• 沒有找到結果。

Does using e-portfolios for reflectivewriting enhance high schoolstudents’ self-regulated learning?

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Does using e-portfolios for reflectivewriting enhance high schoolstudents’ self-regulated learning?"

Copied!
23
0
0

加載中.... (立即查看全文)

全文

(1)

http://www.researchgate.net/publication/276964942

Does using e-portfolios for reflective

writing enhance high school

students’ self-regulated learning?

ARTICLE in TECHNOLOGY PEDAGOGY AND EDUCATION · APRIL 2015 Impact Factor: 0.68 · DOI: 10.1080/1475939X.2015.1042907 READS

31

5 AUTHORS, INCLUDING: Chi-Cheng Chang National Taiwan Normal University 78 PUBLICATIONS 401 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Ju-Shih Tseng National Taiwan Normal University 11 PUBLICATIONS 20 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Available from: Chaoyun Liang Retrieved on: 04 October 2015

(2)

This article was downloaded by: [Chaoyun Liang] On: 19 August 2015, At: 07:17

Publisher: Routledge

Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: 5 Howick Place, London, SW1P 1WG

Click for updates

Technology, Pedagogy and Education

Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:

http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rtpe20

Does using e-portfolios for reflective

writing enhance high school students’

self-regulated learning?

Chi-Cheng Changa, Chaoyun Liangb, Kuen-Ming Shuc, Kuo-Hung Tsengd & Chun-Yu Lina

a

Department of Technology Application & Human Resource Development, National Taiwan Normal University, Taipei, Taiwan

b

Department of Bio-Industry Communication and Development, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan

c

Department of Mechanical and Computer-Aided Engineering, National Formosa University, Yunlin, Huwei, Taiwan

d

Graduate Institute of Business and Management, Meiho University, Neipu, Pingtung, Taiwan

Published online: 19 Aug 2015.

To cite this article: Chi-Cheng Chang, Chaoyun Liang, Kuen-Ming Shu, Kuo-Hung Tseng &

Chun-Yu Lin (2015): Does using e-portfolios for reflective writing enhance high school students’ self-regulated learning?, Technology, Pedagogy and Education

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1475939X.2015.1042907

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,

(3)

Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

(4)

Does using e-portfolios for re

flective writing enhance high school

students

’ self-regulated learning?

Chi-Cheng Changa*, Chaoyun Liangb, Kuen-Ming Shuc, Kuo-Hung Tsengdand Chun-Yu Lina

aDepartment of Technology Application & Human Resource Development, National Taiwan

Normal University, Taipei, Taiwan;bDepartment of Bio-Industry Communication and Development, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan;cDepartment of Mechanical and Computer-Aided Engineering, National Formosa University, Yunlin, Huwei, Taiwan;

dGraduate Institute of Business and Management, Meiho University, Neipu, Pingtung,

Taiwan

(Received 27 September 2013;final version received 29 June 2014)

The study aims to examine whether reflective writing using e-portfolios enhances high school students’ self-regulated learning. Participants included two classes of eighth-graders majoring in Information Processing and taking a course called ‘Website Design’ at a vocational high school in Taiwan. There were 41 students, with 18 males and 23 females. The experiment lasted 10 weeks, and students used e-portfolios to reflect on their learning. The results showed that students after using e-portfolios to reflect on their learning had significantly better self-regulated learning than before. This indicates that using e-portfolios for reflection enhanced self-regulated learning. It also shows that high-reflection students had significantly better self-regulated learning than moderate-reflection and low-reflection students, which implies that reflective performance had a significantly positive effect on self-regulated learning.

Keywords: portfolio; e-portfolio; reflection; self-regulated learning

1. Introduction

1.1. E-portfolios and reflection

Electronic portfolios (e-portfolios) have features of reflection, breadth, dynamics and sharing (Bartlett & Sherry, 2006) and can store the contents of students’ reflections (Barrett,2010). There are many kinds of content in e-portfolios, and reflection is one of the important activities (Abrami et al.,2008; Barrett & Garrett,2009; Chang, Liang, & Chen, 2013; Chau & Cheng,2010; Wade, Abrami, & White,2006). E-portfolios not only help students deeply understand the intention of learning but also cultivate students’ ability in active learning because students can reflect on and self-assess their selected projects and outcomes online during the development of portfolios (Linn & Miller, 2005). Some research also shows that using portfolios can cultivate students’ reflective ability (Barrett,2010; Carroll, Markauskaite, & Calvo,2007; Jenson,2011; Lopez-Fernandez & Rodriguez-Illera, 2009; Mansvelder-Longayroux, Beijaard, & Verloop,2007; Tubaishat, Lansari, & Al-Rawi,2009; Wang,2009). McCready (2007) *Corresponding author. Email:samchang@ntnu.edu.tw

© 2015 Association for Information Technology in Teacher Education

http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1475939X.2015.1042907

(5)

pointed out that reflection can change learning behaviour during the process of developing portfolios. In sum, reflection on the process of developing portfolios is not only an important learning behaviour but also a dynamic for improving learning behaviour.

1.2. E-portfolio and self-regulated learning

Some studies revealed that reflection can facilitate self-regulated learning, but these experiments were not done with e-portfolios (Anderson,2009; van den Boom, Paas, & van Merrienboer, 2007; van den Boom, Paas, van Merrienboer, & van Gog, 2004; Zimmerman, 2008a). For example, Anderson (2009) enhanced students’ self-regulated learning by reflective writing. Van den Boom et al. (2004, 2007) facilitated students’ self-regulated learning through reflection prompts and teacher feedback. Some studies demonstrate that e-portfolios can help students improve their learning behaviour and facilitate self-regulated learning by self-review (Abrami et al., 2008; Carneiro, Lefrere, Steffens, & Underwood, 2011; Chang & Tseng, 2011; Gama & Idan, 2007; McCready, 2007; Wade et al., 2006). However, these studies did not confirm whether the results were caused by reflection or other fac-tors. For instance, Jenson (2011) used e-portfolios to facilitate students’ critical reflection and enhance self-regulated learning; Gama and Idan (2007) pointed out that e-portfolios can facilitate self-regulated learning and can be helpful for students’ online reflection, teacher feedback, student feedback and storage of projects.

McCready (2007) stated that e-portfolios can be considered as a tool for reflection because reflection can stimulate learners to self-observe, self-judge, self-regulate and self-confirm repeatedly (Dysthe, Engelsen, & Lima,2007). Self-observation and self-judgement are the constructs of self-regulated learning, so e-portfolios can facilitate the enhancement of different constructs for self-regulated learning. Zimmerman (2008a) suggested that reflection is a process for self-regulated learning and can be one of the factors that enhance self-regulated learning. Conversely, self-regulated learning can accelerate learners’ reflection, which makes learners able to monitor the learning context (Gama & Idan,2007). As a result, reflection and self-regulated learning can affect each other reciprocally. However, do students have better self-regulated learning when they reflect on their learning through e-portfolios? Can the constructs of self-regulated learning be enhanced? These questions were examined in the present study.

