relationship between related factors (such as various online reading activities, navigation skills and metacognitive strat-egies) and reading literacy? These are the questions that drove the current study. Gender differences in PRA are a worthwhile pursuit in order to find a possible solution to closing the gender gap, so is an investiga-tion in ERA.

Printed vs. electronic reading

Printed reading and electronic reading share several reading strategies, such as planning/goal setting, reread-ing, monitorreread-ing, evaluating and correcting (Akyel & Erçetin, 2009; Coiro & Dobler, 2007; Winne, 1995). Research shows that instruction in metacognitive strat-egies and metacognitive awareness can help students learn to read better (Boulware-Gooden, Carreker, Thornhill, & Joshi, 2007). Thus, a positive correlation has been found between knowledge of metacognitive strategies and reading literacy (Artelt, Schiefele, & Schneider, 2001).

However, besides metacognitive awareness and prior knowledge of the reading content, in a web-based envi-ronment, readers also need knowledge of the website structure as well as the search engine in order to be able to read and glean meaning (Coiro & Dobler, 2007). These navigation skills are pivotal to Internet reading because hyperlinks and search functions are the unique features of reading in a digital environment; however, at the same time, they are the cause of non-linearity, which often leads to learner distraction, disorientation and shallow reading (Akyel & Erçetin, 2009; Birkerts, 2006; Liu, 2005; Mangen, 2008).

Analysing the reading pattern of skilled sixth graders, Coiro and Dobler (2007) proposed a recursive cycle of reading pattern in a web-based open environ-ment. The cycle consisted of four elements: plan, pre-diction, monitor and evaluation. Readers should have a goal and build a mental model at first, predict where the link will lead, monitor after an action is taken and evaluate the pertinence of the decision (Coiro & Dobler, 2007). Although this four-part reading cycle is similar to that used when reading paper-based materials, the predicting, monitoring and evaluating parts focus on the uncertainty of what readers will end up with when they make a decision rather than where the author of the book (or paper-based material) will lead them. As such, electronic reading capitalizes on individual differences in navigational skills, which involve ‘constantly making a decision on how to proceed while reading, and monitoring of this process’ (Akyel & Erçetin, 2009, p. 145), and are a reflection of metacognitive strategies specific to web-based reading.

Online reading activities and reading literacy

Historically, active engagement in reading has been positively associated with reading literacy (Froiland & Oros, 2013). In the digital age, different types of reading activities have emerged and can be broadly categorized into social and information seeking (OECD, 2011b). That is, people use different reading strategies based on the types of reading activities they engage in, which, in turn, have distinct influences on their reading in either the printed and digital environment.

Social reading activities, such as online chatting, have been found to have an adverse effect on learning due to its nature to make people distracted (Bowman, Levine, Waite, & Gendron, 2010; Fox, Rosen, & Crawford, 2009; Junco & Cotten, 2011). In contrast, information-seeking activities that involve constant decision making and monitoring resemble serious reading tasks and are beneficial to reading comprehen-sion (Coiro & Dobler, 2007; Lee & Wu, 2012, 2013). Gender differences in reading literacy and the essen-tial factors that affect reading ability (e.g., engagement in online reading, knowledge of metacognitive strat-egies and navigation skills) as well as the mutual rela-tionship among these factors form the foundation of the current research.

(3)

Purpose of the study

In a preliminary analysis of the PISA database, we found a zero-order correlation of 0.85 on ERA and PRA for the 19 participating countries. The correlation between the two variables, when converted to the concept of variance explained, showed a shared vari-ance between ERA and PRA of 72%. Our motivation behind the present study was to explore possible factors that can explain the common variances in ERA and PRA and discover possible mediators that account for the relationship between the various online reading activities and ERA/PRA. As mentioned previously, navigation skills are specific to online reading environ-ment (Akyel & Erçetin, 2009), whereas metacognitive strategies are universal to both printed and electronic reading (Coiro & Dobler, 2007). Therefore, we used metacognitive strategies, specifically self-regulated learning (Pintrich & De Groot, 1990), as the theoretical framework for examining how knowledge of meta-cognitive strategies and online navigation skills mediate the relationship between online reading activ-ities and PRA and ERA across 19 countries using the PISA 2009 database.

The current study was designed to answer the fol-lowing research questions using structural equation modeling in an overall model as well as in individual country models:

1. What are the direct effects of social reading activ-ities on knowledge of metacognitive strategies, navigation skills, and ERA and PRA, controlling for socio-economic status and gender?

2. What are the direct effects of information-seeking reading activities on knowledge of metacognitive strategies, navigation skills, and ERA and PRA, controlling for socio-economic status and gender? 3. What are the direct effects of knowledge of

metacognitive strategies on ERA and PRA, control-ling for socio-economic status and gender? 4. What are the direct effects of navigation skills on

ERA and PRA, controlling for socio-economic status and gender?

5. What are the indirect effects of social reading activ-ities on ERA and PRA, as demonstrated by knowl-edge of metacognitive strategies and navigation skills, controlling for socio-economic status and gender?

6. What are the indirect effects of information-seeking reading activities on ERA and PRA, as demon-strated by knowledge of metacognitive strategies

and navigation skills, controlling for

socio-economic status and gender?

Besides testing the hypothesized relationships, we also examined the moderating effect of gender on online reading engagement, knowledge of metaco-gnitive strategies, navigation skills, and PRA and ERA by asking:

7. What is the gender moderation effect on online reading engagement, knowledge of metacognitive strategies, navigation skills, and ERA and PRA, holding other variables constant?

The hypothesized model is exhibited in Figure 1.

Literature review

Instead of merely describing the relationship between different online activities and PRA/ERA, we explored possible mediators that explained the mechanism behind the relationship and examined moderating effects on the essential variables that influence reading. Specifically, we investigated the interplay of navigation strategies and knowledge of metacognitive strategies on the relationship between various online reading activities and reading literacy across 19 countries. Figure 1 Hypothesized Mediation Model

Note. Inform= Information-Seeking Online Reading Activities;

Social= Social Online Reading Activities; Meta = Perceived

Use-fulness of Metacognitive Strategies; Navi= Navigation Skills;

PRA= Printed Reading Literacy; ERA = Electronic Reading

Lit-eracy. The Effects of Gender (Male= 1, Female = 0) and ESCS on

(4)

The following is a review of the literature related to metacognitive strategies and reading literacy, naviga-tion skills and reading literacy, and the gender modera-tion on these factors.

Metacognitive strategies and reading literacy

Metacognitive strategies are internal psychological processes that influence readers’ reading comprehen-sion and are central to self-regulated learning pro-cesses. Metacognition involves the state of being aware of one’s thinking along with the control and regula-tion of one’s cognitive behaviours (Flavell, 1979; Zimmerman, 2002).

