• 沒有找到結果。

(1)Chapter Two Literature Review The present research aims to investigate the effects of story-mapping instruction on picture writing

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "(1)Chapter Two Literature Review The present research aims to investigate the effects of story-mapping instruction on picture writing"

Copied!
31
0
0

加載中.... (立即查看全文)

全文

(1)

Chapter Two Literature Review

The present research aims to investigate the effects of story-mapping instruction

on picture writing. In this chapter, the literature of the prewriting strategies, picture

writing, and the story mapping theory will be reviewed.

2.1 The Importance of Pre-writing Strategies

Over the past 40 years, the focus on writing instruction has shifted from product

of writing to process of writing. According to Applebee (1986), the writing instruction

in the past was product-centered, emphasizing correct usage and mechanics and there

was little use of process approaches to writing instruction. Some recent trends in

writing instruction emphasize the writing process more than the writing product.

Hayes and Flower (1980) propose a cognitive process model of composing, which

contains three phases: planning, translating and reviewing. These phases do not take

place in a fixed, linear progression but in a recursive manner. With the change of

focus in writing instruction from product to process, both researchers and instructors

emphasize the importance of involving younger writers in prewriting experience

before expecting them to write (Noyce & Christie, 1989). Many researchers (Brown,

1988; Tompkins, 1990; Routman, 1991; Graves, 1994) consider the most crucial of

the writing process to be the prewriting stage. The prewriting activities can provide

(2)

students with rich experiences, help students in generating ideas for writing and aid

students in the structuring of content (Tompkins & Friend, 1986).

The prewriting stage is a writer’s warm-up allowing the writer to be prepared to

write. During the prewriting stage, Noyce & Christie (1989) suggest that learners need

to engage in talking, picturializing and planning before beginning to write, such as

brainstorming, clustering, outlining, and mapping, which help students to decide what

to write and how to go about it. Through such experience, writers discover what they

want to say when they write. However, young writers often demonstrate lack of

planning and reflection in advance of and during the act of writing. Many of these

students approach the writing task impulsively, rather than thinking about the episodic

structure beginning, middle, and end of the narrative product (Troia, Graham, &

Harris, 1999).

Moreover, researchers point out that because many students spend little time

planning in advance of writing, they have problems finding enough to say (Harris &

Graham, 1992; Charnry, Newman & Palmquist, 1995). They also find that not

knowing what to write often leads to writing apprehension, which has a negative

impact on the writing performance. The feeling of not being able to express

themselves clearly in English results in learners’ frustration and anxiety in writing.

They suggest that teaching prewriting strategies helps students in two important ways:

(3)

First, they become more secure and confident when they can use prewriting strategies

in their writing. Second, as their confidence increases, their approach to apply

prewriting activities becomes more mature. Ellis (2003) also indicates that when the

students write, using prewriting format to generate content is helpful to learners’

narrative writing. The strategic prewriting activities help writers in using their

knowledge of structure and content to orchestrate the writing process and forge their

own ideas. Therefore, it is proposed that prewriting strategies can facilitative writing

performance.

Studies which have sought to determine the causes of writing apprehension

(Thompson, 1983; Walsh, 1986; Pajares & Viliante, 1997) have found that students’

fear of writing exposure is possibly due to inadequate practice. Since writing anxiety

is caused by a writer’s lack of writing skills, improving those skills should reduce

writing apprehension. An accepted method of improving students’ writing skills is to

familiarize students with the writing process, in particular, the prewriting stage.

Researches have shown that teachers who emphasize prewriting activities have

enabled their students to produce higher proficiency scores in writing (Becker, 1991;

Goldstein & Carr, 1996; Schweiker-Marra & Marra, 2000; Huang, 2003, Saddler, et

al., 2004). Additionally, the prewriting strategies help to decrease students’ writing

apprehension (Schweiker-Marra & Marra, 2000).

(4)

From above, it is shown that prewriting strategies are important in two ways:

1. Prewriting activities enhance learners’ writing fluency.

2. Prewriting activities reduce learners’ writing apprehension or anxiety.

2.1.1 The Related Studies on Pre-writing Strategies

Becker (1991) investigates the effects of prewriting, an activity associated with

process approaches to teaching writing. The participants in this study included 424

adult learners of German at various levels in a Brazil language school. The students in

the experimental group used prewriting activity for 5 minutes before writing whereas

students in the control group did not. Both raters identified a higher percentage of

imagery and interesting ideas in experimental than in control group compositions. In

addition, on a measure of writing fluency, the students of the experimental group

significantly outperformed those in the control group.