1.3. Reflection and self-regulated learning

Leung and Kember (2003) found that performance on reflection can be categorised into different levels. Yen and Chen (2008) pointed out that performance on reflection can influence students’ academic performance and performance on meta-cognition and self-regulated learning. Performance on reflection can also affect learners’ think-ing process, learnthink-ing diagnosis and evaluation as well as change their self-regulated learning behaviour (Mansvelder-Longayroux et al., 2007). Strijbos, Meeus, and Libotton (2007) stated that performance on reflection can affect performance on portfolio and regulation of learning behaviour. Folkesson and Swalander (2007) pointed out that the depth of reflection can influence reading and writing abilities on computers. Chang and Tseng (2011) believed that e-portfolios can make learners continuously self-regulate their learning and achieve the best learning performance

(6)

by introspection, teacher feedback and peer feedback. However, they did not exam-ine whether learners’ self-regulated learning can be facilitated through reflections by e-portfolios. Mansvelder-Longayroux et al. (2007) argued that through reflective writing in portfolios, students can review their learning experiences and compare them with current learning experiences. This way enables students to generate deep thinking or critical behaviour. Deep thinking can facilitate students’ regulations of learning behaviour. In sum, performance on reflection has a certain effect on self-regulated learning. However, with e-portfolios, are there positive relationships between performance on reflection and self-regulated learning? This was a critical issue examined in the present study.

1.4. Research objectives and questions

Based on the aforementioned research background, the purpose of the present study was to examine whether using e-portfolios for reflection processes can enhance self-regulated learning. The research questions are the following:

(1) Do learners have significantly better self-regulated learning after using e-portfolios for reflection processes?

(2) Do learners obtaining high scores on reflections have significantly better self-regulated learning than learners obtaining low scores on reflections?

2. Method 2.1. Participants

Participants included two classes of eighth-graders majoring in Information Processing and taking a course called ‘Website Design’ at a vocational high school in Taiwan. There were 41 students, with 18 males and 23 females. The mean and standard deviation of the age of these students were 13.5 and 0.5. The experiment lasted 10 weeks, with students using e-portfolios to reflect on their learning. The course was related to design and creation, which required students to submit their computerised projects and to reflect on their learning processes, so it was appropriate for the implementation of an e-portfolio.

2.2. Research framework

The approach using pretest–posttest of experiment design was adopted in the present study. The effect of e-portfolios on self-regulated learning was examined in the present study with performance on reflections as the independent variable and self-regulated learning as the dependent variable. During the experimental process, the students did not accept any other interventions that could have an impact on their improvements in self-regulated learning. If a student simultaneously accepted the other intervention, he or she would be requested to quit the experiment.

The measurement of students’ self-regulated learning was administered before and after the experiment. The t-test was performed to examine students’ differences in self-regulated learning before and after reflections. Pearson’s correlation was used to identify the relationship between reflective performance and self-regulated learning. The simple regression was adopted to explore the effect of reflective

(7)

performance on self-regulated learning. Moreover, analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to examine students’ differences in self-regulated learning among dif-ferent performances on reflection. A preliminary analysis on student e-portfolio con-tents was also conducted to be supportive and supplementary to statistical results.

Self-regulated learning refers to learners’ performance on self-regulated learning including six constructs, which are self-efficacy, task value, learning anxiety, observation, judgement (e.g. peer model, teacher criteria and student self-set goals) and self-reaction (e.g. adaptive self-reaction and defensive self-reaction). Performance on reflection refers to scores that learners obtained from their reflections in e-portfolios. Learners’ performance on reflection was assessed by the course instructor. Scores for reflections were categorised into three groups including high, middle and low.

2.3. Experimental process

The duration of the experiment was 10 weeks, consisting of two 90-minute classes each week, and students were required to complete two projects. Three stages of self-regulated learning, including forethought, performance and reflection, proposed by Zimmerman (2002), were incorporated into the processes of designing and creat-ing the projects. E-portfolio assessments, includcreat-ing self-assessment, peer assessment and teacher assessment, and two reflective activities were performed after the com-pletion of each project. The researchers hoped to enhance students’ self-regulated learning behaviour by two self-regulated learning processes. The experimental procedures are shown in Table1.

For thefirst week, students were given an orientation about the course including the conception of portfolios, learning-goal setting and reflective writing. Besides, the teacher demonstrated how to use the reflective mechanism in the e-portfolio system. Before the end of the class, the pre-test for self-regulated learning was administered to the students.

From the second week to the fifth week, the teacher taught students knowledge related to the first project. During the stage of forethought in the second week, the teacher explained instructional goals and showed sample work. Students set learning goals through the e-portfolio system based on course contents, instructional goals and their own learning abilities. The teacher gave students a guideline for goal set-ting, explained key points for learning goals and guided students in how to write learning goals. By the stage of performance in the third and fourth weeks, students designed and created thefirst project according to self-set learning goals. The project was uploaded to the e-portfolio system after the completion. At the stage of re flec-tion in thefifth week, the teacher provided students a guideline for reflective writing, explained its key points and guided students in how to write about reflection. Students reflected on their achievement of learning goals, first project and learning progresses by using the reflective mechanism embedded in the e-portfolio system. Furthermore, students engaged in the observation of project, self-assessment, peer assessment and feedback online. The teacher also engaged in online assessment and feedback.

The learning tasks from the second week to the fifth week were repeated from the sixth week to the tenth week, except that the course contents and the projects were different. In the tenth week, students reflected on their project online, engaged in the observation of the project, self-assessment, peer assessment and feedback, and

(8)

T able 1. Experimental procedures. W eek Self-regulated learning process Course unit Instructional activity 1 Orientation for the course 1. T eacher introduced syllabus. 2. T eacher explained e-portfolios. 3. T eacher explained learning-goal setting. 4. T eacher explained re fl ection. 5. T eacher demonstrated how to use the e-portfolio system. 6. Students practised with the system. 7. Students fi lled in the questionnaire for self-regulated learning. 2 Forethought Introduction of image processing 1. T eacher explained instructional objectives. 2. T eacher demonstrated examples. 3. T eacher guided students how to set learning goals. 4. Students set learning goals online. 3 Performance Design of desktop wallpaper 1. Students designed and created the fi rst project. 4 Performance Design of main page 1. Students designed and created the fi rst project. 5R efl ection Assessment on projects 1. Students upload ed their project to the e-portfolio system. 2. T eacher guided students to re fl ect on their learning. 3. Students re fl ected on their learning online. 4. Students enga ged in observation of project online. 5. Students enga ged in self-assessment online. 6. Students enga ged in peer -assessment and feedback. 7. T eacher engaged in assessment online. 6 Forethought Introduction of Fron tPage 1. Students set learning goals online. 7 Performance Development of background and frame 1. Students designed and created the second project. 8 Performance Design of uploading, menu and banner in a virtual space 1. Students designed and created the second project. 9 Performance Creation of marks and web visual ef fects, and application of table 1. Students designed and created the second project. 10 Re fl ection Assessment on projects (Continued )

(9)

T able 1. (Continued ). W eek Self-regulated learning process Course unit Instructional activity 1. Students upload ed their project to the e-portfolio system. 2. T eacher guided students to re fl ect on their learning. 3. Students re fl ected on their learning online. 4. Students enga ged in observation of project online. 5. Students enga ged in self-assessment online. 6. Students enga ged in peer -assessment and feedback. 7. T eacher engaged in assessment online. 8. Students fi lled in the questionnaire for self-regulated learning.

