• 沒有找到結果。

Tridimensional Personality of Adolescents With Internet Addiction and Substance Use Experience

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Tridimensional Personality of Adolescents With Internet Addiction and Substance Use Experience"

Copied!
8
0
0

加載中.... (立即查看全文)

全文

(1)

Tridimensional Personality of Adolescents With

Internet Addiction and Substance Use Experience

Chih-Hung Ko, MD

1,2

, Ju-Yu Yen, MD

1,3

, Cheng-Chung Chen, MD, PhD

4,5

, Sue-Huei Chen, PhD

6

,

Kuanyi Wu, MD

1

, Cheng-Fang Yen, MD, PhD

4,5

Key Words: Internet addiction, substance abuse, adolescents, personality, harm avoidance Objective: This study aimed to examine the differences in personality characteristics between

adolescents with and without Internet addiction and substance use experience as defined by the Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire (TPQ), and to compare personality characteristics among groups of adolescents with both Internet addiction and substance use experience (comorbid group), those with only Internet addition (Internet addiction group), those with only substance use experience (substance experience group), and those without Internet addiction or substance use experience (control group).

Method: In the cross-sectional investigation, we recruited 3662 students (2328 boys and

1334 girls) from high schools in southern Taiwan. Our investigation was conducted using the TPQ, the Chen Internet Addiction Scale, and Questionnaires for Experience in Substance Use.

Results: Adolescents with Internet addiction were more likely to have substance use

experience. High novelty seeking (NS), high harm avoidance (HA), and low reward dependence (RD) predicted a higher proportion of adolescents with Internet addiction. High NS, low HA, and low RD predicted a higher proportion of adolescents with substance use experience. Of the 4 groups, the Internet addiction group had the highest HA scores and the comorbid group had the lowest HA scores.

Conclusion: Adolescents with high NS and low RD should be provided with effective

strategies for preventing Internet addiction and substance use. In addition, the Internet addiction group and the comorbid group should be provided with different preventative strategies focused on HA.

(Can J Psychiatry 2006;51:887–894)

Information on funding and support and author affiliations appears at the end of the article.

Clinical Implications

· Adolescents with Internet addiction had a higher risk of substance use experience.

· Preventive strategies for Internet addiction and substance use should be provided for adolescents with high NA and low RD.

· Different preventive strategies focusing on HA should be provided to the Internet addiction group and to the comorbid group, respectively.

Limitations

· Our investigations relied on self-reported data from adolescents.

· Social restrictions on substance use may make adolescents unwilling to admit substance use, even in anonymous questionnaires.

· The study’s cross-sectional design could not confirm causal relations between Internet addiction and personality.

(2)

I

n recent years, Internet and computer use have become pop-ular worldwide. However, this has also had a negative impact on some individuals and on society at large. The devel-opment of addiction to the Internet is the most prominent dele-terious impact. Preoccupation with the Internet, like gambling, can interrupt daily life to the point that an individ-ual neglects other productive and creative activities. Owing to the media characteristics of the Internet, its use has also been associated with other risky behaviour such as substance use (1,2). Substance use has been found to negatively affect adolescents’ health (3); thus, identification of the risk factors of Internet addiction and substance use experience is of clinical significance as risky behaviour can be identified and preventive measures can be instituted.

Much attention has been paid to the predictive value of per-sonality characteristics for addictive behaviours. Perper-sonality is reported to play an important role in the predisposition, pre-cipitation, and perpetuation of substance use disorder (4). Many studies have shown the characteristics of an “addictive personality,” which include impulsivity, sensation seeking, psychoticism, and a tendency toward social deviance (5–9). These results provide essential information for substance use prevention.

In contrast to reports on substance abuse, there have been few studies on behavioural addiction. Pathological gambling is the most commonly studied behavioural addiction and is thought to share similar clinical and neuropsychiatric presen-tations with substance use disorder (10–12). Internet addic-tion is a newly noted behavioural addicaddic-tion (13). Ko and others proposed a diagnostic criteria for adolescent Internet addiction that is composed of Criteria A, B, and C. Criterion A contains 9 characteristic symptoms of Internet addiction, including preoccupation, uncontrolled impulse, more than intended use, tolerance, withdrawal, impairment of control, excessive time and effort spent on the Internet, and impair-ment of decision-making ability. Criterion B describes func-tional impairment secondary to Internet use. Criterion C lists

the exclusive symptoms to eliminate the possibility of psy-chotic disorder and bipolar I disorder. The cut-off point with 6 symptoms of Criterion A had the best diagnostic accuracy (95.4%), with high specificity (97.1%) and accepted sensitivity (87.5%) (14).