In Schweiker-Marra & Marra’s (2000) research, it is shown that with the use of

extensive prewriting activities, students could improve their writing abilities while

eliminating or lessening their writing apprehension. The participants included 29

participants who were elementary students with poor writing skills. The control group

received no formal writing instruction while the experimental group underwent direct

instruction on the writing process with emphasis on prewriting skills. The

experimental students were exposed to prewriting activities in order to improve their

(5)

writing skills and to reduce their writing anxiety. The two instruments utilized for

collecting the quantitative data were a holistic rubric for narrative writing and Daly &

Miller’s Writing Apprehension Scale (1975). The results of the research demonstrated

that the participants in the experimental students improved in their written expression

scores from beginning to end of the treatment and in comparison with the control

group. Writing anxiety decreased for the experimental group during the study through

writing instruction that emphasized prewriting activities.

In an EFL learning surrounding here in Taiwan, there are also studies on the

positive effects of pre-writing strategies on English writing. For example, in Huang

(2003), the researcher reports the positive effects of teaching organizing strategies in

the prewriting phase on Chinese students’ English writing proficiency. The

participants of the study included 80 intermediate-level EFL student writers. They

were divided into two groups: the experimental group receiving the instruction in

organizational exercises and the control group the instruction in grammatical

exercises. The adopted prewriting activities included brainstorming, clustering, and

outlining. The comparison of mean pre-post scores on the five factors, organization,

content richness, grammatical accuracy, vocabulary appropriateness, mechanics

accuracy included, indicated that the experimental group made more significant

improvement than the control group in content and organization. The researcher

(6)

suggests that prewriting strategies did have positive effects on content and

organization in students’ writing and English instructors could incorporate the

organizing strategy in the prewriting phase to help the beginning EFL writers.

In the present study, the researcher applies the story mapping instruction as a

prewriting activity to story writing class to see if there is progress in the students’

writing performance. That is because the story-mapping instruction relatively has not

received much attention here in Taiwan with comparison with other prewriting

activities, such as brainstorming, outlining, clustering, etc. It is also because no

research here in Taiwan has been conducted to investigate the effects of the

story-mapping strategy on EFL writers. Besides, as will be reviewed in 2.3, research

has shown that story mapping is one of the pre-writing activities that can promote the

story writing ability and decrease the writing apprehension of EFL beginning writers

since it provides students with the basic story structure or scaffolding.

2.2 The Trend of Picture Writing

The reason why the researcher focuses on finding a workable strategy to help

students with picture writing is that picture writing is the trend of writing tests in

Taiwan. In recent years, the English writing tasks have shifted from topic-guided

writing to picture-guided writing. Since 2003, the local writing competence tests have

adopted picture-guided writing, with a single picture writing in the writing part of

(7)

GEPT and sequential-picture writing in JCEE respectively. It becomes more likely

that students have to face the challenge of picture writing, but up to now there have

been few studies here in Taiwan conducted to investigate a workable approach to help

student with the English picture writing. It is the purpose of the present study to

provide a workable writing strategy to help student with picture writing.

2.2.1 The Related Study on Picture Writing

To the best of our knowledge, in Taiwan up to now only Chiang (2003) has

investigated the effectiveness of the application of picture writing on EFL writing In

Chiang (2003), there were sixty participants, the control group (N=30) for the

topic-guided story writing and the experiment group (N=30) for picture-guided story

writing. The result showed that the group receiving the picture aids outperformed its

counterpart in constructing a story with a complete story format, that was a beginning,

a middle and an ending. Besides, with the help of the given pictures, the students

could make stories with longer texts and richer vocabulary. Also, there was a positive

change of the students’ attitude toward and motivation for story writing in English

class. In Chiang’s study, although she demonstrated the effectiveness of

picture-guided story writing, there were still many students who failed to write a

well-organized story. The reason to account for the student’s disability to write a story

with better organization with the help of pictures is that the students lacked the

(8)

thorough knowledge of story structure. In her study, Chiang asked the students guided

questions of the storyline without giving them the whole picture of the story elements.

In the present study, the researcher would like to further study if the story mapping

instruction can help the students generate a well-organized story with rich content

when they face a picture writing task. The reason that the researcher used the story

mapping strategy to picture writing is that there is a strong bond between them. Both

sequential pictures and story mapping can explicitly display graphic story elements.