(10)

took the post-test for self-regulated learning. The teacher also engaged in online assessment and feedback.

2.4. Instrument

2.4.1. E-portfolio system

The e-portfolio system and its reflective mechanism were designed and developed by the researcher. Students can login to the e-portfolio system for creating a personal e-portfolio including learning-goal setting, reflective writing and project uploading. Students can also participate in online observation, self-assessment and peer assess-ment. The system contains several functions. The main function is reflective mecha-nisms including (Figure 1): (a) reflective writing and editing which provide a text editor for students to write, add, delete and revise their reflections online conve-niently and to write reflections regarding different learning topics; (b) guidance for reflective writing which provides learners an outline for writing reflections, a search tool and reflective hints; (c) observation of reflection which allows students to view peers’ reflections online; (d) feedback which allows students and teachers to give feedback online; and (e) assessment on reflective performance which allows students to assess their own or peers’ reflective performance.

2.4.2. Scale of reflective performance

The sub-scale for reflection, with a total of five items, in the e-portfolio assessment scale developed by Chang and Chou (2011) was employed in the present study. Items are related to reflections on learning goals, projects, learning achievements,

Reminder , Basic Infor m ation, and S elf Intr oduction Updat ed Le ar ning Re co rd s Portfolio Ite m s Cou rse In formation e-Portfolio System

Figure 1. A page of a personal portfolio.

(11)

learning attitudes, observation of peer performance and peer feedback. The measurement is based on a 5-point Likert scale. Each point describes different levels of performance. The sub-scale was used and confirmed by Chang and Chou. The reliability coefficients of the scale in the present study were greater than 0.80, as measured by Cronbach’s α, suggesting that the items had a relatively high reliability.

2.4.3. Scale of self-regulated learning

The scale of self-regulated learning employed in the present study was developed by Wu (2005) based on self-regulated learning theory proposed by Bandura (1986), Schunk (2005) and Zimmerman (2002, 2008a, 2008b). The measurement is based on a 7-point Likert scale, with a total of 50 items. There are six constructs including self-efficacy (8 items), task value (5 items), learning anxiety (6 items), self-observation (6 items), self-judgement (12 items) and self-reaction (13 items). Among these constructs, a higher score of self-efficacy, task value and learning anxiety indicates a strong motivational belief. Self-judgement includes three sub-constructs, which are peer model (4 items), teacher criteria (4 items) and student self-set goals (4 items). Self-reaction includes two sub-constructs, which are adaptive self-reaction (8 items) and defensive self-reaction (5 items). A higher score of self-observation, self-judgement and self-reaction indicates better behaviour.

Intentions and examples for each construct:

(1) Learning motivation: Learners’ willingness and intention toward learning. (a) Self-efficacy: Learners’ belief about whether they can achieve the preset

goals. For example, I am confident of passing the course.

(b) Task value: Learners’ belief about the importance of a subject or task. For example, I think the course content is valuable.

(c) Learning anxiety: Learners’ belief about anxiety and pressure from their learning. For instance, I am worried about not doing well on my learn-ing.

(2) Self-observation: Learners’ records and monitoring on whether they achieve the preset goals. For example, I review after the course is over.

(3) Self-judgement: Learners’ belief about whether they achieve preset goals based on the work of peers, criteria set by teachers and goals set by them-selves.

(a) Peer model: Learners’ beliefs about whether they achieved preset goals based on the work of peers. For instance, I often compare my learning progress with peers.

(b) Teacher criteria: Learners’ beliefs about whether they achieved preset goals based on criteria set by the teachers. For instance, I will evaluate my learning progress to see if I achieve the standards set by the teacher. (c) Self-set goals: Learners’ beliefs about whether they achieved preset goals

based on goals set themselves. For example, I will evaluate my learning progress to see if I achieve the goals set by me.

(4) Self-reaction: Learners’ feelings toward their progress on goal achievement.

(12)

(a) Adaptive self-reaction: Learners’ positive feelings and acceptance toward their progress on goal achievement. For instance, I am satisfied by my current learning progress.

(b) Defensive self-reaction: Learners’ negative feelings and acceptance toward their progress on goal achievement. For example, I am frustrated by my current learning progress.

2.4.3.1. Item analysis. For the discrimination of the scale, a test of criterion of internal consistency was performed in the present study. An independent samples t-test was conducted to compare differences between high scores and low scores in each item. Three items did not show a significant t value, which indicated that they should be deleted since the discrimination indexes were unacceptable.

Pearson’s correlations were performed after the three items were deleted. The result showed that the relationships between the scores of each item and the overall scale were significant (r > 0.6), which showed a high association. This implied that the item internal consistency of the scale was high.

2.4.3.2. Factor analysis. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measures of Sampling Adequacy (KMO) for pre-test and post-test were greater than 0.7, meaning that it was appropriate to proceed to a factor analysis. Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity was sig-nificant, which implied that there were common factors among items. Principal components analysis with direct oblimin method of oblique rotation approach was conducted for the factor analysis in order to allow relationships to exist among factors (or constructs).

For thefirst factor analysis, the result of the pre-test showed that the factor loading for item 9 (thefirst item in the construct of task value) in the scale was smaller than 0.3, therefore should be deleted. After item 9 was deleted, the result of the pre-test and post-test for the second factor analysis revealed that factor loading for each item was greater than 0.5, therefore no item was deleted in the scale. The eigenvalue of each construct was greater than 1, hence six constructs andfive sub-constructs could be established. The total accumulated variances of all constructs in the pre-test and post-test were all close to 80%, implying that the validity of the scale was adequate. 2.4.3.3. Reliability. The reliability coefficients of the overall scale and the constructs for both pre-test and post-test were higher than 0.86, as measured by Cronbach’s α, suggesting that the items had a relatively high internal consistency. 3. Results

3.1. Effects of reflection on self-regulated learning

The t-test was employed to examine differences in self-regulated learning before and after reflection. As shown in Table 2, there was a significant difference in self-regulated learning, and the post-test was significantly better than the pre-test. There were significant differences in the constructs of self-efficacy, task value, self-observation and self-judgement, and the post-test was significantly better than the pre-test. Based on the effect size, self-observation had the highest effect size (η² = 0.227), followed by self-judgement, task value and self-efficacy, which showed that the system had the greatest effect on self-observation. On the other hand, learning anxiety and self-reaction did not have significant results.