Results of empirical studies have indicated that pro-ficient readers exert appropriate metacognitive strat-egies (Lau, 2006; Lau & Chan, 2003). Specifically, good readers monitor and evaluate the reading process and regulate reading methods to achieve their reading purposes. The importance of metacognitive strategies is also evident in the digital reading environment (Akyel & Erçetin, 2009; Stadtler & Bromme, 2007). For example, Jairam and Kiewra (2010) found that students who used the full SOAR (select, organize, associate and regulate) strategies had better online reading scores than those in the control or partial-strategy groups. In addition, skilled readers performed better in monitoring their comprehension of questions and information searching during online task-oriented reading assignments (Vidal-Abarca, Mañá, & Gil, 2010).

Navigation strategies and reading literacy

In order to read efficiently in a digital environment, readers need to travel across the nodes in the online texts effectively, which requires proficient navigation strategies to access, organize and integrate multiple sources of information. These strategies were referred to as ‘advanced navigation strategies’ to distinguish them from the basic navigation strategies pertaining to website structure (OECD, 2011b).

At the centre of advanced navigation processes are frequent self-monitoring and self-regulating of one’s reading comprehension, which includes the ability to decide the next move that will lead to relevant information as well as to access, integrate and evaluate

information from various web pages (Naumann, Richter, Christmann, & Groeben, 2008; Salmerón & García, 2011).

Researchers have studied navigation skills in relation to online reading in terms of navigation types (Barab, Bowdish, & Lawless, 1997; Bousbia, Rebaï, Labat, & Balla, 2010), link selection criteria (Salmerón, Kintsch, & Caãs, 2006a, 2006b), and reading models that contain navigation and other factors (Naumann et al., 2008; Salmerón & García, 2011).

For the first category, Lawless, Brown, Mills, and Mayall (2003) commented that three online reader pro-files can generally be identified: knowledge seekers, feature explorers and apathetic users. Knowledge seekers read the online documents in pursuit of the content. The feature explorers spent more time inter-acting with multimedia to know how things work than with the content. The apathetic users were unmotivated readers who only did a limited amount of web page exploration.

For the second category, Salmerón et al. (2006a) found that the coherence of text representation was related with reading comprehension based on the construction-integration model (Kintsch, 1988). Further, low-knowledge readers following the coher-ence strategy had better reading comprehension than those following the interest strategy, but no such effect was found among intermediate-knowledge readers (Salmerón et al., 2006b).

Regarding the third category, Naumann et al. (2008) revealed that navigation skills mediated the relation-ship between learning strategy training and learning outcome. Students with high reading ability or working memory benefited from strategy training as demon-strated by better navigation behaviour and learning out-comes, but this effect was not found in students with low reading ability or working memory. Based on the assumption of coherence strategy, Salmerón and García (2011) showed that reading skills predicted one’s navigation strategy measured in terms of the cohesion of navigation path, which in turn predicted reading performance.

The current study followed up on the third category to explore the mediation effects of knowledge of metacognitive strategies and navigation skills between various online reading activities and ERA and PRA. Besides investigating the general pattern, the 19 indi-vidual models from participating Asian, European,

(5)

South American and Oceanian countries were exam-ined to check their congruency with the general pattern.

Gender effect on literacy, metacognitive strategies, online engagement and navigation skills

Gender differences in reading have been found consist-ently over the past decades and across geographical regions (for a review, see Lietz, 2006). For example, using the 2003 PISA data, Chiu and McBride-Chang (2006) found that girls had better reading performance than boys across 41 countries. Evidence from four lon-gitudinal epidemiology studies also revealed that the per cent of reading disability was higher in boys with or without taking IQ into account (Carroll, 2004). Further, in OECD countries, the gender gap in reading literacy was especially noticeable in the underperformance group, where one in eight girls performed below the baseline proficiency level while one in four boys per-formed below that level (OECD, 2011a).

Metacognition makes a unique contribution in pro-cessing difficult texts beyond intelligence (Veenman & Beishuizen, 2004). Gender differences in choice of metacognitive strategies may help explain girls’ advan-tages in reading comprehension. Girls reported more metacognitive strategy use than boys during reading (Sheorey & Mokhtari, 2001) and scored higher on comprehension monitoring (Kolic´-Vehovec & Bajšanski, 2006). However, some research has sug-gested that even though girls had higher scores in metacognition than boys, the difference was not statis-tically significant (Roeschl-Heils, Schneider, & van Kraayenoord, 2003). This study revisited the gender difference in knowledge of metacognitive strategies using the large-scale, high-quality PISA data.

With regard to online engagement, results on gender differences were mixed. For example, no gender dif-ference was found in e-mail use, chatting online, web surfing or information search in a sample of 340 Greek high school students aged 12–16 (Papastergiou & Solomonidou, 2005). Nevertheless, Chen and Fu (2009) found that eighth-grade girls reported using the Internet more often for both online searching and chat-ting than boys, even though the frequency of Internet use for girls was significantly lower than for boys in a sample of 1409 Taiwanese adolescents. Likewise, Tsai and Tsai (2010) found that boys had greater intensity of Internet use than girls; however, boys tended to use the

Internet for explorative purposes whereas girls tended to use it for communicative purpose. The current study examined gender differences in online engagement of social reading and information-seeking activities.

As for navigation skills, gender differences may be examined in search patterns, actual skills or self-reported measures. Analysing eighth graders’ search behaviours, Roy and Chi (2003) found that more boys than girls preferred horizontal moves by fast scanning information at an early reading stage, whereas more girls than boys preferred vertical moves by reading in a linear sequence between documents. However, stu-dents with high knowledge gains preferred horizontal moves regardless of gender. Early scanning on the web may reflect students’ evaluation strategies to determine whether information is relevant to the reading topic and is essential to reading. As for self-reported measures, in a sample of 324 high school students, Tsai (2009) evaluated boys and girls’ search strategies in the behav-ioural, procedural and metacognitive domains. No gender difference was found in the metacognitive strat-egy domain but a significant male advantage was found in Internet control and procedural strategies. Thus, even though men usually have higher self-assessed web skills, their actual search results may not be statistically different from women’s (Hargittai & Shafer, 2006).

As illustrated, the literature on gender difference in navigation revealed mixed results. Therefore, the current study examined this issue by using the centred number of relevant page visited (OECD, 2011b) as an objective measure of navigation skills.

Method Sample

The study used the PISA 2009 dataset for analysis. PISA 2009 utilized two-stage stratified sampling scheme to collect data. In the first stage, schools with probabilities proportionate to the number of 15-year-old students within the schools were selected from a sampling frame in a comprehensive national school list. A minimum of 150 schools were chosen in each of the participating countries. In the second stage, a random sample of 35 students was selected within par-ticipating schools. A total of 107 394 students from 19 countries and regions (Korea, Japan, Hong Kong-China, Macao-Kong-China, New Zealand, Australia, Ireland, Iceland, Sweden, Norway, Belgium, Denmark, France,

(6)

Spain, Poland, Hungary, Austria, Chile and Colombia) participated in both the PRA and ERA.