Tseng (2002) also reported the effectiveness of using pictures to enhance

students’ Chinese writing performance. In her study, Tseng explained the topic in the

given pictures, guided the six participants to observe the details in the pictures and

asked them to discuss about the pictures before they wrote. After the instruction, the

participants improved in organization of their narratives and their interest in writing

and observation of details were enhanced. Tseng indicated that pictures, providing the

students the arrangement of the order of paragraphs, helped them with story writing.

Chiang and Tseng both indicated that as long as the students followed the frame

of the given pictures, they could be motivated to compose a unified story. However,

many students still feel anxious and frustrated when writing English compositions of

the picture-writing form. The researcher would like to see if the story-mapping

strategy is a practical and workable writing strategy to guide the students how to

(9)

compose unified picture writing.

2.3 The Theory of Story Mapping

Story mapping is a schematic and strategic learning tool based on the concept of

story grammar. It is of great importance to reading comprehension and writing

instruction. Since it provides the visualized story schema or basic story elements of a

story, readers can have a better picture of the relationship among all story elements

and hence comprehend the story more quickly and easily. Writers as well can benefit

from the assistance of story mapping. Based on the concept of story structure or story

grammar, writers can develop and construct better-organized and coherent narratives.

Different fields of cognitive and psychological theories, including schema,

metacognitive, and reduction theories have supported the application of story

mapping (Foley, 2000).

The schema theory supports the application of story mapping. Researchers have

determined that narrative stories in general have a similar, internal structure or

component parts (Stein & Glenn, 1975; Rumelhart, 1975; Mandler & Johnson, 1977;

Rumelhart, 1977; Rumelhart, 1981). Knowledge of this structure is beneficial to

writing process since it provides the prior knowledge of a story. When writing a topic,

authors must access their prior knowledge (schemata) of that topic. The schema

serves as the source of the content of writing. Without them, writers have nothing to

(10)

write about (Noyce & Christie, 1989). Hayes and Flower (1980) also proposes that in

order to be successful in the area of writing, writers must search in their cognitive

structures and relate and synthesize many bits and pieces of information to complete a

writing assignment. These bits and pieces of information stored in the learner’s long-

and short-term memory constitute a student’s prior knowledge or schema. In the case

of story-mapping strategy, story grammars can become text structure schemata, and

the elements outlined on the story-map slots, or parts of a schema. While writing

narratives, writers retrieve the information from those slots and generate more ideas

from the story schema (Foley, 2000). Story structure in the context of reading and

written texts has been examined by many educators and cognitive psychologists.

Mandler and Johnson (1980) found that when young children were given brief

instruction on the major story parts, they were able to use this story schema in aiding

them to recall information. It has been demonstrated that a knowledge of story schema

facilitates both reading comprehension and narrative writing (Brown, 1988).

Additionally, cognitive psychologists using the rationale of a top down information

processing model see the importance of schema as it applies to students’ ability to

retrieve information while completing a writing task (Rentel & King, 1983). When

writers make decisions about what they want to say and how they want to approach

their topic, the decisions are influenced by writer’s prior knowledge. Background

(11)

knowledge or schema is a necessary component that facilitates the writing process. In

the story-writing process, writers have to retrieve the existing schema of the story

structure to compose their stories. Schema theory assumes that the reason why writers

have difficulty knowing what to write may lies in they fail to activate prior knowledge

structure as they write, or that they simply do not see the relationship between

existing schemata and new topic. With the help of a schema, which provides a prior

knowledge of narratives, the writer might compose more organized and coherent texts.

Hence, in order to enhance students’ narrative writing performance, it is suggested

that one should facilitate the story schema building process.

Metacognitive and reduction theories have also supported the application of

story mapping. Since story-mapping strategies provide writers with a step-by-step

procedure for how to write a story, they then become a metacognitive strategy for the

way to think about writing. If students are to become better writers, teachers need to

make them aware of the psychological processes entailed in writing. With repeated

practice using story maps to guide their writing, students begin to internalize this

procedure. Like scaffolding, the external control of the story map is gradually

removed as writers establish internal control and begins to self-monitor their own

writing. Reduction theory claims that all complex process can best be understood by

breaking them down into their constituent parts and then putting them back together,

(12)

piece by piece (Kincheloe, 1993). This is precisely what story maps require students

to do. Researchers believe that most narratives have a similar story structure and basic

story elements called story grammar. Also story maps provide the visual

demonstration of this story structure. If the language learner has the concept of story

grammar, the constituent parts of stories, it might help them in the complex process of

writing narratives.

2.3.1 Definition of Story Mapping

Before we define story mapping, we should have a clear concept of “story.”

