• 沒有找到結果。

In the subsequent sections, three phases of language learning in text-based CMC including, vocabulary, reading &amp

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "In the subsequent sections, three phases of language learning in text-based CMC including, vocabulary, reading &amp"

Copied!
26
0
0

加載中.... (立即查看全文)

全文

(1)

CHAPTER II

LITERATURE REVIEW

This chapter begins with a discussion over the effect of text-based CMC on the

development of language learning, which serves well as a pedagogical background for

investigating the effect of voice-based CMC on the enhancement of foreign language

oral proficiency, then followed by the studies in respect of the comparison between

the effect of text-based ACMC and SCMC in language learning. Subsequently,

learners’ general perception of CMC in comparison with face-to-face traditional class

is also introduced in detail.

2.1 Text-based Computer-Mediated Communication

Over the past decade, numerous studies have attempted to quantify the features

of how text-based CMC facilitates foreign language learning and enhances linguistic

competence (Blake, 2000; De la Fuente, 2002; Spitzer, 1989; Warschauer, 1996). In

the subsequent sections, three phases of language learning in text-based CMC

including, vocabulary, reading & writing, and speaking are discussed respectively.

The reasons why one of the receptive skill, listening is absent in the following part is

that little attention has been paid to voice-based CMC, leading to the absence of

listening ability from discussions.

2.1.1 Vocabulary in Text-based CMC

(2)

The Interaction Hypothesis proposed by Long and Robinson (1998) states that L2

learners’ meaning negotiation with other speakers crucially enhances second language

acquisition. With the emergence of the hypothesis, there has been many literatures

proving and ensuring the positive effect of text-based CMC on vocabulary learning

(Blake, 2000; De la Fuente, 2002; Ellis & He, 1999; He, 1998). Ellis and He (1999)

found that the ‘superior dialogic interaction’ in a ‘modified output group’ resulted in

the higher levels of receptive and productive acquisition of L2 words, accounting for

that negotiation focusing on lexical aspects of the language may be beneficial for L2

vocabulary acquisition.

By applying Interaction Hypothesis to vocabulary learning, Blake (2000)

subsequently concluded in his paper that incidental negotiation in text-based CMC

commonly occurred, especially dealing with learners’ lexical confusions. In Blake’s

study (2000), 50 intermediate Spanish learners were asked to have networked

discussion in dyads through a synchronous text-based chat program, Remote

Technical Assistance (RTA). The result indicated that up to 75% to 95% of all

negotiations were for lexical; in other words, the major part of negotiations was

triggered by lexical confusion. Therefore, the researcher claims that because CMC

raises L2 learners’ metalinguistic awareness of their L2 vocabulary development and

generates learners’ interaction on lexicons, L2 vocabulary can subsequently be

(3)

modified and improved in such a text-based learning environment. Subsequent to

Blake’s experiment (2000), De la Fuente (2003) even centered the sutdy on examining

the different potential effects of negotiation of meaning on L2 vocabulary

development between text-based computer-mediated interaction and face-to-face one.

20 elementary Spanish learners randomly assigned into Oral Interaction Group (OIG)

and Virtual Chat group (VCG) were asks to finish tasks with different information

gaps. Before the treatment, a pretest of vocabulary was administered to select 14

target Spanish words that learners had never learned before. The result showed that

face-to-face as well as CMC text-based interaction was equally effective in

developing written receptive and productive acquisition and retention of L2

vocabulary. However, in the promotion of oral acquisition of L2 words, and above all,

from the productive aspect, the latter group seemed to be less effective when

compared with face-to-face interaction. De la Fuente (2003) concluded that “cognitive

factors such as attention and depth of processing are the key to unveiling what

elements in the negotiation process facilitate L2 vocabulary development through

CMC, synchronous interactive tasks” (p. 74).