Among the 107 394 students, navigation data were recorded in the log file for only 34 104 students on the ERA. Therefore, only observations with available navi-gation data were included in the current analysis, resulting in a sample of 34 104 students (female: 50.1%).

Materials and instruments PRA

The PRAs were designed to assess multiple aspects of reading skills, which may be divided into two groups: reading process and text composition. The reading process included three aspects: accessing and retrieving, integrating and interpreting, reflecting and evaluating. The text composition consisted of two aspects: continu-ous texts and non-continucontinu-ous texts. Continucontinu-ous texts are organized from sentences, paragraphs, to sections. Examples of non-continuous texts are lists, tables, graphs, diagrams, advertisements and schedules.

We used a combined scale reading score with all five aspects to evaluate student reading literacy. The PISA-combined scale had an average score of 500 and a standard deviation of 100.

ERA

Like the PRA, the ERA also focused on the three reading processes: accessing and retrieving, integrating and interpreting, reflecting and evaluating. The ERA simulates the online reading situation, which requires students to use their Internet control skills, such as clicking on a particular link or replying a comment on the discussion forum, to search for particular informa-tion or to explore the links that are relevant to the reading topic. For example, in accessing and retrieving, students may need to retrieve an answer to the question from a specific web page without navigating to other pages. In integrating and interpreting, students may need to integrate information on different web pages to develop an explanation. In reflecting and evaluating, students may consult an additional web document that is authoritative and trustworthy to support their prefer-ences on one suggestion over another.

An item pool of 29 digital reading tasks formed three reading clusters. Each student was administered a 40-min ERA, randomly drawn from two of the three

clusters. The scores on the ERAs and PRAs were equated with the means within the countries or regions so that the results could be compared. The mean ERA score was 499, with a standard deviation of 90 for the current sample.

Social and information online reading activities Students’ online activities may be divided into social reading activities and information-seeking reading activities. According to OECD (2009a), the former include reading e-mail and chatting online; the latter include reading online news, using online dictionaries

or encyclopaedia (e.g., Wikipedia®

), searching online information to learn about a particular topic or search-ing for practical information online. The online readsearch-ing activities were evaluated in terms of frequency of use, ranging from 1 (I don’t know what it is) to 5 (several times a day).

Knowledge of metacognitive strategies

Knowledge of metacognitive strategies consisted of two metacognitive strategy index variables. The index of summarizing (Metasum) and the index of under-standing and remembering (Undrem) emphasize readers’ abilities to monitor, evaluate and integrate reading materials. The following is a sample item for Metasum: ‘I read through the text, underlining the most important sentences, and then I write them in my own words as a summary.’ Sample item for Undrem is: ‘I concentrate on the parts of the text that are easy to understand.’

Students were asked to rate the usefulness of strat-egies within each index variable. Experts also rated the usefulness of the strategies. Higher scores on the indices meant that students’ ratings agreed more with the experts’ ratings; namely, better knowledge of the metacognitive strategies.

Navigation skills

The measure of navigation skills was number of visits to relevant pages recorded in the computer log file when students were taking the ERA. Twenty-nine digital reading tasks were organized into three clusters, and students received two of the three clusters ran-domly. Therefore, students would not respond to the same set of digital reading items. To account for the effect of reading assessment orders and test composi-tions, we followed the reporting practice of the PISA

(7)

and used the centred number of relevant pages visited, which is calculated with equal weights to the OECD countries per test and then subtracted from individual students’ values, as the indicator of navigation skills in this study.

The ESCS (economic, social and cultural status) covariate and the gender moderator

We controlled students’ socio-economic status (ESCS) and tested the gender-moderating effect for knowledge of metacognitive strategies, navigation skills, and ERA and PRA in the hypothesized mediation model. The state of socio-economic status has been a dominating factor in student outcomes (White, 1982). This study controlled for socio-economic status with a focus on other variables of interest.

Data analysis

The hypothesized model was tested with mediation analysis (MacKinnon, 2008) using Mplus 6.11 (Muthén & Muthén, 2010). To calculate indirect effects of social reading and information-seeking reading activities on PRA and ERA scores, we utilized the Sobel (1982) test along with the delta method to test statistical significance (Krull & MacKinnon, 1999, 2001). All data analysis procedures were conducted following the suggestions of OECD (2009b). We used

sampling weight and replicate weight to correct for biased parameter estimates and their standardized error estimates due to the two-stage stratified sampling schemes (Wu & Kwok, 2012). The plausible values were also used to approximate each participant’s true score and ability.

Model fit was determined using root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA; Steiger, 1998) and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR; Hu & Bentler, 1999). The model fit chi-square test and related model fit indices are not provided because they were not available for an analysis employing replicate weights. A reversed model with all paths pointing in opposite directions from the original model was tested to examine if the reverse of hypothesized relationship existed. Besides testing the moderator effect of gender on the mediators, online engagement, PRA and ERA, which resembles t-test or mean gender differences, we also conducted a multigroup comparison to determine if the hypothesized model was statistically similar for boys and girls.

Results

This section presents the result for the overall and individual country mediation models. The correlations among the observed variables are presented in Table 1. As illustrated, the proposed mediation model exhibited

adequate model fit with overall data (RMSEA= .063

Table 1. Zero-Order Correlation Among Items, Indices and Observed Variables

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 1 ESCS – 2 PRA .368** – 3 ERA .405** .850** – 4 Social01 .200** .114** .140** – 5 Social02 .121** −.013* .038** .393** – 6 Inform01 .107** .096** .110** .311** .342** – 7 Inform02 .172** .221** .201** .282** .253** .423** – 8 Inform03 .190** .153** .152** .279** .196** .374** .548** – 9 Inform04 .163** .155** .169** .259** .249** .368** .408** .472** – 10 Metasum .205** .431** .388** .065** −.014* .031** .118** .091** .072** – 11 Undrem .171** .373** .337** .069** −.023** .034** .101** .085** .070** .451** – 12 Navi .262** .623** .683** .146** .088** .109** .230** .156** .161** .321** .269** –

Note. ESCS= students’ economic, social and cultural status; PRA = printed reading literacy; ERA = electronic reading literacy;

Social01= reading e-mails; Social02 = <chat online> (e.g., MSN®, Microsoft, Redmond, WA, USA); Inform01= reading online news;

Inform02= using an online dictionary or encyclopaedia (e.g., Wikipedia); Inform03 = searching online information to learn about a

particular topic; Inform04= searching for practical information online (e.g., schedules, events, tips, recipes); Metasum = perceived

usefulness of summarizing strategies; Undrem= perceived usefulness of understanding and remembering strategies; Navi = the

centred numbers of the relevant pages visited by students during the digital reading assignment.

(8)

and SRMR= .063). We provided both standardized and unstandardized path coefficients in the corresponding tables for the mediation models, but interpretation of the result was primarily made using unstandardized solution so that variables can be interpreted on mean-ingful metrics. While there were some significant paths for the reversed hypothesized model, the parameter estimates were very small in terms of effect size, ranging from 0 to 0.095. The significant results were regarded as trivial and may be due to the large sample size. Therefore, we focused our attention on the hypothesized model.