“Story” is a sub-type of narrative and it is a specific, past-time narrative that tells

about a series of events which took place at specific unique moments in a unique

past-time world (Cortazzi, 1994). Text-analysis research has shown that most stories

share the same, predictable basic structure or pattern of events that create the

properties of a story (Noyce & Christie, 1989). This organizational familiarity, which

may occur consciously or unconsciously, is called story grammar (Mandler & Johnson,

1977; Thorndyke, 1977; Stein & Trabasso, 1982). In simple stories, there is a

universal story grammar which involve a main character who encounters a problem,

sets a goal, experiences events while he or she attempts to achieve the goal, and either

succeeds or fails at achieving the goal. Story grammar is an attempt to describe the

rules that people use to create and remember stories. Like sentence grammar, a story

(13)

grammar is a set of structural rules. For example, all well-formed sentences must

have a subject and predicate, and the predicate describes the action of the subject.

Similarly, according to story grammar rules, stories are expected to have a main

character, who is confronted with some type of problem (Shanahan & Shanahan,

1997). Researchers might have slightly different story elements in their research due

to the different needs or purposes of the researches. Idol (1987), for example, defines

story grammar information as (a) the setting, including the characters, time, and the

place; (b) the problem; (c) the goals; (d) the action; and (e) the outcomes of the story;

while Gurney, Gersten, Dimino, &Carnine, (1990) include main problem/conflict,

characters, resolution, and theme in their story grammar. Harris and Graham (1992)

provide detailed story elements in their story map. The eight story grammar units are

as follows.

● Character(s): the main person(s) or animal(s) in the story.

● Locale: the place that the main events happen.

● Time: the specific time that the main events happen.

● Starter:an action that sets up a problem or dilemma for the story.

● Goal: what the main character(s) attempts to achieve.

● Action(s): the attempts by the main character(s) to resolve the problem or conflict.

● Ending: the outcome of the attempts to resolve the problem or conflict.

(14)

● Reaction: how the main character(s) feel toward the ending.

The present study adopted the story grammar in Harris and Graham (1992) since

the model provides a thorough knowledge of story structure which leads the

participants to generate stories with complete story structure.

A story map is a visual depiction of such story grammar. It can be used as

frameworks for storytelling or retelling, and as outlines for story writing. The purpose

of story maps is to assist students as they examine the structure of a story. Students

gain a thorough understanding of the story by identifying the setting, problems, goals,

events, and solutions as portrayed in the narrative text. It is proved that story-mapping

strategy can assist inexperienced writers in organizing their story writing, composing

longer texts with richer lexical items, eliciting more creativity in students’

compositions and cultivating a positive attitude toward writing and a stronger

motivation for it as well.

2.3.2 Values and Related Studies on Story Mapping

There has been a great deal of research on the effects of story mapping. Story

mapping benefits learners’ language learning a lot in many fields. Based on

observation of the research done related to story mapping, Jackie (1989) concludes

that the story mapping strategy can assist language learning in four fields, inclusive of

listening, speaking, reading comprehension, writing fluency. Besides, it is reported

(15)

that story-mapping strategy can lower the writing apprehension as well.

2.3.2.1 Effectiveness of Story Mapping in Improving Reading and Listening

Comprehension.

Many researchers suggest that story maps are effective for improving narrative

comprehension because they provide concreteness, visual and opportunities for active

engagement (Fitzgerald & Spiegel, 1983; Fitzgerald & Teasley, 1986; Idol & Croll,

1987; Baumann & Bergeron, 1993). Story grammar represents the basic structure of a

narrative text, and story maps are templates that graphically represent these elements.

While there are various interpretations of story grammar, there are common story

elements. These basic story elements are beneficial to students’ reading

comprehension (Foley, 2000). For students who come from less literate background,

or who have learning disabilities, explicit instruction of story grammar has proven

beneficial to reading comprehension development (Carnine & Kinder, 1985; Idol,

1987; Idol & Croll, 1987; Montague et al., 1990; Schmelzer & Dickey, 1990; Leaman,

1993; Simmons, 1993).

Story mapping is a helpful instructional teaching tool that can be used before

reading to activate the internal cognitive awareness of story structures, during reading

to promote the story elements, and after reading to diagnostically assess the degree of

comprehension concerning the story (Reutzel, 1984; Davis, 1994). In the late 1970s,

(16)

schema theory becomes pivotal to empirical investigations of encoding and retrieval

processes in reading (Anderson & Pearson, 1984).