2.1.2 Reading and Writing in Text-based CMC

By the same token, in reference to the development of reading and writing ability,

text-based CMC such as email exchanges can positively enhance learners’ reading as

(4)

well as writing ability. As Robb (1996) stated, “electronic penpals or ‘keypals’ is a

highly motivating way to get valuable practice in both reading and writing” (p. 8).

Warschauer (1996) claimed that compared with face-to-face discussion, the language

in text-based CMC has been more lexically and syntactically complex due to the

written nature. He even further suggested that text-based CMC seem to be a good

vehicle for raising writing ability to greater levels of complexity and improving

learners’ overall writing (Cooper & Selfe, 1990; Mabrito, 1989; Spitzer, 1989;

Warschauer, 1996). Moreover, in Sullivan and Pratt’s (1996) study, they involved 38

intermediate ESL participants in the research examining learners’ attitudes, writing

apprehension, and writing quality in the two different writing environments,

computer-assisted class (text-based CMC) and oral one (traditional face-to-face class).

The quantitative analyses showed that the two writing environments might have no

strong effect on attitudes toward writing while the writing quality reaching the

significant level (0.08) was indeed improved in the computer-assisted classroom. The

study concluded that more than fifteen-week participation in networked computers

was more beneficial to the development of student writing than that in oral discussion.

In addition, Coniam and Wong’s (2004) focused their research on the correlation

between use of text-based CMC and the development of writing ability. 26 students in

one Hong Kong English-medium-instruction secondary school were involved in the

(5)

study and were divided into experimental group with the use of ICQ (15) and control

group (11). The experimental group was asked to accumulate a total of at least 20

hours of CMC with the attention to the target grammar, ‘only one verb with time and

number marking per main clause’. The result indicated that text-based CMC group

had promoted the use of English to exchange complex ideas, accounting for the higher

occurrence rate of complex sentences in learners’ use of English. Moreover, compared

with the control group, the writings of CMC group also showed the decrease of errors

in units with single verb, suggesting a better control of the target grammar. The effect

of text-based CMC on improving writing ability, consequently, is both theoretically

and empirically proved.

2.1.3 Speaking in Text-based CMC

Recently there has been growing interest in CMC because it can not only benefit

the writing but also the oral ability in second or foreign language learning. Text-based

CMC shares abundant of similar characteristics with spoken communication and

produces parallel advantages of oral discussions without temporal and spatial

constraints imposed by the classroom setting (Blake, 2000). Foto’s research (2004)

claimed that even ACMC in the form of email exchanges, despite the decrease of

non-verbal (i.e. facial expressions) and verbal cues (i.e. rising and falling of

intonation), could be as interactive as speech interactions. Furthermore, CMC is a

(6)

forum where demands of real time communication can be compensated for through a

set of affordances that the medium offers foreign language learners and teachers

(Meskill & Anthony, 2005). Many researchers and teachers (Abrams, 2003; Bölke,

2003; Chun, 1998; Kern, 1995; Salaberry, 2000) believe that text-based CMC can

effectively enhance learners’ spoken performance in the second language because

students tend to have greater equity in participation and produce more language with

more complex structure in text-based CMC than in face-to-face context.

Kern’s study (1995), for example, illustrated the effect of CMC on quantity and

variety of language oral production with the experiment comparing forty French

learners’ oral production during InterChange session (text-based CMC) and during an

face-to-face oral class discussion. The result indicated that overall students made from

two to three and a half times more turns in the InterChange sessions than the

traditional one. Then, consistent with the higher number of turns, students produced

two to four times more sentences with greater variety of discourse functions during

the CMC sessions than during the oral discussion. In the light of Kern’s claim,

Beauvois (1997) conducted a longitudinal study of text-based SCMC and L2 oral

proficiency with 83 participants, suggesting that learners in text-based CMC

outperformed those in non-CMC groups in oral exams in respect of pronunciation,

grammatical accuracy, lexical choice and accuracy, and content.