(13)

Among the sub-constructs of self-judgement, there was a significant result for self-judgement of comparison with student self-set goals, and the post-test was sig-nificantly better than the pre-test. There was no significant result for self-judgement of comparison with the peer model and teacher criteria. Among the sub-constructs of self-reaction, there was a significant result for adaptive self-reaction, and the post-test was significantly better than the pre-test. However, there was no significant result for defensive self-reaction.

3.2. Effects of reflective performance on self-regulated learning

Pearson’s correlation was used to identify the relationships between reflective perfor-mance and self-regulated learning. The results revealed that there was a significantly positive relationship between reflective performance and overall self-regulated learn-ing (r = 0.418, p < 0.01). Specifically, for the constructs of self-regulated learning, there were significantly positive relationships between reflective performance and four constructs– learning anxiety (r = 0.274, p < 0.05), self-observation (r = 0.396, p < 0.01), self-judgement (r = 0.384, p < 0.01) and self-reaction (r = 0.263, p < 0.05). Moreover, there were significantly positive relationships between reflec-tive performance and two sub-constructs – self-judgement of comparison with the teacher criteria (r = 0.326, p < 0.05) and self-judgement of comparison with self-set goals (r = 0.425, p < 0.01). However, there was a significantly negative relationship between reflective performance and one sub-construct – defensive self-reaction (r =–0.443, p < 0.01).

Simple regression analysis was used to find if reflective performance significantly influences self-regulated learning. The result revealed that reflective performance significantly and positively influenced overall self-regulated learning (R2 = 0.175, p < 0.01). In other words, better reflective performance led to better overall self-regulated learning. Explained power of reflective performance on overall self-regulated learning was quite sufficient (R2= 17.5%) and significant (F = 9.357, p < 0.01), indicating that in this regression model, it was proper to explain overall self-regulated learning by reflective performance.

Table 2. The t-test for differences in self-regulated learning before and after reflection.

Construct Sub-construct Pre-test Post-test t Sig. Effect size Mean SD Mean SD Self-efficacy 35.05 6.75 38.85 8.05 –2.315 0.021* 0.146 Task value 23.40 5.82 26.37 6.06 –2.717 0.010* 0.178 Learning anxiety 17.00 7.15 18.82 7.53 –1.448 0.157 0.058 Self-observation 21.22 6.37 24.57 5.40 –3.162 0.003** 0.227 Self-judgement 48.14 12.35 52.80 11.81 –3.133 0.004** 0.224 Peer model 14.40 5.99 15.91 6.74 –1.925 0.063 0.098 Teacher criteria 16.62 4.74 17.80 3.80 –1.813 0.079 0.088 Self-set goals 17.11 3.58 19.08 .69 –3.174 0.003** 0.229 Self-reaction 45.62 6.58 47.54 9.15 –1.200 0.239 0.041 Adaptive 16.80 4.17 19.17 6.04 –2.549 0.015* 0.160 Defensive 28.82 4.43 28.37 5.40 0.477 0.636 0.007 Overall 174.91 24.65 192.51 28.03 –3.809 0.001** 0.299 * p < 0.05,**p < 0.01.

(14)

Reflective performance significantly influenced four constructs – learning anxiety (R2 = 0.075, p < 0.05), self-observation (R2 = 0.157, p < 0.01), self-judgement (R2 = 0.148, p < 0.01) and self-reaction (R2 = 0.263, p < 0.05) and three sub-constructs – self-judgement of comparison with the teacher criteria (R2 = 0.069, p < 0.05), self-judgement of comparison with self-set goals (R2 = 0.181, p < 0.01) and defensive self-reaction (R2 = 0.189, p < 0.01). Except for negative effect on defensive self-reaction, reflective performance positively influenced other constructs. Better reflective performance led to lower defensive self-reaction.

The ANOVA for effects of reflective performance on self-regulated learning is shown in Table 3. Reflective performance was equally divided into three Table 3. ANOVA for effects of reflective performance on self-regulated learning.

Constructs

Sub-constructs

Reflective

performance Mean SD F Sig.

Effect size Post-hoc test Self-efficacy H 41.63 6.03 M 38.78 9.24 0.233 0.794 0.014 L 38.20 9.54 Task value H 28.81 5.54 M 25.85 6.90 1.519 0.234 0.087 L 24.40 4.88 Learning anxiety H 23.45 6.33 H > M, L M 16.00 7.07 3.673 0.037* 0.187 L 17.70 7.58 Self-observation H 29.00 5.63 H > M > L M 22.57 4.43 7.831 0.002** 0.329 L 17.50 3.40 Self-judgement H 62.63 9.69 H > M, L M 47.78 11.53 7.426 0.002** 0.317 L 49.00 7.58 Peer model H 19.81 5.17 M 14.07 7.11 3.005 0.064 0.158 L 14.20 6.46 Teacher criteria H 20.54 3.01 H > M, L M 16.21 4.15 5.427 0.009** 0.253 L 17.00 2.40 Self-set goals H 22.27 2.64 H > M, L M 17.50 3.87 8.681 0.001*** 0.352 L 17.80 1.98 Self-reaction H 28.78 6.76 L > H, M M 30.09 6.22 3.658 0.037** 0.186 L 34.20 6.25 Adaptive H 21.00 5.58 L > H, M M 18.00 6.49 0.776 0.469 0.046 L 18.80 5.99 Defensive H 12.63 4.73 L > H, M M 10.78 6.10 9.452 0.001*** 0.371 L 15.40 6.48 Overall H 216.00 23.43 H > M, L M 178.71 23.81 8.339 0.001*** 0.343 L 186.00 22.86

Notes: H: High reflective performance (27 students); M: Middle reflective performance (28 students); L: Low reflective performance (27 students).

*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

(15)

groups – high reflective performance group (27 students), middle reflective group (28 students) and low reflective group (27 students). There were significant differences in the overall self-regulated learning among three groups of reflective performance. Scheffe’s post-hoc test was performed. The result showed that students with high reflective performance had significantly better self-regulated learning than students with middle and low reflective performance. Among the constructs of self-regulated learning, there were significant results for learning anxiety (F = 3.673, p < 0.05), observation (F = 7.831, p < 0.001), self-judgement (F = 7.426, p < 0.001) and self-reaction (F = 3.658, p < 0.001), revealing that reflective performance had significant effects on these four constructs. Moreover, among the sub-constructs of self-regulated learning, there were significant results for self-judgement of comparison with the teacher criteria (F = 5.427, p < 0.001), self-judgement of comparison with self-set goals (F = 8.681, p < 0.001) and defensive self-reaction (F = 9.452, p < 0.001).