Criterion A of Internet addiction demonstrates clinical symp-toms similar to the criteria of substance dependence and pathological gambling of the DSM-IV (10). In addition, Internet addiction has been correlated with personality char-acteristics, high sensation seeking, and disinhibition (15). These traits were also reported to be associated with substance use disorder. Evaluation and comparison of the personality traits associated with Internet addiction and substance use is essential for developing Internet addiction prevention strate-gies that are based on a well-established model of substance use prevention. To our knowledge, however, no study has compared the personality characteristics of adolescents who are addicted to the Internet with those who suffer from substance use.

Cloninger (16) proposed 3 dimensions of human tempera-ment based on NS, HA, and RD. NS is hypothesized to be a heritable tendency for intense exhilaration or excitement to novel stimuli. HA is a heritable tendency to respond intensely to signals of aversive stimuli. RD is a heritable tendency to respond intensely to signals of reward (particularly verbal sig-nals of social approval and sentiment) and to maintain or resist extinction of rewarded behaviour. The tridimensional person-ality model may provide a basis for analyzing the interaction between behavioural traits and environmental stimuli to pre-dict personal proneness to an adpre-dictive behaviour (17,18). However, we could find no studies that examined the relation between personality characteristics on the TPQ and Internet addiction.

This study has 2 purposes. First, we examined the differences in personality characteristics on the TPQ between adolescents with and without Internet addiction, as well as those with and without substance exposure. Second, we compared the per-sonality characteristics among adolescents with both Internet addiction and substance use experience (comorbid group), with only Internet addiction (Internet addiction group), with only substance use experience (substance experience group), and without Internet Addiction or substance use experience (control group).

Method

Participants

For evaluation, we selected 7 of 87 junior high schools, 6 of 33 senior high schools, and 4 of 20 vocational high schools at Kaohsiung City and County in Taiwan. The selected schools included 8, 5, and 3 schools from urban, suburban, and rural areas, respectively. We randomly selected 2 classes from each

Abbreviations used in this article

ANOVA analysis of variance

CIAS Chen Internet Addiction Scale HA harm avoidance

LSD Fisher Least Significant Difference test NS novelty seeking

OR odds ratio

Q-ESU Questionnaires for Experience in Substance Use RD reward dependence

SD standard deviation

(3)

grade in these schools. We recruited 3662 students (2328 male and 1334 female students). Their mean age was 15.48 years (SD 1.65, range 11 to 21). There was a significant sex differ-ence between eligible students and the population of high school students (÷2= 212.39, P < 0.001) because we selected more classes in senior and vocational high schools, where there were more male students and more technologically ori-ented classes. After we obtained informed consent, we invited all the students in the selected classes to anonymously com-plete a questionnaire. We omitted 250 participants because 23.2%, 29.2%, and 54.8% of them did not complete the CIAS, the Q-ESU, and the TPQ, respectively. The excluded partici-pants were more likely to be male (÷2= 22.8, P < 0.001) and older (t295= 3.14, P = 0.002) than other participants. For the

final statistical analysis, we used data for the 3412 (93.2%) participants (2134 boys and 1278 girls). The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Kaohsiung Municipal Hsiao-Kang Hospital.

Measurement Instruments

Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire. The Chinese ver-sion of the TPQ contains 100 self-administered true or false questions designed to measure the NS, HA, and RD dimen-sions of personality (19). The 1-month test–retest reliability was 0.58 to 0.77. It also had acceptable construct validity (8). Chen Internet Addiction Scale. The CIAS contains 26 items on a 4-point Likert scale that assesses 5 dimensions of Internet related problems: compulsive use, withdrawal, tolerance, interpersonal and health problems, and time management problems. The internal reliability of the scale and the subscales in the original study ranged from 0.79 to 0.93 (20). According to the diagnostic criteria of Internet addiction (14), the 63 out of 64 cut-off point of the CIAS had the highest diag-nostic accuracy (87.6%), accepted sensitivity (67.8%), and specificity (92.6%) (21). Accordingly, we classified individu-als with CIAS scores of 64 or more as the Internet addiction group.