(7)

Not only had the above-mentioned linguistic but also pragmatic knowledge of

the target language made essential contribution to L2 oral proficiency. Chen (2005) in

her recent paper concerning about the use of text-based CMC to develop EFL

learners’ communicative competence (Hymes, 1972). With more and more attention

paid to communicative competence and approach, communication has been

recognized as an important learning objective in most current approaches to foreign

language teaching. The mastery of linguistic forms as well as the knowledge of social

factors plays an important role in communication. Chen concluded that the integration

of text-based CMC into EFL learning can offer learners more authentic input and

more opportunities to be engaged in the target sociocultural contexts (Light, 1989). In

effect, both linguistic and pragmatic knowledge can be enhanced for learners to

develop higher L2 oral proficiency.

In addition to comparing the effect of text-based CMC and face-to-face

discussion on oral proficiency, within text-based CMC itself, task types also greatly

influence the amount of learners’ negotiation, which has something to do with the

effect of text-based CMC on the development of oral proficiency. In Blake’s study

(2000), participants were asked to carry out three tasks types by means of SCMC,

including jigsaw, information-gap, and decision-making. The researcher reported that

jigsaw tasks accounted for 93% and 78% of the total negotiations for the two learning

(8)

sessions respectively. He then further concluded that jigsaw tasks promoted and

elicited more negotiations in accordance with what the previous study had predicted

(Pica, Kanagy & Falodum, 1993). Moreover, Smith (2003a) involved fourteen

nonnative-nonnative dyads to collaboratively complete 4 communicative tasks,

composed by 2 jigsaw and 2 decision-making tasks, in the form of SCMC. Partly

different from Blake’s conclusion, however, the result showed that compared with the

jigsaw tasks, learners negotiated a significantly higher percentage of turns in the

decision-making tasks. Statistically speaking, up to 78% of the target items were

negotiated during the decision-making tasks while only 22% were negotiated during

the jigsaw tasks. The researcher further concluded that task-type definitely influences

the extent to which learners engaged in negotiation, but the claim is not necessarily

true in the mode of face-to-face discussions. Furthermore, Peterson (2006) also

carried out a study aiming at shedding more light on the relationship between

task-type and level and numbers of negotiations in SCMC. Twenty-four intermediate

level EFL participants were invited to undertake a variety of language tasks in SCMC.

The data highlighted and confirmed the influence of task types on the quantity of

negotiations once again. There were more negotiation occurred in the

decision-making tasks than in the jigsaw as well as opinion-exchange tasks, which

was consistent with Smith’s (2003a) study. However, in line with what the previous

(9)

literature suggests (Smith, 2003a; Pica, 1993), in face-to-face contexts, jigsaw tasks

elicit a higher degree of negotiation than other task types.

Regardless of what task types was involved in those studies, it has been widely

and empirically recognized in literature that one blessing virtue of CMC, more

specifically, lies in the development of second language oral proficiency indirectly

mainly through the text-based CMC with written channel. Nevertheless, the ideal

parallel transfer from networked written ability into oral proficiency remains in doubt.

As the result of indirect transfer between written and oral, language abilities originally

belong to different paradigms in language learning; some unique features or even

genres have drawn many researchers’ attention. Therefore, the underlying

communicative features in text-based CMC accompanied with the influence of CMC

language on communication process and empirical comparison between CMC

language and spoken/written language will be discussed in detail.

The linguistic characteristics in text-based CMC were classified by Murray

(2000) into four major points: ‘similarity to spoken or written language’, ‘use of

simplified registers’, ‘structure of organization’ and ‘mechanisms for topic cohesion’

(p.400). In the field of text-based ACMC, email and electronic text-based bulletin

board are pervasively used nowadays. Based on Yate’s corpus of email (1996) and the

researcher’s personal e-messages, Crystal (2001) suggested that in spite of some

(10)

variations of terminology, individual emails consisted of a series of functional

elements, such as headers, greetings, and farewells, which were principally similar to

those found in traditional letters and memos (Flynn and Flynn, 1998; Gain, 1998; Li,