Based on the effect size, self-observation had the highest effect size (η² = 0.329), followed by self-judgement, learning anxiety and self-reaction. On the other hand, there was no significant result for self-efficacy and task value. The result for the post-hoc test revealed that students with high reflective performance had signifi-cantly better self-observation, self-judgement and reduced learning anxiety than stu-dents with middle and low reflective performance, and students with middle reflective performance had significantly better self-observation than students with low reflective performance. Since students with low reflective performance had sig-nificantly better defensive self-reaction than students with middle and high reflective performance, students with low reflective performance had significantly better self-reaction than students with middle and high reflective performance.

4. Discussion

4.1. Amelioration in self-regulated learning after using e-portfolio for reflection The performance in the overall self-regulated learning after reflection was signifi-cantly better than the performance before reflection, revealing that using portfolios for reflection would be helpful for the enhancement of self-regulated learning. The result was consistent with the study results done by Chamber and Wickersham (2007), Gama and Idan (2007), Mansvelder-Longayroux et al. (2007), Masui and Corte (2005), Simpson and Courtney (2007), Strijbos et al. (2007), Wade et al. (2006), and Yen and Chen (2008). However, participants in these studies did not reflect on their learning by e-portfolios.

Students’ self-efficacy after reflection was significantly better than before, showing that using e-portfolios for reflection was beneficial for the enhancement of self-efficacy. The result was consistent with the study results found by Hadwin, Wozney, and Pontin (2005), Hsieh (2009) and Yost (2006). Some study results also show that reviewing the performance on learning-goal setting in the stage of forethought based on the self-regulated learning in the stage of reflection was helpful for the enhancement of self-efficacy (Gama & Idan, 2007; Hadwin et al., 2005; Iannotti et al., 2006; Wade et al., 2006). However, participants in these studies did not reflect using e-portfolios.

Students’ task value after reflection was significantly better than before, revealing that using e-portfolios for reflection was helpful for the enhancement of task value.

(16)

The result was consistent with the study results discovered by Cole, Bergin, and Whittaker (2008) and Kay, Li, and Fekete (2007). However, participants in these studies did not reflect using e-portfolios. Cole et al. (2008) stated that learning atti-tude is an important factor that affects self-efficacy and task value. Chang and Tseng (2011) pointed out that there is a positive relationship between reflection and learn-ing attitude. Therefore, a learner’s interest in a subject can be observed through reflection (Kay et al.,2007; Yost,2006).

Students’ learning anxiety after reflection was not significantly higher than before, which was consistent with the study result found by Silvia, Eichstaedt, and Phillips (2005). Struyven, Dochy, Janssens, Schelfhout, and Gielen (2006) stated that loadings accumulated from creating portfolios can lead to learning anxiety. In fact, the time required for students to spend on the development of portfolios and assessments was not too long, so there was not too much learning anxiety. More-over, reflection did not generate learning anxiety, which can be further examined in the future.

Students’ self-observation after reflection was significantly better than before, revealing that using e-portfolios for reflection was helpful for the enhancement of self-observation. The result was consistent with the study results, which indicated that reflection was a good way to cultivate self-observation (Berrill & Whalen, 2007; Chapman,2006; Forneris & Peden-McAlpine,2007; Kay et al.,2007; Linn & Miller,2005; Mansvelder-Longayroux et al.,2007; Wade, Abrami, Meyer, & White, 2008; Yost, 2006). However, participants in these studies did not reflect using e-portfolios.

Students’ self-judgement after reflection was significantly better than before, revealing that using e-portfolios for reflection was helpful for the enhancement of self-judgement. The result was consistent with the study results found by Beishuizen et al. (2006), Mansvelder-Longayroux et al. (2007), Smith and Tillema, (2007), Strijbos et al. (2007), van den Boom et al. (2007), Wade et al. (2008), and Yen and Chen (2008). However, participants in these studies did not reflect on their learning by e-portfolios. Among the sub-constructs of self-judgement, there was no signi fi-cant result for teacher criteria and peer model, but there was a significant result for student self-set goals, revealing that using e-portfolios to reflect on learning was helpful for viewing students’ own preset goals (Hadwin et al., 2005) and can facilitate self-judgement of student self-set goals. The reason for the insignificant results for teacher criteria and peer model might be that students reviewed more on their own behaviour during the process of reflections in the experiment. In other words, students participated in reflection and review mainly according to their self-set goals, which caused a small effect of using e-portfolios on self-judgement of teacher criteria and peer model. Therefore, the effect of using e-portfolios on self-judgement of teacher criteria and peer model can be enhanced if teacher-to-student and peer-to-peer activities, such as peer assessment and feedback, are emphasised in an instruction. Another reason might be that the reflective mechanisms, such as observation, feedback and assessment, did not produce the effect, which should be strengthened.

Students’ self-reaction after reflection was not significantly better than before. However, among the sub-constructs of self-reaction, there was a significant result for adaptive self-reaction, but there was an insignificant result for defensive self-reaction. This showed that using e-portfolios for reflection was helpful for the facilitation of adaptive self-reaction. Similar to the study result found by Wu (2005),

(17)

there is a negative correlation between adaptive reaction and defensive self-reaction. In other words, students with high adaptive self-reaction had less defensive self-reaction, whereas students with low adaptive self-reaction had more defensive self-reaction. Yost (2006) also stated that reflective behaviour can represent students’ willingness for continuous learning. Therefore, the study result was reasonable.

Wu (2005) pointed out that there is a positive relationship between self-efficacy and task value. The present study result showed that there was no significant out-come for self-reaction, but there was a significant outcome for adaptive self-reaction. This result might be due to the small effect sizes for self-efficacy and task value, which led to an insignificant result for self-reaction.

4.2. Effect of reflective performance on self-regulated learning

Some studies revealed that the quality of reflection can affect self-regulated learning (Simpson & Courtney,2007). Some studies also showed that the higher the level of reflection, the better the self-regulated learning (Aslan, Schmid, & Abrami, 2009; Mansvelder-Longayroux et al., 2007; van den Boom et al., 2007; Yen & Chen, 2008), which was similar to the present study result that reflective performance had a positive effect on overall self-regulated learning. Students with better reflective performance would have better self-regulated learning.

There was no significant difference in self-efficacy among students with different reflective performance, revealing that reflective performance did not have a significant effect on self-efficacy. Wu (2005) stated that there was a positive correla-tion between self-efficacy and reaction. In other words, when a student’s self-reaction tended to be adaptive, his or her self-efficacy would be better. On the other hand, when a student’s self-reaction tended to be defensive, his or her self-efficacy would be low. In the present study, students’ self-reaction was defensive, so the insignificant result for efficacy was reasonable. The insignificant result for self-efficacy might be caused by students’ lack of learning experiences, which led to low intrinsic motivation (Wu, 2005). Yost (2006) also believed that the reason why reflective performance did not affect self-efficacy was that reflective activities were not interesting to students, which led to low self-efficacy.