Questionnaires for Experience in Substance Use. The Q-ESU asked whether participants currently used tobacco, alcohol, or areca (betel nut) on a regular basis or whether they had ever used or experimented with cannabis, amphetamines, glue, h e r o i n , 3 , 4 - me t h yl e n e d io x y me t h a mp h e t a mi n e , o r ketamine (22).

Statistical Analysis

The participants who fit the diagnostic cut-off point of the CIAS were classified as Internet addicts. Since the most com-mon pattern of substance use in community adolescents is experimental or recreational use that does not usually meet the DSM-IV criteria for substance use or dependence (23) and since any pattern of illicit substance use by adolescents is potentially hazardous, lifetime illicit substance use is usually

used to define the adolescents who are in the at-risk group (23,24). Conversely, having used alcohol or tobacco is common among adolescents (23); thus, it is appropriate to examine current and regular tobacco and alcohol use to define an at-risk group of adolescents (25, 26). In this study, we defined subjects with substance use experience as adolescents who had a lifetime experience of illicit substance use or who regularly used alcohol, tobacco, or areca, as determined by the Q-ESU.

We compared the personality characteristics on the TPQ of the adolescents with and without Internet addiction and sub-stance use experience, using t tests and logistic regression. We used ANOVA and post hoc tests with Fisher LSD tests to fur-ther compare the personality characteristics on the TPQ of the comorbid group, the Internet addiction group, the substance experience group, and the control group. All statistical analy-ses were performed with the SPSS computer program (Version 10.0, SPSS Inc, 2005). We considered any P value less than 0.05 significant.

Results

Table 1 compares demographic characteristics, experience with substance use, and personality characteristics on the TPQ of adolescents with and without Internet addiction. Adoles-cents with Internet addiction were more likely to be male (÷2=114.29, P < 0.001), to be students at vocational schools (÷2 = 64.16, P < 0.001), to be older (t1206= 5.10, P < 0.001),

and to have experienced substance use (÷2= 59.08, P < 0.001). Adolescents with Internet addiction had higher scores on the NS (t1180= 7.67, P < 0.001) and HA (t3410= 2.63, P = 0.009)

dimensions and lower scores on the RD dimension (t3410=

–5.75, P < 0.001) than those without Internet addiction. Table 2 compares between the demographic characteristics and personality characteristics of adolescents with and with-out substance use experience. Adolescents with substance use experience were more likely to be male (÷2= 62.43, P < 0.001), to be students at vocational schools (÷2= 42.72, P < 0.001), to be older (t3401= 5.37, P < 0.001), and to have

Internet addiction (÷2= 59.08, P < 0.001). Adolescents with substance use experience had higher scores on the NS dimen-sion (t3410= 7.03, P < 0.001) and lower scores on the HA

(t3410= –4.23, P < 0.001) and RD (t3410= –4.13, P < 0.001)

dimensions than those without substance use experience. Table 2 shows the results of the logistic regression model used to predict Internet addiction and substance use experience. Sex, age, and school were controlled in the model. Higher NS (OR 1.06 to 1.11), higher HA (OR 1.02 to 1.05), and lower RD (OR 0.93 to 0.97) significantly predicted Internet addiction, but NS was the most significant predictor for Internet addic-tion. Higher NS (OR 1.07 to.13), lower HA (OR 0.96 to 0.995), and lower RD (OR 0.92 to 0.98) significantly

(4)

predicted substance use experience. Higher NS was also the most significant predictor of substance use experience. We further classified the participants into comorbid, Internet addiction, substance experience, and control groups. Table 4 shows the differences in personality on the TPQ among these 4 groups as analyzed by ANOVA. There were significant dif-ferences in NS (F3,3408= 20.74, P < 0.001), HA (F3,3408= 40.27,

P < 0.001), and RD (F3,3408= 13.99, P < 0.001) across the 4

groups. LSD post hoc tests showed that the comorbid group, the substance experience group, and the Internet addiction group had significantly higher NS scores than the control group. The comorbid group had significantly higher NS scores than the Internet addiction group. The Internet addic-tion group had significantly higher HA scores than control group. The control group had significantly higher HA scores than the substance experience and the comorbid groups. The comorbid group, the substance experience group, and the Internet addiction group had significantly lower RD scores than the control group.