As Gurney, Gersten, Dimino, & Carnine, (1990) point out, story mapping directs

students’ attention to relevant elements of stories using a specific structure. Story

maps provide a visual-spatial display for key information in narrative text. These

maps function to prompt learners to identify story elements and provide space for

them to record this information. Story maps may be used before reading a passage to

elicit prior knowledge, facilitate discussion, and record relevant information about a

topic. The use of story maps while reading a passage provides a guide for readers to

record significant information and serves as a review after reading. Story-mapping

procedures have been used, modified, studied, and evaluated with a range of

individuals with various abilities and grade levels (Beck & McKoewn, 1981). For

example, story maps have been utilized to increase reading comprehension skill by

prompting students to recognize story grammar elements, such as characters, setting,

the problem (Dimino, et al.1995); organizing and sequencing story information

(Pearson, 1985); and making connections between story components (Pearson, 1982).

Sorrell (1990) describes story mapping as a tool for providing or building upon prior

knowledge or schema. He explains that story mapping can help student with

interpreting, organizing and comprehending new information prior to, during, and

(17)

after reading stories.

From 80s to early of 21st century, story-mapping instruction has been proved to

have positive influence on different groups of learners. (Singer and Donlan, 1983;

Carnine & Kinder, 1985; Idol & Croll, 1987; Gurney, Gersten, Dimino, & Carnine,

1990; Davis, 1994; Mathes, Fuchs, & Fuchs, 1997; Gardill & Jitendra, 1999;

Boulineau, Fore III, Hagan-Burke, & Burke, 2004). The research conducted to be

related to reading comprehension are fruitful and show promising outcome.

The first explicit systematic instruction in story grammar study is conducted by

Singer and Donlan (1983). The researchers provided instruction of five story grammar

elements to 11th- grade L1 students in an attempt to improve their ability to

comprehend complex short stories in high school literature anthologies. Results from

daily 10-item, multiple choice comprehension assessments indicated that story

grammar components can serve as the basis for an effective instructional strategy.

Carnine & Kinder (1985) report positive effects of story grammar training with

elementary-age students and students with learning disabilities. Effects are reported

over a 4-week delay interval.

Idol & Croll (1987) investigated the effects of story mapping on reading

comprehension. In this study, the L1 students with learning disabilities were taught in

heterogeneous groups along with their non-handicapped peers. A direct instruction

(18)

paradigm was followed, moving from a teacher-modeling phase to a teacher

assistance phase, to an independent practice phase. Four of the five students with

learning disabilities demonstrated significant improvement in response to

comprehension questions, story writing, and listening comprehension.

Idol (1987) used model-lead test procedures to teach how to complete a story

map while reading. The worksheet on the story map required students to fill in story

grammar information that included (a) the setting, including the characters, tine, and

the place;(b) the problem; (c) the goals; (d) the action; and (e) the outcomes of the

story. The participants in the study were third and fourth graders, including five

learning disabled and low achieving students, who were taught to use a story mapping

strategy to improve reading comprehension. The dependent measures were (1) the

percentage of correct, written responses to 10 comprehension questions which refer

directly to the components of the story map, (2) oral scores for reading

comprehension and word meaning on the Nelson Reading Skills Test, and (3)

listening comprehension test. The results indicated that elementary-age students who

were taught these specific procedures improved their responses to written

comprehension questions, listening comprehension and reading comprehension. Idol

(1987) found similar improvements when examining the effects of a story-mapping

strategy on text comprehension in a study of 27 low-achieving 3rd and 4th graders. Idol

(19)

suggested that if the comprehension instruction provided a framework for

understanding, conceptualizing, and remembering important story events, even

students with poor reading comprehension can be successfully accommodated for

comprehension instruction.

Gurney, Gersten, Dimino & Carnine (1990) examined the effectiveness of story

mapping strategy for teaching comprehension of literature to seven high school

students with learning disabilities. These students were divided into three groups,

including two groups of two students from one high school and one group of 3

students from the other school. There were two experimental phases: (a) traditional

basal literature instruction and (b) story grammar instruction. Through learning the

important story elements, namely, main problem/conflict, characters, resolution, and

theme, the participants showed improvements in the students’ ability to answer

questions based on the stories read. The results showed that story grammar instruction

provided the students with a framework that assisted them in comprehending stories at

a more sophisticated level.

Mathes, Fuchs, & Fuchs (1997) explored the effects of Cooperative Story

Mapping (CSM) on L1 students’ reading comprehension. The story map applied in

this study was based on the work of Idol (1987) and Idol and Croll (1987). It included

main characters, setting, problem or conflict, major events, and outcome or resolution.