There was no significant difference in task value among students with different reflective performance, revealing that reflective performance did not have a signifi-cant effect on task value. A study done by Wu (2005) showed that there was a rela-tionship between task value and self-efficacy which was consistent with the present study result that there was no significant result for self-efficacy, so there was also no significant result for task value. Therefore, based on the relationships between the two constructs, the insignificant result for task value could be affected by the insignificant result for self-efficacy.

Some studies revealed that more learning experiences and enjoyment in a learn-ing process would enhance task value (Cole et al., 2008; Hsieh, 2009; Kay et al., 2007; Mansvelder-Longayroux et al., 2007; Schank, 2004). However, it was found from students’ reflective writing that they had both positive and negative learning experiences. Students with high reflective performance had better learning experi-ences than students with low and middle reflective performance, but there was no significant result for task value. This result was confirmed by a study by Schank (2004) that students with enough learning experiences did not have better task value because they might be affected by other factors which would affect their intrinsic

(18)

motivations and interests toward the course. Furthermore, low task value might be due to negative learning attitudes because learning attitudes could represent the importance of task value (Cole et al.,2008).

  There were significant differences in learning anxiety among students with different reflective performance, revealing that reflective performance had a signifi-cant effect on learning anxiety. Students with high reflective performance had sig-nificantly higher learning anxiety than students with middle and low reflective performance. This result also showed that students would have different levels of learning anxiety and fear based on reflective performance. In other words, students with better reflective performance would have higher learning anxiety (Silvia et al., 2005) and higher anxiety accumulated from creating a portfolio (Struyven et al., 2006).

There were significant differences in self-observation among students with differ-ent reflective performance, showing that reflective performance had a significant effect on self-observation. This result was consistent with the study results found by Chapman (2006) and Kay et al. (2007). However, participants in these studies did not reflect on their learning using e-portfolios. Students with high reflective perfor-mance had significantly better self-observation than students with middle and low reflective performance, and students with middle reflective performance had signifi-cantly better self-observation than students with low reflective performance. This result showed that students with better reflective performance not only had a better quality of reflection (Kay et al., 2007) but also had clear thoughts about their own learning problems and progress (Yen & Chen, 2008) and more expectations about their performance (Simpson & Courtney, 2007), implying that reflection was a good method for self-observation.

There were significant differences in self-judgement among students with differ-ent reflective performance, showing that reflective performance had a significant effect on self-judgement. This result was consistent with the study results found by Kay et al. (2007). However, participants in these studies did not reflect on their learning using e-portfolios. Students with high reflective performance had signifi-cantly better self-judgement than students with middle and low reflective perfor-mance. Van den Boom et al. (2007) stated that students could be easily affected by goals set by teachers and themselves, but rarely affected by goals set by peers. Among the sub-constructs of self-judgement, reflective performance had significant effects on teacher criteria and students’ self-set goals, implying that reflective perfor-mance would be helpful for the enhancement of self-judgement based on teacher criteria and student self-set goals. This result was consistent with the study result found by van den Boom et al. (2007). Students with high reflective performance had significantly better scores on self-judgement of teacher criteria and student self-set goals than students with middle and low reflective performance, showing that students’ self-judgement could be easily facilitated by high reflection, teacher criteria and student self-set goals. Students with high reflective performance could easily change their self-judgement by teacher feedback and self-review. However, reflective performance did not affect self-judgement of the peer model, which needed to be further examined.

There were significant differences in self-reaction among students with different reflective performance, showing that reflective performance had a significant effect on self-reaction. Students with high reflective performance had significantly better self-reaction than students with middle and low reflective performance. Among the

(19)

sub-constructs of self-reaction, there were significant differences in defensive self-reaction among students with different reflective performance, implying that reflective performance had a significant enhancement on defensive self-reaction. Stu-dents with high reflective performance had significantly better defensive self-reaction than students with middle and low reflective performance. This result revealed that self-reaction was defensive, and students with low reflective performance tended to be more defensive. Wu (2005) stated that students with low self-efficacy and low task value tended to have defensive self-reaction. Thus, students’ defensive self-reaction in the present study was probably caused by self-efficacy and task value. Students with low reflective performance had more defensive self-reaction than students with high reflective performance because they were afraid of learning or unwilling to learn. Moreover, high reflection could lower defensive self-reaction because students could engage in self-review and enhance learning experiences through reflection and then further affect self-reaction.

4.3. Preliminary analysis on student e-portfolio contents

Based on the contents of reflections, using portfolios for reflection is helpful to enhance the quality of reflection and convenient for students to handle their reflections, which facilitates self-regulated learning. Using e-portfolios for reflec-tion has the best effect on self-observareflec-tion, revealing that students get used to reflecting on their learning based on their learning progress and learning perfor-mance. However, according to the contents of reflections, self-efficacy and task value were the most important elements for the enhancement of self-observation; therefore, the enhancement of motivational beliefs (e.g. self-efficacy, task value and anxiety learning) was a factor that facilitated self-observation. If e-portfolios pervade in schools, students should be encouraged to reflect on their learning using e-portfolios. The reflective mechanisms in the system which are convenient for students can enhance students’ quality and effectiveness of reflections and positively affect self-regulated learning. This result of the study can be a reference for teachers who want to use the reflective mechanisms in the system as a tool for reflection in future instruction.

According to the contents of reflections, students with high reflection perceived that portfolio assessment would make them anxious. Grossman (2009) and Silvia et al. (2005) also had similar findings. Students with high reflection had high self-expectation and were concerned about their reflective performance. Zimmerman (2002) pointed out that as long as there is a test or homework in the process of learning, learning anxiety will exist. Students who had better performance in re flec-tion would generate more anxiety because they were afraid of bad performance or assessments, which teachers should pay attention to. Students who did not perform well in reflection would generate defensive self-reaction because they had fewer experiences of reflection. Most teachers believed that time and energy spent in reflective activities was an extra burden, plus there was pressure of achieving course goals, so they were unwilling to include reflective activities in the course (Aslan et al., 2009). Therefore, most students were unfamiliar with reflection. Without training on reflection, students with low reflection could easily generate defensive self-reaction, which was negative learning behaviour that the teacher should pay attention to.

(20)

5. Conclusion and implication 5.1. Implication for teaching practice

Unfortunately, reflective performance did not decrease learning anxiety. Thus, per-spectives about the effect of learning anxiety on self-regulated learning have still been inconsistent. However, according to Struyven et al. (2006), different methods of assessment will generate different levels of learning anxiety. Anxiety generated from the portfolio assessment was probably due to extra burdens accumulated from creating portfolios and assessments. The researcher believed that the generation of learning anxiety was a phenomenon of self-awareness for a learner. Accordingly, appropriately providing students with rewards and assistances for reflection was one of the strategies for decreasing anxiety. Therefore, it is suggested that reflective reward mechanisms can be added into the system by providing an incentive for good portfolios and reflections. Students should be appropriately given encouragement during the development of portfolios and reflection in order to decrease their anxiety about reflective learning and assessment.