2000). Yet, the language style in email was uniquely different from both spoken and

written languages. Some emails were highly telegrammatic in style and analogies

between email and other forms of communication were drawn by writers to orientate

email in part of communication. Email was situated right between a conversation and

a letter (Hale& Scanlon, 1999; Naughton, 1999), and in the final analysis, was not

either spoken or written style. Crystal (2001) in his book even concludes that “The

consensus seems to be that it is, formally and functionally unique” (p.125). By the

same token, synchronous text-based electronic discourse is claimed to incorporate

features both from spoken and written languages (Crystal, 2001; Weininger & Shield,

2001) and to be a different, ‘medium-dependent register’ (Cherny, 1999; Weininger &

Shield, 2003, p. 330). Moreover, in M.J. Weininger & L. Shield’s (2003) research,

they further compared non-native-speaker (NNS) discourse with native-speaker (NS)

MOO-discourse (synchronous text-based CMC) and found that both native and

non-native speakers shared the same patterns in discourse and that even perceived

different formality of context and situation, NNS MOO-discourse still varied in a

similar way as native speakers did, which was in line with those results shown in

(11)

other literature (Cherny, 1999; Schwienhorst, 2004). The conformation of NS’ and

NNS’ discourse clearly proved that the uniqueness of linguistic characteristics in

text-based CMC was universal and cross-cultural. Concerning about ACMC as well as

SCMC, some studies indicate that text-based CMC should be recognized as spoken

genre because compared with face-to-face conversations, text-based CMC contains

greater complexity in lexical and syntax level of language (Coniam & Wong, 2004;

Warschauer, 1996). Notwithstanding being rejected in the spoken genre, interestingly,

research has been suggested that language in both text-based SCMC and ACMC tend

to be more ‘speech-like’ than ‘written’ (Ferrera et al., 1991; Yates, 1996).

Besides the language style of text-based CMC, one of the main differences

between speech and text-based CMC lies in the use of pronouns (Fowler and Gunther,

1979; Chafe and Danielewicz (1987). In Yates’ corpus-based study (1993), the result

showed that despite its similarity to writing in terms of overall frequency of pronoun

use, text-based CMC was quite different from writing in how pronouns of each type

(1st, 2nd, and 3rd person pronouns) are distributed. Then, from the aspect of relative

proportional usage of each type of pronoun, text-based CMC was much closer to

spoken language. Nevertheless, text-based CMC made greater proportional use of first

and second person pronouns (64%) than speech (58%) and writing (27%) did. In

addition to pronoun use, the study also suggested that the usage of modals in

(12)

text-based CMC was significantly higher than that of either speech or writing. On the

other hand, similar to the case with pronoun use, the overall relative frequencies of

modal usage across different semantic groups in text-based CMC were closest to

speech, which supported Ferrera’s study (1991) as well.

After the brief introduction to the linguistic features in text-based CMC, some

underlying communicative features appear to influence the communication process.

Text-based SCMC, for example, occurs without the significant delay of time but on

written channel; hence, participants do not have to take temporally sequential turns

since oral interruption is impossible in the written mode (Beauvois, 1992, 1998; Kelm,

1992; Kitade, 2000). Moreover, due to the lack of the aural (i.e. intonation) and visual

(i.e. a smile or a frown) paralinguistic cues in the face-to-face conversation, both

listener and speaker’s orientation to the discourse such as agreement or annoyance

cannot be precisely signified (Kieler et al., 1984). Finally, knowledge of social factors

that is crucial in communicative competence is hardly presented in the context of

text-based CMC without visual and aural paralinguistic cues for the distinguishability

of gender, ethnic background, and other social factors, especially when participants

use pseudo names (Hayne, Rice & Licker, 1994; Marjanovic, 1999; Selfe & Meyer,

1991; Sullivan, 1998).