Discussion

Previous studies have reported that high NS is associated with adolescent substance use (8,27). Sensation seeking, which is a trait similar to NS, has also been associated with Internet dependence and higher Internet use (15,28). This study revealed that high NS is the most significant predictor for

Internet addiction and substance use experience in adoles-cents. Since NS is thought to reflect the brain’s incentive, or behaviour activation, system and is associated with the dopa-mine system (29), individuals with high NS readily engage in new interests and activities but tend to neglect details and are quickly distracted or bored (16). Internet activities, especially online games, provide a highly varied virtual environment that satisfies the adolescents’ NS needs. Adolescents with high NA might engage in Internet activity with higher motiva-tion and arousal responses. Therefore, high NS may predis-pose an individual to heavy Internet use. This is similar to the effect of high NS on substance use experience, as reported in this study and Cloninger’s research (30).

Associations between substance use, NS, and dopamine func-tion have been reported (31). The mesocorticolimbic dopa-mine system, originating from the ventral tegmental area and projecting toward a wide range of the limbic structure, is asso-ciated with NS (29). The association between high NS and Internet addicts may reveal that, as in substance addiction, Internet addiction is possibly associated with an impaired dopamine system.

HA is thought to reflect variation in the brain’s punishment, or behaviour inhibition, system, which includes the septohippocampal system, with serotonergic projections from the raphe nuclei in the brain system. Individuals with low HA are confident, optimistic, carefree, uninhibited, and

Table 1 Comparisons of demographic characteristics, experience with substance use, and personality characteristics between adolescents with and without Internet addiction (n = 3412) Internet addiction Characteristic Yes (n = 706) No (n = 2706) c2or t P Sex, n (%) Male participants 564 (79.9) 1570 (58.0) 114.29 <0.001 Female participants 142 (20.1) 1136 (42.0) School, n (%)

Junior high school 247 (35.0) 1182 (43.7) 64.16 < 0.001

Senior high school 189 (31.7) 898 (33.2)

Vocational high school 270 (38.2) 626 (23.1)

Substance use experience, n (%)

Yes 115 (16.3) 190 (7.0) 59.08 < 0.001 No 591 (83.7) 2516 (93.0) Age, mean (SD) 15.7 (1.5) 15.4 (1.7) 5.1 < 0.001 TDQ, mean (SD) NS 18.2 (4.3) 16.8 (4.7) 7.67 < 0.001 HA 18.2 (6.1) 17.5 (6.5) 2.63 0.009 RD 16.4 (3.7) 17.3 (3.8) –5.75 < 0.001

(5)

energetic (16). Low HA is reportedly associated with early substance use and frequent alcohol use in adolescents (27,32). In the present study, low HA scores predicted substance use experience. The results correspond to previous reports and assumptions that high HA inhibits risk behaviour with nega-tive results. In this study, high HA predicted Internet addic-tion. Since the Internet provides an anonymous virtual world, adolescents usually perceive less responsibility and harm from it than they do from the real world. Consequently, the online disinhibition effect (33) may relax adolescents with high HA in real life and make them vulnerable to Internet addiction.

RD is thought to reflect variations in the brain system that facilitates the acquisition of conditioned signals or reward or relief from punishment (16). In the present study, low RD scores significantly predicted Internet addiction and sub-stance use experience. Adolescents with low RD are impaired in their responsiveness to verbal approval and social rein-forcement, and they have poor persistence (16). They demon-strate little tolerance for unpredictable frustrations in real life. Immediate and predictable achievement from Internet activity such as online gaming could therefore provide satisfactory resources for novelty and esteem without unpredictable frus-tration. Consequently, low RD predicted the development of Internet addiction.