Although reflection was an efficient learning method for students, students gener-ally did not have high willingness to reflect on their learning during reflective activi-ties in e-portfolios (Aslan et al.,2009; Chang & Tseng,2011; Gama & Idan,2007). According to Schank (2004), defensive self-reaction frequently results from stu-dents’ spurious willingness or behaviours. However, students are actually unwilling to react in their deep minds. Students with low reflective performance tend to have defensive self-reaction, and to muddle their reflections. Therefore, it is suggested to enhance students’ willingness, habit and adaptation toward reflection and use of reflective mechanisms. It is also suggested that teachers should look after students who do not participate in reflection in order to weaken their defensive self-reaction behaviour and enhance their adaptive self-reaction.

5.2. Limitation and future study

In the present study, the course was‘Website Design’, which was appropriate for the implementation of e-portfolio research and practice because the projects required for the course were all web page products which were suitable to be included in stu-dents’ e-portfolios. It is recommended that a future study might try different types of courses for multiple applications in e-portfolios. The same students were examined for their differences before and after using the reflective mechanisms in the system. According to the intervention of the quasi-experimental research, the explanatory power was not sufficient, which was one of the limitations of the present study. Therefore, for the future study, a control group (without using portfolios) can be added in the experiment for examining differences in self-regulated learning between students using e-portfolios to reflect on learning and students without using e-portfolios to reflect on learning, which will make the study results persuasive.

  

Notes on contributors

Dr Chi-Cheng Chang is a distinguished professor at the Department of Technological Application and Human Resource Development, National Taiwan Normal University. He gained his PhD in Workforce Education and Development from Pennsylvania State University, USA. His current research interests are focused on e-learning, e-assessment and educational technology.

(21)

Dr Chaoyun Liang is a professor at the Department of Bio-Industry Communication and Development, National Taiwan University. He gained his PhD in Instructional System from Indiana University, USA. His current research interests are focused on digital media design, information communication and imagination.

Dr Kuen-Ming Shu is a professor at the Department of Mechanical and Computer-Aided Engineering, National Formosa University. He gained his PhD in Material Science and Engi-neering from the National Chiao Tung University, Taiwan. His current research interests are focused on CAD/CAM, mechanical manufacture and engineering education.

Dr Kuo-Hung Tseng is a chair professor at the Graduate Institute of Business and Manage-ment, Meiho University, Taiwan. He gained his PhD in Educational Administration from the University of Northern Colorado, USA. His research interests are focused on WPBL, concept mapping learning and knowledge transfer in web systems.

Mr Chun-Yu Lin is a member of Steering Committee for Mandarin Test, National Taiwan Normal University. He gained his Master in Technological and Vocational Education from the National Taipei University of Technology. His current research interests are focused on e-learning, e-assessment and computer technology.

References

Abrami, P. C., Wade, C. A., Pillay, V., Aslan, O., Bures, E. M., & Bentley, C. (2008). Encouraging self-regulated learning through electronic portfolios. Canadian Journal of Learning and Technology, 34(3), 93–117.

Anderson, K. L. (2009). Motivating middle school learners: Reflective writing, self-efficacy, and self-regulated learning. Saarbrücken: VDM Verlag.

Aslan, O., Schmid, R. F., & Abrami, P. C. (2009, April). Using an electronic portfolio to develop SRL and writing skills in at risk students. Paper presented at the International Conference on Multimedia and Information and Communication Technologies in Education (m-ICTE), Lisbon, Portugal.

Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Barrett, H. C. (2010). Balancing the two faces of ePortfolios. Educação, Formação & Tecnologias, 3, 6–14.

Barrett, H. C., & Garrett, N. (2009). Online personal learning environments: Structuring electronic portfolios for lifelong and life wide learning. On the Horizon, 17, 142–152. Bartlett, A., & Sherry, A. (2006). Two views of electronic portfolios in teacher education:

Non-technology undergraduates and technology graduate students. International Journal of Instructional Media, 33, 245–253.

Beishuizen, J., van Boxel, P., Banyard, P., Twiner, A., Vermeij, H., & Underwood, J. (2006). The introduction of portfolios in higher education: A comparative study in the UK and the Netherlands. European Journal of Education, 41, 491–508.

Berrill, D. P., & Whalen, C. (2007). Where are the children? Personal integrity and reflective teaching portfolios. Teaching and Teacher Education, 23, 868–884.

Carneiro, B., Lefrere, P., Steffens, K., & Underwood, J. (2011). Self-regulated learning in technology enhanced learning environments: A European perspective. Rotterdam: Sense Publishers.

Carroll, N. L., Markauskaite, L., & Calvo, R. A. (2007). E-portfolios for developing transferable skills in a freshman engineering course. IEEE Transactions on Education, 50, 360–366.

Chamber, S. M., & Wickersham, L. E. (2007). The electronic portfolio journey: A year later. Education, 127, 351–360.

Chang, C. C., & Chou, P. N. (2011). Effects of reflection category and reflection quality on learning outcomes under a web-based portfolio assessment environment: A case study of high school students in computer application course. The Turkish Online Journal of Educational Technology, 10, 101–114.

(22)

Chang, C. C., Liang, C., & Chen, Y. H. (2013). Is learner self-assessment reliable and valid in a web-based portfolio environment for high school students? Computers & Education, 60(1), 325–334.

Chang, C. C. & Tseng, K. H. (2011). Using a web-based portfolio assessment system to elevate project-based learning performances. Interactive Learning Environments, 19, 211–230.

Chapman, R. S. (2006). Language learning in Down syndrome: The speech and language profile compared to adolescents with cognitive impairment of unknown origin. Down Syndrome Research and Practice, 10(2), 61–66.

Chau, J., & Cheng, G. (2010). Towards understanding the potential of e-portfolios for inde-pendent learning: A qualitative study. Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 26, 932–950.

Cole, J. S., Bergin, D. A., & Whittaker, T. A. (2008). Predicting student achievement for low stakes tests with effort and task value. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 33, 609–624.

Dysthe, O., Engelsen, K. S., & Lima, I. (2007). Variations in portfolio assessment in higher education: Discussion of quality issues based on a Norwegian survey across institutions and disciplines. Assessing Writing, 12, 129–148.

Folkesson, A. M., & Swalander, L. (2007). Self-regulated learning through writing on computers: Consequences for reading comprehension. Computers in Human Behavior, 23, 2488–2508.

Forneris, S. G., & Peden-McAlpine, C. (2007). Evaluation of a reflective learning intervention to improve critical thinking in novice nurses. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 57, 410–421.

Gama, C., & Idan, E. (2007, May). Electronic portfolios and self-regulated learning in the classroom. Paper presented at the 30th McGraw-Hill Ryerson National Teaching Learning and Technology Conference, Concordia University Montreal, Quebec.