Most literature consents to the positive effect of text-based CMC on

(13)

foreign/second language learning up to present although the difference between

text-based CMC and oral communication is surely existed, verifying that the practice

in the form of text-based CMC might not necessarily be properly transferred to the

development of oral proficiency which requires learners to directly ‘talk’ to with

interlocutors on spoken channel rather than to type the language. Based on the

previous studies, it appears that a number of studies have addressed the question of

how well the oral abilities can be indirectly improved in the text-based CMC context

by means of measuring syntactic complexity, lexical density, and diversity that

learners display. However, perhaps restricted by the limited bandwidth that barely

support the fluent online voice chat in the past, little research (Chang & Yang, 2006)

in the field of language learning to date have focused on this issue directly and

explicitly through voice-based SCMC as well as ACMC.

2.2 Synchronous vs. Asynchronous text-based CMC

Since the positive effect of CMC on language learning in comparison with the

face-to-face communication has been recognized according to the literature for the

decades, some researchers (Abrams, 2003; Sotillo, 2000) begin to turn their attention

to the profounder understanding on the combination as well as comparison between

the effect of ACMC and SCMC on language learning.

According to the numerous discussions either about the effect of ACMC or

(14)

SCMC on language learning, it could be reasonably inferred that the combination of

the two in language learning might be effective as well. Then, Harris & Wambeam’s

(1996) experimental study provide a solid evidence for the inference. The authors

tried to address the issue of how a combination of both text-based ACMC (e-mail) and

text-based SCMC (MOO) activities enhanced participants’ opportunities to improve

their writing skills. Aside from participants’ writing skills, learners’ attitudes toward

text-based CMC learning context were carefully observed through the three

Likert-styled questionnaires. The authors divided the participants who enrolled in the

university first-year writing classes into three groups; two experimental groups were

assigned to have synchronous (on Diversity University MOO) and asynchronous

(Internet journals) communication over the Internet. As for the control groups, 17

university students were involved and follow the same syllabi as the experimental

classes without engaging in the any CMC and participating in MOO sessions. In line

with other literature reporting the effect of text-based SCMC and ACMC on language

learning respectively, the result suggested that the experimental groups were more

effective than the traditional experience of the control group. The frequency of

participation was much higher for the experimental group (78%) than for the control

group (18%); that is, the combination of the two modes of text-based CMC also led to

more contributions toward the online journals and brought on learners’ enjoyment of

(15)

writing.

Concerning with the comparative effect between text-based SCMC and

text-based ACMC on language learning, Sotillo (2000) conducted an experiment with

the two modes of text-based CMC. In Sotillo’s exploratory study, two instructors and

25 students from advanced ESL writing classes were involved and completed four

synchronous discussions and posted 105 messages to an asynchronous threaded

discussion about reading assignments. Through the collected data of learners’ output,

the author quantitatively and qualitatively analyzed discourse functions and syntactic

complexity between the two modes. In terms of the discourse functions, the results

showed that more greetings, imperatives, requests for clarification and information,

and adversarial moves were included in SCMC data included, while primarily topic

initiation moves, questions, student responses to questions and comments about

postings were contained in ACMC data. Then, in respect of syntactic complexity,

t-units, error-free t-units, total number of words, subordinate clauses, total embedded

subordinate clauses and t-unit length were adopted as measurements. For synchronous

discussions, on one hand, Sotillo concluded that teacher’s domination of discussion

was significantly decreased in SCMC, and that the specific topics of interest

embedded in the multiple threads of SCMC mostly engaged learners’ attention. Also,

more attention was paid to fluency than to grammatical accuracy and the

(16)

conversations tended to become more informal. On the other hand, during the

asynchronous discussions, student output was lengthier and more syntactically

complex, which seemed to facilitate the structure as well as meaning of the target

language. The researcher concluded that both modes of text-based CMC could be

exploited for different pedagogical purposes, enhancing the language acquisition

process accordingly.