(20)

On average, students had made significantly great fluency and comprehension

improvement from pretest to posttest. Additionally, both teachers and students

reported high levels of satisfaction with Cooperative Story Mapping. According to the

teachers, CSM was important to the reading achievement of their students (mean

score: 4.63 with the total score of 5). They would be likely to continue to use CSM in

the future (mean score: 4.68). Working in cooperative group enhances reading

achievement for all leaner types (mean score: 4.58). Students said they enjoy CSM

(mean score: 4.22) and they believed the procedure had helped them to become better

readers (mean score: 4.28).

Gardill & Jitendra (1999) used a multiple baseline across participants to

determine the effects of advanced story-map instruction on the reading

comprehension of six L1 students in 6th and 8th grade identified with SLD. The

independent variable was a story map that provides space for students to write the

significant information from stories read. Results indicate an increase on the basal

comprehension tests for each participant.

Boulineau, Fore III, Hagan-Burke, & Burke, (2004) examined the use of

story-mapping to improve the reading comprehension of six 3rd and 4th grade L1

students with specific learning disabilities who exhibited reading deficits. Using a

descriptive, ABC phased single participant design, the effect of story instruction on

(21)

students’ comprehension of story-grammar elements was monitored. The ABC phase

included Phase A: the baseline phase, Phase B: the intervention phase, in which the

elements of story grammar are explicitly taught using a story map as a visual aid and

an organizer for guided practice and Phase C: the maintenance phase. The results of

the study showed that story grammar instruction improved the students’ identification

of story elements when reading narrative text. Each participant’s percentage of correct

story grammar elements increased from baseline to intervention conditions. The

effectiveness of story mapping strategy still maintained and lasted once the instruction

was terminated.

In Taiwan, an EFL learning environment, there is also some research conducted

to investigate the effects of story mapping on reading comprehension. However, the

participants were mostly elementary-age students or students with different learning

disabilities. The following are the results of the related studies.

Based on the model of Mathes, Fuchs, and Fuchs (1997), Yeh (2002) investigated

the effects of cooperative story mapping on the Chinese reading comprehension of the

elementary average students and students with low-performing reading. The

participants included 64 average students and 8 low-performing reading students. The

researcher found that cooperative story mapping could not only promote the reading

comprehension of average students but also helped the students of low reading

(22)

comprehension in reading. Also, the students expressed high level of satisfaction and

looked forward to an implementation of cooperative story mapping in the future.

Chen (2003) reported the positive effects of story grammar instruction on the

Chinese reading comprehension of five 2nd grade elementary students with reading

disabilities. All the five subjects’ scores of story grammar questions increased after

the story mapping instruction and its effectiveness lasted for at least 3 weeks. Another

finding was that the participants’ performance in 6 story grammar components were

improved, especially in main “character” and “setting.”

Chang (2003) reported that all the three participants performed better on their

reading comprehension when using the story mapping strategy. The results of this

study suggested that story-grammar instruction improved the participants’

identification of story grammar elements via story mapping when reading a narrative

text. Lin (2003) also reported similar positive results when examining the effects of

story mapping on reading comprehension of students with learning disabilities.

Additionally, story mapping helps with students’ listening comprehension of

stories. According to Idol & Croll (1987) and Idol (1987), the students demonstrated

improvement in their listening comprehension of stories after receiving the story

mapping instruction because the instruction provided them with the knowledge of

story structure which enabled them to predict what may happen next in the stories

(23)

they heard and comprehended the stories better.

2.3.2.2 Effectiveness of Story Mapping in Improving Story Telling

Successful use of story structure as a comprehension strategy involves the

integration of various story elements. In preparing to tell a story, students map the

story’s structure and make meaningful connection between the components.

Jackie (1989) demonstrated that story mapping helped storytelling since story

maps provided organized visual aid of story elements which helped L1 students with

story telling. Having the structure in mind enabled students to tell their stories without

stilted memorization of lines. Cannizzar & Coelho (2002) investigated the treatment

of story grammar ability, in an 39-year-old individual TBI (Traumatic Brain Injury).

Treatment emphasized mata-linguistic comprehension of story grammar structure and

the identification and generation of episode components within stories. Over the

course of treatment, a marked increased in the number of complete episodes generated

by the individual with TBI was noted in story grammar probe.

2.3.2.3 Effectiveness of Story Mapping in Lowering Students’ Writing

Apprehension in the Target Language

Researches find that students who are apprehensive about writing or who lack

confidence in their writing ability are less motivated to write frequently, write less

well when they do (Charney, Newman, Palmquist, 1995). Pajares and Johnson (1994)

(24)

also find that undergraduates who are apprehensive about writing have lower

confidence scores and that the ratings of their writing abilities are predictive of

writing performance. Writing anxiety leads to difficulties in producing effective and

coherent written pieces. Students with writing anxiety have problems in writing

anything from simple letters to complex reports. Studies which have sought to

determine the causes of WA (Thompson, 1979; Walsh, 1986; Pajares, 1997) have

found that students’ fear of writing exposure is possibly due to inadequate practice.