The personality characteristics of adolescents with Internet addiction included high NS, high HA, and low RD. Adoles-cents with substance use experience had high NS, low HA, and low RD. Kuo and others (8) reported similar results for adolescents with substance use. ANOVA analysis revealed that the comorbid group had significantly higher NS and lower HA than the Internet addiction group. The comorbid group and the Internet addiction group had significantly lower and higher HA than the control group, respectively. This shows that individuals in the comorbid group had personalities that distinctly differed from those in the Internet addiction group. There were no differences between the sub-stance experience group and the comorbid group. Similar to the antisocial personality defined by the TPQ (16), the sub-stance experience and comorbid groups had higher NS, lower HA, and lower RD. This indicated that the comorbid group and the substance experience group might be classified as one group that is different from the Internet addiction group. Since the comorbid group have personalities that differ from the Internet addiction group, they may have different reasons for using the Internet. Individuals in the Internet addiction group, with high HA scores, may use Internet activity to avoid stress and to alleviate fear of real life harm. They use the Internet because they feel it is a safe way to satisfy their needs. Thus they experience little risky or problematic behaviour in real life. Conversely, individuals in the comorbid group may

Table 2 Comparisons of demographic characteristics and personality characteristics between adolescents with and without substance use experience (n = 3412)

Substance use experience

Characteristic Yes (n = 305) No (n = 3107) c2 or t P Sex, n (%) Male participants 255 (83.6) 1879 (60.5) 62.43 < 0.001 Female participants 50 (16.4) 1228 (39.5) School, n (%)

Junior high school 99 (32.5) 1330 (42.8) 42.72 < 0.001

Senior high school 78 (25.6) 1009 (32.5)

Vocational high school 128 (42.0) 768 (24.7)

Internet addiction, n (%) Yes 115 (37.7) 591 (19.0) 59.08 < 0.001 No 190 (62.3) 2516 (81.0) Age, mean (SD) 15.9 (1.7) 15.4 (1.7) 5.37 < 0.001 TDQ, mean (SD) NS 18.8 (4.4) 16.9 (4.6) 7.03 < 0.001 HA 16.2 (6.2) 17.8 (6.4) –4.23 <0.001 RD 16.3 (3.6) 17.2 (3.8) –4.13 < 0.001

(6)

use Internet activity to satisfy their NS. They may be impul-sive or aggresimpul-sive in real life and they try risky behaviour both online and in real life. Hence, the Internet addiction group and comorbid group need different intervention strategies. Ado-lescents with low HA should receive more attention in the form of preventive strategies for substance use.

Although there is a lack of evidence to clarify the causal rela-tion between personality and Internet addicrela-tion and substance use, we suggest 2 models to explain why the comorbid group is distinctly different from the Internet addiction group. The first model indicates that vulnerable personality charac-teristics increase the risk of Internet addiction and substance use experience. Individuals in the comorbid and Internet

addiction groups were attracted to the Internet because of their personality characteristics (high NS and low RD). The Internet addiction group, however, did not engage in sub-stance use because of high HA. Adolescents with contrary HA therefore comprise the Internet addiction group, and the HA characteristic determines the risk of substance use problems. In the second model, personality differences are the result of substance use. Substance use has been reported to damage the frontal lobe (34), which impairs the inhibiting function; hence, adolescent substance use lowers HA. This effect shows why the comorbid group had different personality characteris-tics from the Internet addiction group. Which model most appropriately explains the results of this study may be evalu-ated with a prospective study.

Table 3 Odds ratios of the TPQ (NS, HA, and RD) for Internet addiction and substance use experience, controlling for sex, school, and grades

Internet addiction Substance use experience

Personality type Waldc2 P OR (95%CI) Wald

c2 P OR (95%CI)

NS 65.63 <0.001 1.08 (1.06–1.11) 46.18 < 0.001 1.10 (1.07–1.13)

HA 25.15 <0.001 1.04 (1.02–1.05) 5.88 <0.02 0.98 (0.96–0.995)

RD 18.11 <0.001 0.95 (0.93–0.97) 11.76 < 0.001 0.95 (0.92–0.98)

Table 4 Comparisons of TPQ score among participants with Internet addiction only, with substance use experience only, with both Internet addiction and substance use experience, and without Internet addiction or substance use experience

Personality type Mean (SD)

Degrees of

freedom F P Post hoc analysis (LSD)

NS

Control subjects 16.7 (4.6) 3, 3408 20.74 < 0.001 Comorbid > internet addicts > control subjects