Direct effects

Table 2 presents the direct path estimates for the overall 19 countries. As illustrated, all the hypothesized direct paths in the overall mediation model were statis-tically significant except the one from social reading activities to navigation skills in the overall model

(b= 0.05, SE = .46, p > .05). This finding was not

sur-prising because social reading activities require fewer navigation skills; therefore, increasing the frequency of social reading activities will not lead to better naviga-tion skills.

Across the 19 countries, only the patterns for Korea and Chile differed from the overall model. Social reading activities had a negative effect on navigation

skills in Korea (b= −4.02, SE = 1.32, p < .05) whereas

it had a positive effect on navigation skills (b= 2.05,

SE= .06, p < .05) in Chile. Social reading activities

also negatively predicted metacognitive strategy

awareness (b= −.37, SE = .08, p < .05), PRA (b =

−66.65, SE = 14.79, p < .05) and ERA (b = −47.41,

SE= 11.64, p < .05) in the overall model.

In examining these effects across 19 countries, we found that the individual patterns agreed with the overall model for social reading on metacognitive strat-egy and PRA, but the effect on ERA tended to be null in most countries. That is, 14 out of 19 countries had an insignificant path from social reading to ERA. On the other hand, positive direct effects were observed from information-seeking activities on metacognitive

strat-egies (b= .27, SE = .04, p < .05), navigation skills (b =

2.95, SE= .29, p < .05), PRA (b = 38.44, SE = 5.76,

p< .05) and ERA (b = 27.92, SE = 4.74, p < .05). The

significance pattern was consistent across the partici-pating countries with few exceptions.

The effects of metacognitive strategies and naviga-tion skills on the PRA and ERA were all positive and conformed to the literature on both the overall and the individual models. Specifically, for every one point increase in knowledge of metacognitive strategies,

stu-dents’ PRAs increased by 46.20 points (SE= 2.10,

p< .05) and ERAs increased by 36.93 points (SE =

2.91, p< .05). Better navigation skills also led to higher

PRA (b= 3.59, SE = .15, p < .05) and ERA scores

(b= 4.67, SE = .27, p < .05).

Indirect effects

Table 3 exhibits the result for the indirect effects in the 19 countries. We calculated the indirect effects from social or information-seeking reading activities on the PRA and ERA via metacognitive strategies or naviga-tions skills, yielding eight indirect effects. The indirect effects on the PRA and ERA from social

read-ing activities were negative via metacognitive

strategies (bsocial→meta→PRA= −16.85, SE = 3.52, p <

.05; bsocial→meta→ERA= −13.47, SE = 2.75, p < .05). The

indirect effects on the PRA and ERA from

social reading activities were neutral via

naviga-tion skills (bsocial→navi→PRA= .19, SE = 1.64, p > .05;

bsocial→navi→ERA= .25, SE = 2.15, p > .05). For both direct and indirect effects, results showed that social reading activities had either a negative or no effect on the PRA and ERA in overall and individual country analyses. The indirect effects on the PRA and ERA from information-seeking reading activities were positive

via metacognitive strategies (binfo→meta→PRA= 12.55,

SE= 1.47, p < .05; binfo→meta→ERA= 10.03, SE = 1.26,

p< .05). The indirect effects on the PRA and ERA from

information-reading activities were also positive via

navigation skills (binfo→navi→PRA= 10.57, SE = .97,

p< .05; binfo→navi→ERA= 13.78, SE = 1.65, p < .05).

Overall, the results indicated that information-seeking activities had a positive effect on the ERA or PRA, as demonstrated by knowledge of metacognitive strat-egies or navigation skills.

Effects of gender differences and ESCS on online reading engagement, mediators, and PRA and ERA

We examined the moderating effects of gender on social or information-reading engagement, knowledge of metacognitive strategies, navigation skills, and the

(9)

T able 2. The Dir ect Path Estimates for the 19 Countries Country or area (ranking) 19 countries Hong Kong Korea Japan Macau New Zealand Australia (5) (1) (4) (12) (2) (2) Model fit indices RMSEA = .063 RMSEA = .073 RMSEA = .061 RMSEA = .046 RMSEA = .070 RMSEA = .068 RMSEA = .066 SRMR = .063 SRMR = .054 SRMR = .060 SRMR = .037 SRMR = .062 SRMR = .071 SRMR = .061 Direct effect Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Social → Meta 0.37 0.08 0.30 0.10 0.78 0.20 0.29 0.23 0.26 0.08 0.11 0.04 0.10 0.04 (− .24) (− .24) (− .48) (− .12) (− .25) (− .13) (− .12) → Navi 0.05 0.46 − 0.23 0.98 4.02 1.32 − 1.57 2.28 0.20 0.65 0.60 0.34 − 0.33 0.28 (.003) (− .02) (− .30) (− .06) (.02) (.06) (− .03) → PRA 66.65 14.79 32.53 9.25 73.86 14.36 70.08 25.72 19.68 5.31 7.45 3.40 − 4.60 2.69 (− .34) (− .23) (− .47) (− .26) (− .16) (− .07) (− .04) → ERA 47.41 11.64 − 9.88 5.08 19.95 9.64 − 16.85 14.72 2.29 4.06 4.44 2.64 0.42 2.00 (− .23) (− .08) (− .14) (.27) (.02) (.04) (.004) Inform → Meta 0.27 0.04 0.41 0.08 0.64 0.14 0.16 0.08 0.38 0.07 0.35 0.06 0.36 0.04 (.27) (.33) (.51) (.18) (.35) (.27) (0.31) → Navi 2.95 0.29 3.02 0.79 4.19 0.86 2.84 0.83 1.75 0.64 2.48 0.57 3.48 0.45 (.23) (− .20) (.41) (.30) (.13) (.18) (.25) → PRA 38.44 5.76 32.06 7.34 50.26 11.89 27.30 7.31 22.36 4.93 4.92 4.34 20.40 3.71 (.29) (.23) (.41) (− .07) (.17) (.03) (.13) → ERA 27.92 4.74 20.95 4.53 26.47 7.18 19.25 4.32 8.18 3.92 2.57 3.71 14.66 3.26 (.20) (.16) (.25) (.24) (.07) (.02) (.10) Meta → PRA 46.20 2.10 33.78 4.38 30.57 4.56 54.67 7.67 35.83 3.45 48.68 4.22 45.90 3.72 (.35) (.29) (.31) (.49) (.30) (.38) (.35) → ERA 36.93 2.91 21.84 2.77 25.66 3.99 35.79 5.29 23.31 2.58 41.90 3.68 35.93 2.71 (.27) (.21) (.30) (.39) (.23) (.35) (.29) Navi → PRA 3.59 0.15 3.92 0.27 3.13 0.50 2.78 0.35 4.01 0.16 4.81 0.31 4.92 0.25 (.35) (.42) (.26) (.26) (.57) (.41) (.44) → ERA 4.67 0.27 5.31 .23 4.34 0.38 3.74 0.22 4.59 0.15 5.90 0.25 6.11 0.20 (.43) (.61) (.42) (.43) (.17) (.53) (.58)