Since writing anxiety is caused by a writer’s lack of writing skills, improving those

skills should reduce writing apprehension. An accepted method of improving

students’ writing skills is through the prewriting activities, in particular, the story

mapping.

Since story mapping provides beginning writers with story schema, it might help

to develop the students’ positive attitude toward and stronger motivation for EFL

writing. With the assistance of story mapping, EFL writers can lower their anxiety

level and apprehension about writing in the target language. It is shown that those

with low writing apprehension should perform better on tests of writing skills than

highly apprehensive writers (Lee, 1990; Schweiker-Marra & Marra, 2000; Hassan,

2001). Lee (1990) suggests that training of writing skills, such as prewriting activities

helps decrease writing apprehension of EFL students. Harris & Graham (1992) reports

(25)

that most L1 students feel comfortable with writing after receiving the story mapping

instruction. Since story mapping, one of the prewriting activities, provides beginning

writers with story schema, it might help to develop the students’ positive attitude

toward and stronger motivation for EFL writing. With the assistance of story mapping,

EFL writers can lower their anxiety level and apprehension about writing in the target

language.

2.3.2.4 Effectiveness of Story Mapping in Improving Student’s Story Writing

Performance

Noyce and Christie (1989) point out that lack of story sense undoubtedly inhibits

the students’ abilities to write their own stories. Moreover, students’ writing lacking

coherent information may be randomly stated and have key parts missing. Therefore,

it is suggested that students be given an organized way of getting started when

confronted with writing tasks. According to Ruddell (2004), mapping is an

organizational activity. It therefore precedes writing naturally by providing visual

representation of students’ constructions and organizations of knowledge. While

writing from maps, students can focus on one or more aspects of this organized

scheme and then elaborate their knowledge construction with written text. Mapping is

the primary vehicle for organizing, selecting, and connection information. With the

assistance of story mapping, students are able write well-formed, well-organized, and

(26)

rich-content stories. Students who need help with organizing ideas for fiction stories

can be encouraged to take a few minutes to write down their thoughts on a story map

before writing. This map becomes the framework that guides the writer in keeping

their stories going.

There are three issues involved in the story-mapping strategy to writing. The first

issue is: Can the story-mapping strategy promote students’ writing ability? According

to Brown (1988), the story organizer, one of the three pre-writing strategies in the

study, has best effects on the quality of L1 students’ narrative writing. The participants

in this study, including twenty-four 4th-grade L1 students, received different types of

prewriting activities, with Experimental Group 1 using the story organizer activity,

Experimental Group 2 receiving the webbing activity, and the Control Group

receiving the brainstorming activity. The three prewriting activities had different

focuses. The story organizer was a prewriting activity designed with questions about

story elements to guide the writing of narrative stories, including the setting, initiating

event, internal response, attempt, consequence, and reaction; while the webbing

activity used a visual clustering of ideas that related to the topic and the brainstorming

activity encouraged students to list any ideas that came to their minds relating to the

topic. The participants were given 15 minutes to plan and 30 minutes to write their

narratives. Although the results showed that Control Group scored higher than

(27)

Experimental Group 1 and Experimental Group 2, Experimental Group 1 undergoing

the story organizer scored significantly better than Experimental Group Two and

Control Group when the primary story elements were analyzed. In this study, the story

organizer was the best prewriting activity that helped narrative writing. Also,

Fitzgerald and Teasley (1986) find that when 4th-grade L1 students receiving

instruction on narrative structure, the overall quality and the organization of their

writing was improved. The participants (N=20) were divided into a control group and

an experimental group. The former received instruction on narrative story structure.

The latter received instruction on dictionary skills and word usage. The results

demonstrated that direct instruction of narrative structure did facilitate the

organization of children’s writing. In Taiwan, Hsu (2001), who investigated the direct

instruction of story mapping on Chinese writing proficiency, reported that the 61 6th

graders performed significantly better in writing fluency after the story mapping

instruction. The experimental group outperformed the control group in the “total

score”, “content”, and “organization” of writing performance.