Internet addiction 18.0 (4.2) Substance use experience > control subjects

Substance use experience 18.6 (4.5)

Comorbid 19.3 (4.3)

HA

Control subjects 17.6 (6.4) 3, 3408 40.27 < 0.001 Internet addicts > control subjects > substance use experience

Internet addiction 18.6 (5.9) Internet addicts > control subjects > comorbid

Substance use experience 16.3 (6.2)

Comorbid 16.1 (6.4)

RD

Control subjects 17.4 (3.8) 3, 3408 13.99 < 0.001 Control subjects > Internet addicts

Internet addiction 16.5 (3.7) Control subjects > substance use experience

Substance use experience 16.6 (3.6) Control subjects > comorbid

Comorbid 15.8 (3.5)

Comorbid = the comorbidity of Internet addiction and substance use experience Control subjects = participants without Internet addiction or substance use experience

(7)

Our results should be interpreted in light of several limita-tions. First, all of our investigations relied on self-reported data from adolescents. Second, social restrictions on sub-stance use may predispose adolescents to hiding subsub-stance use even in anonymous questionnaires. Third, the cross-sectional research design of the present study could not confirm causal relations between Internet addiction and per-sonality even if the perper-sonality were presumed stable. Fourth, the study population included more boys, although the sex effect was controlled in regression analysis. Finally, the miss-ing data include more boys and older participants. The effects of sex and age in the missing data need to be further studied.

Conclusion

High NS, high HA, and low RD are significant predictors for Internet addiction. This suggests that effective preventive strategies providing healthy activities that satisfy adolescents’ NS needs, as well as behaviour interventions that provide con-sistent and practical rewards to motivate adolescents with low RD, are essential. High NS, low HA, and low RD predict sub-stance use experience. Individuals in the comorbid group have distinctly different personalities, compared with those in the Internet addiction group, and the comorbid group mem-bers share similar personality characteristics with adolescents with substance use experience. Different preventive strategies focusing on HA should be provided to the Internet addiction group and comorbid group, respectively. Strategies to prevent substance abuse should be provided to adolescents with high NS, low HA, and low RD to protect them from exposure to harmful substances.

Funding and Support

This study was supported by a grant of the National Science Council of Taiwan (NSC 92–2413-H-037–005 SSS).

References

1. Halpern JH, Pope HG Jr. Hallucinogens on the Internet: a vast new source of underground drug information. Am J Psychiatry 2001;158:481–3. 2. Rich M, Bar-On M. Child health in the information age: media education of

pediatricians. Pediatrics 2001;107:156 –62.

3. Donovan JE. Adolescent alcohol initiation: a review of psychosocial risk factors. J Adolesc Health 2004;35:529.e7–18.

4. Le Bon O, Basiaux P, Streel E, Tecco J, Hanak C, Hansenne M, and others. Personality profile and drug of choice: a multivariate analysis using Cloninger’s TCI on heroin addicts, alcoholics, and a random population group. Drug Alcohol Depend 2004;73:175– 82.

5. Allen TJ, Moeller FG, Rhoades HM, Cherek DR. Impulsivity and history of drug dependence. Drug Alcohol Depend 1998;50:137– 45.

6. Eysenck HJ. Addiction, personality and motivation. Hum Psychopharmacol 1997;12:79– 87.

7. Kosten TA, Ball SA, Rounsaville BJ. A sibling study of sensation seeking and opiate addiction. J Nerv Ment Dis 1994;182:284 –9.

8. Kuo PH, Yang HJ, Soong WT, Chen WJ. Substance use among adolescents in Taiwan: associated personality traits, incompetence, and behavioral/emotional problems. Drug Alcohol Depend 2002;67:27–39.

9. Sarramon C, Verdoux H, Schmitt L, Bourgeois M. Addiction and personality traits: sensation seeking, anhedonia, impulsivity. Encephale 1999;25:569–75. 10. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders. 4th ed. Washington (DC): American Psychiatric Publishing; 1994.

11. Holden C. ‘Behavioral’ addictions: do they exist? Science 2001;294:980–2. 12. Potenza MN, Steinberg MA, Skudlarski P, Fulbright RK, Lacadie CM, Wilber

MK, and others. Gambling urges in pathological gambling: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003;60:828–36.