Grossman, R. (2009). Structures for facilitating student reflection. College Teaching, 57, 15–22.

Hadwin, A. F., Wozney, L., & Pontin, O. (2005). Scaffolding the appropriation of self-regulatory activity: A socio-cultural analysis of changes in teacher-student discourse about a graduate research portfolio. Instructional Science, 33, 413–450.

Hsieh, P. H. (2009, January). The role of computer self-efficacy in managing information systems and technologies. Paper presented at the 2009 International Conference on e-Commerce, e-Administration, e-Society, and e-Education, Singapore.

Iannotti, R. J., Schneider, S., Nansel, T. R., Haynie, D. L., Plotnick, L. P., Clark, L. M., Sobel, D. O., & Simons-Morton, B. (2006). Self-efficacy, outcome expectations, and diabetes self-management in adolescents with type 1 diabetes. Journal of Development Behavior Pediators, 27, 98–105.

Jenson, J. D. (2011). Promoting self-regulation and critical reflection through writing students’ use of electronic portfolio. International Journal of ePortfolio, 1, 49–60. Kay, J., Li, L., & Fekete, A. (2007). Learner reflection in student self-assessment. In S. Mann

& S. Simon (Eds.), Proceedings of the ninth Australasian Conference on Computing Education (pp. 89–95). Darlinghurst: Australian Computer Society.

Leung, D. Y. P., & Kember, D. (2003). The relationship between approaches to learning and reflection upon practice. Educational Psychology, 23, 63–71.

Linn, R. L., & Miller, M. D. (2005). Measurement and assessment in teaching (7th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Lopez-Fernandez, O., & Rodriguez-Illera, J. (2009). Investigating university students’ adaptation to a digital learner course portfolio. Computers & Education, 52, 608–616. Mansvelder-Longayroux, D. D., Beijaard, D., & Verloop, N. (2007). The portfolio as a tool

for stimulating reflection by student teachers. Teaching and Teacher Education, 23, 47–62.

Masui, C., & Corte, E. D. (2005). Learning to reflect and to attribute constructively as basic components of self-regulated learning. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 351–372.

McCready, T. (2007). Portfolios and the assessment of competence in nursing: A literature review. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 44, 143–151.

(23)

Schank, R. C. (2004). Making minds less well educated than our own. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Schunk, D. H. (2005). Self-regulated learning: The educational legacy of Paul R. Pintrich. Educational Psychologist, 40, 85–94.

Silvia, P. J., Eichstaedt, J., & Phillips, A. G. (2005). Are rumination and reflection types of self-focused attention? Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 871–881.

Simpson, E., & Courtney, M. (2007). A framework guiding critical thinking through reflective journal documentation: A Middle Eastern experience. International Journal of Nursing Practice, 13, 203–208.

Smith, K., & Tillema, H. (2007). Portfolio appraisal: In search of criteria. Teaching and Teacher Education, 23, 442–456.

Strijbos, J., Meeus, W., & Libotton, A. (2007). Portfolio assignments in teacher education: A tool for self-regulating the learning process? International Journal for the Scholarship of Teaching and Learning, 1, 1–16.

Struyven, K., Dochy, F., Janssens, S., Schelfhout, W., & Gielen, S. (2006). The overall effects of end-of-course assessment on student performance: A comparison between multiple choice testing, peer assessment, case-based assessment and portfolio assessment. Studies in Educational Evaluation, 32, 202–222.

Tubaishat, A., Lansari, A., & Al-Rawi, A. (2009). E-portfolio assessment system for an outcome-based information technology curriculum. Journal of Information Technology Education: Innovations in Practice, 8, 1–15.

van den Boom, G., Pass, F., & van Merrienboer, J. J. G. (2007). Effects of elicited reflections combined with tutor or peer feedback on self-regulated learning and learning outcomes. Learning and Instruction, 17, 532–548.

van den Boom, G., Pass, F., van Merrienboer, J. J. G., & van Gog, T. (2004). Reflection prompts and tutor feedback in a web based learning environment: Effects on students’ self-regulated learning competence. Computers in Human Behavior, 20, 551–567. Wade, A., Abrami, P. C., & White, B. (2006). Using electronic portfolios to help students

become self-regulated learners. The Canadian Association of Principals Journal, 14(2), 23–25.

Wade, A., Abrami, P. C., Meyer, E., & White, B. (2008). ePEARL: Supporting learning using electronic portfolios. In F. Costa & M. Laranjeiro (Eds.), e-Portfolio in education. Practices and reflections (pp. 83–93). Mem Martins: Assoçiao de Professores de Sintra. Wang, S. (2009). E-Portfolios for integrated reflection. Issues in Informing Science and

Information Technology, 6, 449–460.

Wu, P. H. (2005). Exploring teachers’ instructional strategies based on self-regulated learning theory. Educators and Professional Development, 22, 57–65.

Yen, J. C., & Chen, M. P. (2008). Patterns of reflection for problem-solving in a mobile learning environment. International Journal of Education and Informations Technologies, 2, 13–17.

Yost, D. S. (2006). Reflection and self-efficacy: Enhancing the retention of qualified teachers from a teacher education perspective. Teacher Education Quarterly, 33, 59–76.

Zimmerman, B. J. (2002). Becoming a self-regulated learner: An overview. Theory into Practice, 41, 64–72.

Zimmerman, B. J. (2008a). Goal setting: A key proactive source of academic self-regulation. In D. H. Schunk & B. J. Zimmerman (Eds.), Motivation and self-regulated learning: Theory, research, and applications (pp. 267–295). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Zimmerman, B. J. (2008b). Investigating self-regulation and motivation: Historical back-ground, methodological developments, and future prospects. American Educational Research Journal, 45, 166–183.

數據

Figure 1. A page of a personal portfolio.
Table 2. The t-test for differences in self-regulated learning before and after re flection.

參考文獻

相關文件

• to develop a culture of learning to learn through self-evaluation and self-improvement, and to develop a research culture for improving the quality of learning and teaching

Information technology learning targets: A guideline for schools to organize teaching and learning activities to develop our students' capability in using IT. Hong

As each school has its unique school context, including its organisation of the JS and SS curriculum, experience in conducting PL, self-directed learning atmosphere,

 Teachers have to understand the salient features of the three pedagogical approaches of Direct Instruction, Enquiry Learning and Co-construction outlined below and

 After a school term ends, schools should evaluate the effectiveness of work plans and all aspects of their work over the past year (Evaluation) before setting up

(a) Classroom level focusing on students’ learning outcomes, in particular, information literacy (IL) and self-directed learning (SDL) as well as changes in teachers’

(b) Pedagogical and Assessment Practices (e.g. Transforming the Learning and Teaching Culture, Promotion of Self-directed Learning, Skills Development for e-Learning) ... Use

教育局 課程發展處 藝術教育組.. 你的生活