The second issue is: Do the students’ work contain more story elements after the

story mapping instruction? Vallecorsa & deBettencourt (1997) reported the positive

effects of direct transfer reading training on story writing performance. The three

7th-grade L1 students with learning disabilities were taught to use a story map that

(28)

identified elements of the story form as an aid to story recall and story writing

performance. The scale identified eight important story elements developed by

MacArthur and Graham: main characters, locale, time, starter event, goals, ending,

and characters’ reaction. The results showed that the story mapping strategy was an

effective way to help students develop the knowledge of story structure and in turn,

improve their reading and writing performance. Harris & Graham (1992) dealt with

the two issues discussed above. They collected the research results by observing

Danoff’s writing class for one year. They reported positive effects of story mapping

on learners’ writing performance by analyzing the stories written in the pretest and

posttest. Danoff worked with a group of fifth grade L1 students, including both

normally achieving students and several students with learning disabilities with

writing, with a class period a day devoted to the story writing approach. The results

showed that all students made improvement in story writing by including greater

detail and elaboration as well as more “goals” and “actions.” Harris & Graham

indicated that the students had shown improvement in the number of story elements

included in their stories. Although a few of the students were already using all the

story elements in the beginning of the instruction, they showed improvement in

detail and action in their stories as well as improvement in their quality of their

writing after receiving the instruction. Additionally, although some students still

(29)

experienced anxiety about composing and writing difficulties, most students reported

they enjoy writing and were comfortable with it after receiving the story mapping

strategy. According to Saddler, Moran, Graham and Harris (2004), the 6 African

American second-graders composed longer stories with more story elements and

richer story content after the prewriting planning instruction of story elements. Similar

improvement in personal narratives also occurred for all students but one.

The third issue is: Do students make progress in each story elements? In Fine’s

(1991) research, the participants included 77 L1 2nd graders. The teacher provided

direct instruction of story grammar, inclusive of characters, setting, and plot, in the

experimental group. The results of ANOVAs on the means for the posttest story

grammar scores, total adapted-Glazer Narrative Composition Scale scores and holistic

scale scores showed significant improvement in writing achievement of the

experimental group over the control group. Qualitative analysis showed that students

had a good understanding of the concept of character at the pretest and posttest

interview. Eight students who could not manipulate “plot” before the instruction could

do so after direct instruction in story grammar. Five students who could not identify or

manipulate “setting” could do so after direct instruction in story grammar. Moreover,

the students in the experimental group also reported that they would think of

characters, setting, and plot before writing a story after receiving knowledge of story

(30)

grammar as a strategy.

Given the studies reviewed above, it has been shown that story mapping

instruction benefits L1 story writing a lot. It is a pity that few of the studies discuss

the correlation among the three issues and investigate which story elements are

difficult for students to manipulate. Besides, many of the studies focused on the L1

students with learning disabilities instead of average students. To the best of our

knowledge, no research here in Taiwan has been conducted to investigate the effects

of the story-mapping instruction on the EFL average students. Therefore, the present

study aims to investigate the effects of story mapping instruction on students’ story

structure, writing performance and explore which story elements are challenging to

the students.

2.4 Summary

Through the review of the literature on prewriting strategies and story mapping,

it is found that the prewriting strategy, story mapping, can help beginning writers with

their writing performance. The prewriting activities play a crucial role in the writing

process. They involve organizing, generating, and goal setting. It is within this

component that many teachers provide activities to help students get started, and to

organize and generate their thoughts. It has been proposed that story mapping is

helpful as a prewriting activity to facilitate the generation of students’ narrative

(31)

writing since this strategy provides language learners the graphically representation of

the story structure.

參考文獻

相關文件

Write the following problem on the board: “What is the area of the largest rectangle that can be inscribed in a circle of radius 4?” Have one half of the class try to solve this

Reading Task 6: Genre Structure and Language Features. • Now let’s look at how language features (e.g. sentence patterns) are connected to the structure

(a) In your group, discuss what impact the social issues in Learning Activity 1 (and any other socials issues you can think of) have on the world, Hong Kong and you.. Choose the

We explicitly saw the dimensional reason for the occurrence of the magnetic catalysis on the basis of the scaling argument. However, the precise form of gap depends

The packed comparison instructions compare the destination (second) operand to the source (first) oper- and to test for equality or greater than.. These instructions compare eight

Microphone and 600 ohm line conduits shall be mechanically and electrically connected to receptacle boxes and electrically grounded to the audio system ground point.. Lines in

Eliciting young children’s perceptions of play, work and learning using the activity apperception story procedure.. Work and play in

1、 Focusing on the unit of “circle”, the variation of the learning achievement between the experimental group and the control group reached significant difference3. The variation