13. Young KS. Internet addiction: The emergence of a new clinical disorder. Cyberpsychol Behav 1998;1:237–44.

14. Ko CH, Yen JY, Chen CC, Chen SH, Yen CF. Proposed diagnostic criteria of Internet addiction for adolescents. J Nerv Ment Dis 2005;193:728–33. 15. Lin SSJ, Tsai CC. Sensation seeking and Internet dependence of Taiwanese high

school adolescents. Comput Human Behav 2002;18:411–26.

16. Cloninger CR. A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants. A proposal. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1987;44:573–88. 17. Ball SA. Personality traits, problems, and disorders: Clinical applications to

substance use disorders. J Res Pers 2005;39:84–102.

18. Goudriaan AE, Oosterlaan J, de Beurs E, van den BW. Pathological gambling: a comprehensive review of biobehavioral findings. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2004;28:123–41.

19. Chen WJ, Chen HM, Chen CC, Chen CC, Yu WY, Cheng AT. Cloninger’s Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire: psychometric properties and construct validity in Taiwanese adults. Compr Psychiatry 2002;43:158–66.

20. Chen SH, Weng LC, Su YJ, Wu HM, Yang PF. Development of Chinese Internet Addiction Scale and its psychometric study. Chin J Psychol 2003;45:279–94. 21. Ko CH, Yen JY, Yen CF, Chen CC, Yen CN, Chen SH. Screening for Internet

addiction: an empirical research on cut-off points for the Chen Internet Addiction Scale. Kaohsiung J Med Sci 2005;21:545–51.

22. Yen CF, Yang YH, Ko CH, Yen JY. Substance initiation sequences among Taiwanese adolescents using methamphetamine. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2005;59:683–9.

23. Bauman A, Phongsavan P. Epidemiology of substance use in adolescence: prevalence, trends and policy implications. Drug Alcohol Depend 1999;55:187–207.

24. Miller PM, Plant M. Drinking, smoking, and illicit drug use among 15 and 16 year olds in the United Kingdom. Br Med J 1996;313:394–7.

25. Agrawal A, Madden PA, Heath AC, Lynskey MT, Bucholz KK, Martin NG. Correlates of regular cigarette smoking in a population-based sample of Australian twins. Addiction 2005;100:1709–19.

26. Easton A, Kiss E. Covariates of current cigarette smoking among secondary school students in Budapest, Hungary, 1999. Health Educ Res 2005;20:92–100. 27. Masse LC, Tremblay RE. Behavior of boys in kindergarten and the onset of

substance use during adolescence. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1997;54:62–8. 28. Lavin M, Marvin K, McLarney A, Nola V, Scott L. Sensation seeking and

collegiate vulnerability to Internet dependence. Cyberpsychol Behav 1999;2:425–30.

29. Bardo MT, Donohew RL, Harrington NG. Psychobiology of novelty seeking and drug seeking behavior. Behav Brain Res 1996;77:23–43.

30. Cloninger CR, Sigvardsson S, Przybeck TR, Svrakic DM. Personality antecedents of alcoholism in a national area probability sample. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1995;245:239–44.

31. Leyton M, Boileau I, Benkelfat C, Diksic M, Baker G, Dagher A.

Amphetamine-induced increases in extracellular dopamine, drug wanting, and novelty seeking: a PET/[11C] raclopride study in healthy men.

Neuropsychopharmacology 2002;27:1027–35.

32. Galen LW, Henderson MJ, Whitman RD. The utility of novelty seeking, harm avoidance, and expectancy in the prediction of drinking. Addict Behav 1997;22:93–106.

33. Suler J. The online disinhibition effect. Cyberpsychol Behav 2004;7:321–6. 34. Fein G, Di Sclafani V, Meyerhoff DJ. Prefrontal cortical volume reduction

associated with frontal cortex function deficit in 6-week abstinent crack-cocaine dependent men. Drug Alcohol Depend 2002;68:87–93.

Manuscript received March 2006, revised, and accepted August 2006.

1

Psychiatrist, Department of Psychiatry, Kaohsiung Medical University Chung-Ho Memorial Hospital, Kaohsiung Medical University, Kaohsiung, Taiwan.