(10)

T able 2. Continued Country or area (ranking) Chile Colombia Iceland Sweden Ireland Belgium Norway (18) (19) (6) (7) (8) (9) (10) Model fit indices RMSEA = .088 RMSEA = .084 RMSEA = .069 RMSEA = .066 RMSEA = .063 RMSEA = .063 RMSEA = .067 SRMR = .115 SRMR = .109 SRMR = .055 SRMR = .061 SRMR = .053 SRMR = .056 SRMR = .056 Direct effect Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Social → Meta 0.08 0.04 0.11 0.05 0.01 0.11 0.15 0.07 − 0.03 0.03 0.26 0.04 0.25 0.10 (.12) (− .18) (.004) (− .14) (− .04) (− .17) (− .18) → Navi 2.05 0.60 0.98 0.54 − 0.27 0.91 − 0.06 0.50 0.58 0.34 0.04 0.58 0.53 1.21 (.18) (.10) (− .02) (− .01) (.07) (.002) (− .03) → PRA − 2.69 4.00 0.76 4.22 − 14.01 8.36 − 2.22 3.42 7.49 2.73 9.81 3.43 35.46 7.43 (− .03) (.01) (− .10) (− .02) (− .08) (− .05) (− .19) → ERA 0.57 33.55 0.26 3.22 1.09 5.39 0.63 2.91 − 0.13 1.65 5.15 2.99 16.71 6.09 (.01) (.003) (.01) (.01) (.002) (.03) (− .10) Inform → Meta − 0.07 0.07 0.25 0.06 0.09 0.11 0.25 0.06 0.23 0.08 0.25 0.05 0.37 0.10 (− .08) (.26) (.06) (.19) (.18) (.19) (.30) → Navi 2.07 0.73 2.69 0.86 2.15 0.93 2.78 0.57 3.20 0.81 1.46 0.51 2.19 1.11 (.13) (.18) (.12) (.17) (.19) (.09) (.13) → PRA 4.54 5.26 3.99 5.61 21.19 7.97 10.51 4.25 16.82 5.07 3.98 3.53 23.85 7.12 (.04) (.03) (.11) (.06) (.10) (.02) (.15) → ERA 10.81 5.18 7.27 4.69 8.82 6.85 12.14 3.83 14.12 4.26 2.34 2.67 14.26 5.24 (.08) (.06) (.05) (.08) (.09) (.02) (.10) Meta → PRA 51.75 5.35 49.15 6.11 37.38 5.15 47.32 4.08 42.64 4.30 52.56 3.22 46.01 4.95 (.38) (.39) (.31) (.37) (.33) (.42) (.35) → ERA 49.06 5.13 39.76 5.03 25.72 4.93 31.46 3.16 25.24 3.71 39.12 2.74 28.04 2.94 (.34) (.33) (.23) (.27) (.21) (.33) (.24) Navi → PRA 3.44 0.22 3.44 0.29 5.05 0.31 4.57 0.23 4.81 0.24 4.79 0.21 4.48 0.24 (.44) (.42) (.48) (.43) (.48) (.45) (.45) → ERA 4.53 0.19 4.19 0.24 6.14 0.28 5.43 0.23 5.86 0.22 5.50 0.16 5.74 0.19 (.55) (.54) (.61) (.57) (.64) (.56) (.64)

(11)

T able 2. Continued Country or area (ranking) France Denmark Spain Hungary Poland Austria (11) (13) (14) (15) (16) (17) Model fit indices RMSEA = .079 RMSEA = .066 RMSEA = .064 RMSEA = .076 RMSEA = .085 RMSEA = .092 SRMR = .067 SRMR = .064 SRMR = .050 SRMR = .099 SRMR = .093 SRMR = .069 Direct effect Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Social → Meta − 0.07 0.05 0.24 0.12 − 0.03 0.05 − 0.13 0.12 − 0.27 0.15 − 0.11 0.06 (− .08) (− .17) (− .03) (− .11) (− .25) (− .11) → Navi 0.41 0.58 − 1.60 1.47 − 0.52 0.60 − 0.45 1.32 0.34 1.54 − 0.40 0.69 (.03) (− .09) (− .04) (− .03) (.02) (− .03) → PRA − 11.18 8.00 25.58 8.55 9.45 4.04 − 6.65 6.35 − 4.06 9.35 19.35 5.48 (− .09) (− .16) (− .09) (− .05) (− .03) (− .15) → ERA 0.45 3.34 14.78 6.81 − 2.89 3.23 − 5.35 7.37 13.37 9.85 8.33 3.92 (.004) (− .09) (− .02) (− .04) (.11) (− .07) Inform → Meta 0.17 0.06 0.38 0.10 0.14 0.06 0.33 0.09 0.31 0.14 0.34 0.09 (.16) (.30) (.12) (.31) (.32) (.26) → Navi 2.74 0.60 2.87 1.30 3.89 0.73 4.34 1.08 3.01 1.40 3.92 1.20 (.18) (.18) (.24) (.28) (.23) (.20) → PRA 10.69 6.35 21.96 7.30 15.44 4.40 14.23 4.84 6.01 8.98 22.81 6.38 (.07) (.15) (.12) (.11) (.05) (.13) → ERA 2.88 3.12 18.90 5.951 14.69 3.22 14.97 7.03 0.63 8.94 13.45 5.52 (.02) (.13) (.10) (.11) (.01) (.08) Meta → PRA 47.67 6.70 36.09 6.56 37.21 4.13 38.28 4.34 31.92 4.09 42.86 5.55 (.35) (.31) (.31) (.32) (.28) (.31) → ERA 21.61 7.19 35.58 5.45 27.34 3.67 23.70 4.61 28.58 4.01 37.56 5.05 (.17) (.28) (.21) (.18) (.25) (.28) Navi → PRA 4.05 0.90 3.93 0.35 3.97 0.20 3.68 0.24 4.34 0.20 4.35 0.26 (.41) (.42) (.47) (.45) (.52) (.48) → ERA 6.53 0.95 5.43 0.28 5.96 0.26 5.67 0.23 5.24 0.20 5.60 0.25 (.71) (.59) (.65) (.62) (.62) (.62) Note . p values smaller than .05 ar e shown in boldface; p values smaller than .01 ar e shown in bold and italic type. The number in the par enthesis indicates the standar dized dir ect path estimate.