2Postgraduate Student, Graduate Institute of Medicine, College of

Medicine, Kaohsiung Medical University, Kaohsiung, Taiwan.

3

Psychiatrist, Department of Psychiatry, Kaohsiung Municipal Hsiao-Kang Hospital, Kaohsiung Medical University, Kaohsiung, Taiwan.

4

Associate Professor, Department of Psychiatry, Kaohsiung Medical University Chung-Ho Memorial Hospital, Kaohsiung Medical University, Kaohsiung, Taiwan.

5

Associate Professor, Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, College of Medicine, Kaohsiung Medical University, Kaohsiung, Taiwan.

6

Associate Professor, Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan.

Address for correspondence: Dr CF Yen, Department of Psychiatry,

Kaohsiung Medical University, 100 Tzyou 1st Road, Kaohsiung City, Taiwan 807; chfaye@cc.kmu.edu.tw

(8)

Résumé : La personnalité tridimensionnelle des adolescents présentant une

dépendance à Internet et une expérience d’utilisation de substance

Objectif : Cette étude visait à examiner les différences des caractéristiques de la personnalité entre

des adolescents avec et sans dépendance à Internet et une expérience d’utilisation de substance, comme le définit le questionnaire de personnalité tridimensionnelle (TPQ), et à comparer les caractéristiques de la personnalité chez des groupes d’adolescents ayant la dépendance à Internet et une expérience d’utilisation de substance (groupe comorbide), ceux qui ont seulement la

dépendance à Internet (groupe de dépendance à Internet), ceux ayant seulement une expérience d’utilisation de substance (groupe d’expérience de substance) et ceux n’ayant ni dépendance à Internet ni expérience d’utilisation de substance (groupe témoin).

Méthode : Dans l’enquête transversale, nous avons recruté 3 662 élèves (2 328 garçons et

1 334 filles) d’écoles secondaires du sud de Taiwan. Nous avons mené notre enquête à l’aide du TPQ, de l’échelle de dépendance à Internet de Chen, et de questionnaires d’expérience d’utilisation de substance.

Résultats : Les adolescents ayant une dépendance à Internet étaient plus susceptibles d’avoir une

expérience d’utilisation de substance. La recherche de nouveauté (RN) élevée, la prudence craintive (PC) élevée et la dépendance à la récompense (DR) faible prédisaient une proportion plus élevée d’adolescents ayant une dépendance à Internet. Une RN élevée, une faible PC et une faible DR prédisaient une proportion plus élevée d’adolescents ayant une expérience d’utilisation de substance. Sur les 4 groupes, le groupe de dépendance à Internet avait les scores de PC les plus élevés et le groupe comorbide avait les scores de PC les plus faibles.

Conclusion : Il faudrait fournir aux adolescents ayant des RN élevés et des DR faibles des

stratégies pour prévenir la dépendance à Internet et l’utilisation de substance. Il faudrait aussi fournir au groupe de dépendance à Internet et au groupe comorbide différentes stratégies de prévention axées sur la PC.

數據

Table 3 Odds ratios of the TPQ (NS, HA, and RD) for Internet addiction and substance use experience, controlling for sex, school, and grades

參考文獻

相關文件

Application via internet: Foreign students, overseas Chinese students, and ethnic Chinese students shall apply for a work permit through the Internet in accordance with the law..

Write the following problem on the board: “What is the area of the largest rectangle that can be inscribed in a circle of radius 4?” Have one half of the class try to solve this

Teacher 2 and Classroom Assistant work with the whole class while Teacher 1 assesses individuals in a small group on a focus Reading Skill or Strategy.. Assessing

(a) In your group, discuss what impact the social issues in Learning Activity 1 (and any other socials issues you can think of) have on the world, Hong Kong and you.. Choose the

Then they work in groups of four to design a questionnaire on diets and eating habits based on the information they have collected from the internet and in Part A, and with

We explicitly saw the dimensional reason for the occurrence of the magnetic catalysis on the basis of the scaling argument. However, the precise form of gap depends

• Each student might start from a somewhat different point of view and experience the encounters with works of art and ideas in a different way... Postmodern

• The Tolerable Upper Intake level (UL) is the highest nutrient intake value that is likely to pose no risk of adverse health effects for individuals in a given age and gender