(12)

T able 3. The Indir ect Path Estimates for the 19 countries Country or area (ranking) 19 countries Hong Kong Korea Japan Macao New Zealand Australia (5) (1) (4) (12) (2) (2) Model fit indices RMSEA = .063 RMSEA = .073 RMSEA = .061 RMSEA = .046 RMSEA = .070 RMSEA = .068 RMSEA = .066 SRMR = .063 SRMR = .054 SRMR = .060 SRMR = .037 SRMR = .062 SRMR = .071 SRMR = .061 Indirect effect Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE PRA on: Social → Meta 16.85 3.52 10.05 3.13 23.70 5.22 − 15.84 12.01 9.19 2.844 5.52 2.18 4.69 1.77 (− .09) (.01) (− .07) (.02) (− .15) (.03) (− .06) (.04) (− .08) (.02) (− .05) (.02) (− .04) (.02) → Navi .19 1.64 − 0.91 3.80 12.56 3.38 − 4.36 6.10 0.78 2.62 2.87 1.63 − 1.61 1.39 (.001) (.01) (− .01) (.03) (− .08) (.02) (− .02) (.02) (.01) (.02) (.03) (.01) (− .01) (.01) Inform → Meta 12.55 1.47 13.74 2.89 19.65 3.96 8.68 3.89 13.51 2.58 16.95 3.10 16.73 2.71 (.10) (.01) (.10) (.02) (.16) (.03) (.09) (.04) (.11) (.02) (.10) (.02) (.11) (.02) → Navi 10.57 .97 11.83 2.96 13.12 2.65 7.88 1.95 7.01 2.54 11.95 2.71 17.13 2.37 (.08) (.01) (.08) (.02) (.11) (.02) (.08) (.02) (.05) (.02) (.07) (.02) (.11) (.01) ERA on: Social → Meta 13.47 2.75 6.49 2.26 19.89 6.80 − 10.37 7.98 5.98 2.09 4.75 1.90 3.67 1.42 (− .07) (.01) (− .05) (.02) (− .14) (.05) (− .05) (.03) (− .06) (.02) (− .04) (.02) (− .03) (.01) → Navi .25 2.15 − 1.23 5.18 17.44 5.41 − 5.87 8.45 0.90 2.99 3.52 1.97 − 2.00 1.73 (.001) (.01) (− .01) (.04) (− .13) (.04) (− .03) (.04) (.01) (.03) (.03) (.02) (− .02) (.02) Inform → Meta 10.03 1.26 8.88 2.08 16.50 5.04 5.68 2.58 8.79 1.88 14.59 2.78 13.10 1.97 (.07) (.01) (.07) (.02) (.15) (.05) (.07) (.03) (.08) (.02) (.09) (.02) (.09) (.01) → Navi 13.78 1.65 16.01 4.15 18.22 3.85 10.62 2.96 8.04 2.97 14.66 3.33 21.28 2.85 (.10) (.01) (.12) (.03) (.17) (.03) (.13) (.04) (.07) (.03) (.10) (.02) (.14) (.02)

(13)

T able 3. Continued Country or area (ranking) Chile Colombia Iceland Sweden Ireland Belgium Norway (18) (19) (6) (7) (8) (9) (10) Model fit indices RMSEA = .088 RMSEA = .084 RMSEA = .069 RMSEA = .066 RMSEA = .063 RMSEA = .063 RMSEA = .067 SRMR = .115 SRMR = .109 SRMR = .055 SRMR = .061 SRMR = .053 SRMR = .056 SRMR = .056 Indirect effect Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE PRA on: Social → Meta 4.31 2.18 5.42 2.64 0.17 4.06 7.11 3.14 − 1.15 1.44 13.84 2.33 11.34 4.72 (.05) (.02) (− .07) (.03) (.001) (.03) (− .05) (.02) (− .01) (.02) (− .07) (.02) (− .06) (.03) → Navi 7.07 2.15 3.38 1.89 − 1.34 4.60 − 0.26 2.29 2.77 1.68 0.18 2.79 − 2.35 5.33 (.08) (.02) (.04) (.02) (− .010) (.03) (− .002) (.02) (.03) (.02) (.001) (.01) (.01) (.03) Inform → Meta − 3.61 3.86 12.35 3.46 3.35 4.21 11.87 3.04 9.86 3.35 13.26 2.58 17.05 4.42 (− .03) (.03) (.10) (.03) (.02) (.02) (.07) (.02) (.06) (.02) (.08) (.02) (.10) (.03) → Navi 7.14 2.54 9.26 3.17 10.83 4.79 12.72 2.63 15.37 3.69 7.01 2.45 9.83 4.76 (.06) (.02) (.08) (.03) (.06) (.02) (.07) (.02) (.09) (.02) (.04) (.01) (.06) (.03) ERA on: Social → Meta 4.08 2.10 4.39 2.12 0.12 2.83 4.73 2.13 − 0.68 0.84 10.30 1.83 6.91 2.97 (.04) (.02) (− .06) (.03) (.001) (.02) (− .04) (.02) (− .01) (.01) (− .06) (.01) (− .04) (.02) → Navi 9.30 2.63 4.11 2.29 − 1.64 5.63 − 0.31 2.74 3.38 1.98 0.21 3.20 − 3.01 6.91 (.10) (.03) (.06) (.03) (− .01) (.04) (− .003) (.02) (.04) (.02) (.001) (.02) (− .02) (.04) Inform → Meta − 3.42 3.68 9.99 2.83 2.30 3.02 7.89 2.08 5.84 1.95 9.86 1.98 10.39 2.73 (− .03) (.03) (.09) (.03) (.01) (.02) (.05) (.01) (.04) (.01) (.06) (.01) (.07) (.02) → Navi 9.39 3.39 11.27 3.65 13.18 5.82 15.12 3.24 18.72 4.68 8.05 2.77 12.58 6.32 (.07) (.03) (.10) (.03) (.07) (.03) (.10) (.02) (.12) (.03) (.05) (.02) (.09) (.04)

(14)

T able 3. Continued Country or area (ranking) France Denmark Spain Hungary Poland Austria (11) (13) (14) (15) (16) (17) Model fit indices RMSEA = .079 RMSEA = .066 RMSEA = .064 RMSEA = .076 RMSEA = .085 RMSEA = .092 SRMR = .067 SRMR = .064 SRMR = .050 SRMR = .099 SRMR = .093 SRMR = .069 Indirect effect Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE Coefficient SE PRA on: Social → Meta − 3.31 2.56 − 8.52 4.54 − 1.06 1.96 − 5.08 4.60 − 8.45 4.83 − 4.59 2.46 (− .03) (.02) (− .05) (.03) (− .01) (.02) (− .037) (.03) (− .07) (.04) (− .04) (.02) → Navi 1.65 2.37 − 6.29 5.34 − 2.06 2.34 − 1.67 4.78 1.46 6.73 − 1.73 2.97 (.01) (.02) (− .04) (.03) (− .02) (.02) (− .01) (.03) (.01) (.06) (− .01) (.02) Inform → Meta 8.23 3.00 13.62 4.12 5.16 2.39 12.51 3.54 9.83 4.43 14.53 4.21 (.05) (.02) (.09) (.03) (.04) (.02) (.10) (.03) (.09) (.04) (.08) (.02) → Navi 11.12 3.26 11.30 4.54 15.42 2.71 15.94 3.66 13.04 6.08 17.06 5.44 (.07) (.02) (.08) (.03) (.12) (.02) (.13) (.03) (.12) (.06) (.09) (.03) ERA on: Social → Meta − 1.50 1.25 − 7.69 3.96 − 0.78 1.48 − 3.15 2.85 − 7.57 4.40 − 4.02 2.14 (− .01) (.01) (− .05) (.02) (− .01) (.01) (− .02) (.02) (− .06) (.04) (− .03) (.02) → Navi 2.66 3.85 − 8.69 7.74 − 3.10 3.54 − 2.57 7.44 1.76 8.07 − 2.23 3.83 (.02) (.04) (− .05) (.05) (− .03) (.03) (− .02) (.05) (.02) (.07) (− .02) (.03) Inform → Meta 3.73 1.88 12.30 3.63 3.79 1.91 7.74 2.58 8.80 4.04 12.73 3.78 (.03) (.02) (.08) (.03) (.03) (.01) (.06) (.02) (.08) (.04) (.07) (.02) → Navi 17.92 4.89 15.62 6.75 23.168 4.20 24.60 5.82 15.75 7.53 21.95 6.82 (.13) (.03) (.11) (.05) (.16) (.03) (.18) (.04) (.14) (.07) (.12) (.04) Note . p values smaller than .05 ar e shown in boldface. p values smaller than .01 ar e shown in bold and italic type. The number in the par enthesis indicates the standar dized indir ect path estimate.

(15)

PRA and ERA, holding other variables constant. We did not find significant gender differences in the scores of social or information-reading engagement for the

overall 19 countries (for social engagement, b= −0.02,

SE= .01, p = .22; for information engagement, b =

0.01, SE= .02, p = .60). In individual country analyses,

girls were engaged in more online social reading

activ-ities than boys in 4 out of the 19 countries at p= .05

level (bChile= −0.13, SE = .05; bDenmark= −0.11, SE =

.05; bIreland= −0.30, SE = .06; bNewZealand= −0.22, SE =

.06). As for information reading, for 7 out of the 19 countries, we found significant gender differences, but the results were mixed. Boys in four European coun-tries tended to be more engaged in information-reading

activities (bDenmark= 0.14, SE = .05, p < .05; bIceland=

0.17, SE= .04, p < .05; bNorway= 0.08, SE = .03,

p< .05; bSweden= 0.08, SE = .03, p < .01) whereas girls

in three Asian and Oceanian countries were more

involved in information-reading activities (bKorea=

−0.11, SE = .05, p < .05; bMacau= −.09, SE = .02, p <

.05; bNewZealand= −0.10, SE = .03, p < .05).

Girls had consistently better knowledge of

meta-cognitive strategies (b= −.28, SE = .02, p < .05),

navi-gation skills (b= −1.27, SE = .18, p < .05) and PRA

(b= −15.42, SE = 1.72, p < .05) than boys. They also

had better ERA scores than boys (b= −4.78, SE = 1.59,

p< .05) in the overall model. Although the negative

path direction was generally consistent in the ERA, insignificant results were found for 12 out of the 19 countries, indicating that the gender gap in the ERA may be minimal in most countries, holding other vari-ables constant.

We also controlled for students’ ESCS in the analy-sis. Results showed that the higher the ESCS, the higher the students’ knowledge of metacognitive strat-egies, navigation skills, and PRA and ERA, holding everything constant.

Testing model equivalence across genders

To investigate if the hypothesized model was equiva-lent across gender, we performed additional multiple group comparisons. Specifically, we compared the con-strained model, in which girls and boys’ models are the same in parameter estimates, with the unconstrained model, in which the parameters in girls and boys’ models are freely estimated.

The model fit statistics for the constrained model

were BIC= 1 723 547.949, RMSEA = 0.061, SRMR =

0.069; for unconstrained model, they were BIC=

1 723 544.278, RMSEA= 0.066, SRMR = 0.068.

Nevertheless, the differences between the fit statistics

(ΔBIC = 3.671, ΔRMSEA = .005 and ΔSRMR = .001)

were trivial (Chen, 2007; Raftery, 1995), indicating that the hypothesized model was feasible for both girls and boys.

Discussion and implications for classroom instruction

The current study tested the effects of various online activities on two forms of reading assessments and explored the mechanism that explained the shared vari-ance in PRA and ERA. The hypothesized pattern was evaluated and supported with the overall sample as well as individual country samples. Furthermore, the gender group comparison suggested that the hypoth-esized model held for both boys and girls.

By determining the possible mechanism, we will be able to provide effective interventions and treatments in the classroom to enhance student reading. The fol-lowing discussion of the study results will focus on direct effects, indirect effects, and moderating and con-trolled effects.

Direct effects

The effect of metacognitive strategies and navigation skills on reading literacy

Based on the results of the overall and individual mediation models, we confirmed the effectiveness of knowledge of metacognitive strategies and navigation skills on reading literacy. Consequently, we encourage teachers to provide metacognitive strategy instruction not only to facilitate students’ reading but also to promote higher level thinking.

To effectively implement metacognitive instruc-tion requires a solid pedagogical understanding of metacognition, which refers to teachers’ understanding of what is needed to teach students to be metacognitive (Wilson & Bai, 2010). The instructional content should include the strategies, how to implement the strategies and when to apply the strategies (Paris, Lipson, & Wixson, 1994).

Effective pedagogical practices recommended by researchers include thinking out loud (Wilhelm, 2001),

(16)

scaffolding (Choi, Land, & Turgeon, 2005; Clark & Graves, 2005) and debriefing (Leat & Lin, 2003). For example, Clark and Graves (2005) proposed a model of scaffolding students’ comprehension before, during and after reading with moment-to-moment scaffolding and reciprocal teaching. Leat and Lin (2003) suggested using debriefing (defined as small-group or whole-class discussion after learning) to help students con-sciously extend or explore their learning.

It is essential to recognize that students need time to apply these strategies during reading and reflect the adequacy of their use (Wilson & Bai, 2010). Therefore, teachers should create an environment where students can put the metacognitive activities into action and reflect on their thinking (Leat & Lin, 2003). Computer-based programs can also be used for strategy training. For example, Sung, Chang, and Huang (2008) devel-oped the computer-assisted strategy teaching and learn-ing environment to aid students’ strategy acquisition and practice in reading electronic texts.

In addition, teachers may consider including the training of navigation skills as part of their reading curriculum in recognition of the fact that today’s ado-lescents are reading on the Internet and that navigation skills are a key to successful online reading. The strat-egy of debriefing (Leat & Lin, 2003) may also be used in navigation training. Activities such as small-group discussion during or after the online reading session can be used to encourage the sharing of decision-making processes and why students choose to click on a particular link, why they think the particular link will lead to where they expect to go, and how they can integrate and reflect on information across different pages.

The effect of different online reading activities on metacognitive strategies, navigation skills, and the PRA and ERA

The social and information-seeking reading activities had differential effects on metacognitive strategies, navigation skills, and the PRA and ERA. For example, information-seeking reading activities had positive effects on knowledge of metacognitive strategies, navi-gation skills, and ERA and PRA. When students are engaged in information-seeking activities, they need to carefully select the keywords to search, decide on the relevance of each returned query, predict the content of unseen pages and integrate information from multiple

web pages. These intense mental activities were con-stantly recurring whenever students searched to read and learn online. With the increase in information-seeking activities, students’ knowledge of meta-cognitive strategies, navigation skills, and ERA and PRA increase.

In contrast, social reading activities negatively impacted students’ metacognitive strategies and PRA, and had no significant effect on navigation skills. When students are engaged in social reading activities, such as chatting online and reading e-mails, they are mainly sending and returning messages. These automatic rou-tines have little to do with the improvement of navi-gation skills. Moreover, students’ knowledge of metacognitive strategies and literacy scores tended to deteriorate with increased social reading activities.

Although the effect of social reading activities on ERA was negative in the overall model, the majority of countries had a null effect. As a result, the effect of social reading on ERA warrants more research.

Indirect effects of social and information-seeking activities on the ERA and PRA

The indirect effects of information-seeking reading activities on reading literacy shed light on the impor-tance of the reading curriculum moving towards a student-centred reading paradigm. The direct effect told us that reading literacy can improve with better knowledge of metacognitive strategy and navigation skills, which can be best taught through embedded learning activities (Veenman & Beishuizen, 2004). Moreover, if students are engaged in more information-seeking activities, their knowledge of metacognitive strategies and navigation skills will be directly improved, and reading literacy, whether in print or digital form, will be promoted indirectly. The engage-ment in information-seeking activities involves higher order mental activities, which require students to control, monitor, integrate and evaluate their online reading processes.

The implications for classroom practice are to trans-form the teacher-centred paradigm towards a student-centred paradigm where students work on project-based reading activities or topic-specific readings to construct their unique intertext among multiple web pages and make sense of the reading materials (Lee, Waxman, Wu, Michko, & Lin, 2013). Teachers can

(17)

introduce students to online pedagogical tools in their web-based search project. For instance, the IdeaKeeper employs explicit representation of each online inquiry strategy to promote and facilitate students’ planning, information search, analysing and synthesizing on the reading topic (Zhang & Quintana, 2012).

Gender differences in online reading engagement, metacognitive strategies, navigation skills, and ERA and PRA

We found gender differences in knowledge of metacognitive strategies, navigation skills, and ERA and PRA. These results were in line with those of other research (Kolic´-Vehovec & Bajšanski, 2006; Sheorey & Mokhtari, 2001), showing that girls had better knowledge of metacognitive strategies. In addition, the findings in PRA were consistent with prior research, again exhibiting a female reading prominence (Carroll, 2004; Chiu & McBride-Chang, 2006; Lietz, 2006), but the gender gap in ERA tended to be smaller and nearly negligible in most countries in the hypothesized model. Researchers and practitioners have attempted to close the gender gap in reading. Therefore, what is associated with the smaller gender difference found in this study will be of great importance. Though engage-ment in reading can predict students’ achieveengage-ments in reading (Froiland & Oros, 2013), we did not find gender differences in social or information-reading engagement in the overall model. However, based on Bandura’s (1993) self-efficacy theory, it is possible that boys’ greater confidence (Durndell & Haag, 2002; Ring, 1991; Vekiri & Chronaki, 2008) and girls’ higher anxiety (Cooper, 2006) in the electronic reading envi-ronment contributed to the smaller gap in ERA. We recommend that future studies examine whether such an effect exists.

Research in the online search strategies usually have found that boys have better self-reported search (or navigation) skills (Hargittai & Shafer, 2006; Tsai, 2009). But in our study, we observed that girls’ navi-gation skills, measured in terms of relevant pages visited, were better than boys’. In a sample of 29 novice, intermediate level, and expert participants, Tabatabai and Shore (2005) summarized that the key elements in successful web search included ‘(a) using clear criteria to evaluate sites, (b) not excessively navi-gating, (c) reflecting on strategies and monitoring

pro-gress, (d) having background knowledge about information seeking, and (e) approaching the search with a good attitude and enjoying the process’ (p. 239). Among these criteria, metacognitive strategy is a key component. In our proposed model, the correlation between navigation skills and knowledge of

meta-cognitive strategies was .38 (p< .01), showing that the

metacognitive strategies and the navigation skills were modestly and positively correlated so that girls having better knowledge of metacognitive strategies also tended to have better navigation skills. Female advan-tages on these variables lead to better reading literacy.

Conclusion

Reading in the e-learning era encompasses both print and digital media. Besides the infrastructure of infor-mation and communications technology as a tool to access the cyber informational space, students need to be empowered to use appropriate metacognitive strat-egies and navigation skills to achieve their reading goals. The current study emphasized the importance of knowledge of metacognitive strategies and navigation skills for student reading; moreover, our results dem-onstrated that it is essential to provide ample opportu-nities for students to perform information-seeking reading activities, which is an incubator for nurturing metacognitive strategy awareness and navigation skills. In contrast, social reading activities either had no effect or negative effect on learning, unless specially designed for learning purposes (Kabilan, Ahmad, & Abidin, 2010). Future research should focus on stu-dents’ reading profiles or a combination of multiple reading activities on the Internet.

Acknowledgements

The author gratefully thank the action editor and reviewers for their insightful comments and sugges-tions. The present study was partially supported by the National Science Council, Taiwan (Grant No. NSC 101–2628-H-009-003-MY3).

References

Akyel, A., & Erçetin, G. (2009). Hypermedia reading strat-egies employed by advanced learners of English. System, 37(1), 136–152.

數據

Table 1. Zero-Order Correlation Among Items, Indices and Observed Variables

參考文獻

相關文件

For example, the teacher librarians teach students reading strategies while English and Chinese language subject teachers provide reading materials for students to

Through arranging various reading activities such as online reading, book recommendation and extended reading materials, schools help students connect reading to

9 Enhancing Assessment Literacy in the English Language Curriculum at the Secondary Level: (I) Reading and Listening Skills (New). Workshop Jan-Mar

Planning and using digital multimodal texts to facilitate KS2 students’ reading skills development and enrich their reading experiences?.

Students are provided with opportunities to learn and develop the skills, strategies and confidence needed to participate in Guided and Independent Reading as well as the

Using Information Texts in the Primary English Classroom: Developing KS2 Students’ Reading and Writing Skills (New). Jan-Feb 2016

Expecting students engage with a different level of language in their work e.g?. student A needs to label the diagram, and student B needs to

How can our teaching of English reading and listening cater for these dimensions of learner diversity.. auditory