Subsequent to Sotillo’s exploratory research, Abrams (2003) also investigated

different effect of text-based SCMC and ACMC on improving oral performance in

German, adding the face-to-face communication as a control group. 96 students in a

Germany university were divided into three groups: the first group (control group)

had regular class face-to-face activities with no CMC treatment; the second group

engaged in text-based SCMC whereas the third participated in text-based ACMC.

Slightly different from Sotillo’s qualitative as well as quantitative study, Abrams

conducted a quantitative research, making use of various linguistic features such as

lexical richness, lexical density, syntactic complexity, and amount of language output,

which can reflect the increasing of communicative sophistication to evaluate

participants’ development of oral proficiency. The result showed that text-based

SCMC group outperformed those in the control group while text-based ACMC group

indeed produced significantly less output than their peers. The surprising outcome

(17)

indicated that participants in text-based ACMC were less motivated to participate in

the discussion perhaps due to the delayed nature of the interaction.

While the comparison between the effect of text-based SCMC and ACMC start

to unfold, the comparative effect of voiced-based SCMC and ACMC on language

acquisition, which may especially contribute to the development of oral proficiency

and possibly benefit pedagogical process , is still far from being unveiled.

2.3 Learners’ Perception of CMC

An early development in the history of student empowerment in language

learning was the claim of L.S. Vygotsky (1978) that social discourse between people

causes the growth of “inner voice” that offers people sense of mastery and that plays

an extremely important role in language learning. Besides, it is also generally believed

that second language is best learned through active negotiation of meaning (Ellis,

1996). Therefore, collaborative learning has been constantly associated with a process

of empowerment by educators and psychologists (Belmont, 1989; Bruner, 1987;

Cooper and Selfe, 1990; Kollock & Smith, 1996). Many researchers agree that along

with the mechanics in computer-mediated communication, learners will have much

better opportunities for initiating and controlling their own language learning

(Harasim, 1986; McComb, 1993; Santoro and Kuehn, 1988). The computer learning

network facilitates language learners to collaborate with each other in dealing with

(18)

different learning projects in single classroom or in various setting around the world.

Speaking of the blessing part of CMC, accordingly, many recent studies have

suggested the following virtues for CMC: (a) increasing amount and equality of

participation (Chapelle, 1994); (b) increasing the quantity of learner output (Kitade,

2000); (c) increasing quality of learner output (Beauvois 1998; Kitade, 2000).

Warschauer’s (1996) simply classified advantages that CMC brings in as “student

autonomy, equality, and learning skills” (pp. 3). These blessing strengths are closely

related to learners’ positive perception of the new language learning environment,

CMC.

In terms of student autonomy, abundant studies have suggested that participants

in CMC feel more involved in the development of ideas, in determining the way to

discuss topics, and in selection of the topics they are interested in (Baynton, 1987;

Garrison and Kelm, 1992; Kern, 1995; Lawrence, 2002; Ortega, 1997). In other words,

CMC has been positively recognized as having the ability to improve foreign

language learning, presumably by creating a more harmonious and collaborative

learning context. Lawrence (2002) observed a Canadian secondary French core

classroom where e-mail exchange with the other two French-speaking partnered

schools through student-designed online newsletter was used on second language

learning. Lawrence concluded that because students were allowed to post topics in a

(19)

great variety and had to cooperate with multi e-mail partners, they showed greater

interest in networked based discussion. Apart from Lawrence’s study, Yeung (2002)

collected learners’ opinion on asynchronous use of CMC, indicating that most learners

tended to believe that CMC components offer flexible learning context and claimed

that the flexibility indeed improved the level and quality of their learning. Learners

who engaged in asynchronous CMC such as e-mail or electronic bulletin board,

without the traditional limitations in class time or office hours, consequently, can play

the role as initiators to open up the discussions in anytime and any place with their

teachers or with their peers, which resulted in greatly increased both

student-to-teacher and student-to-student interaction (Harasim, 1986; Hartman et al.,

1991; McComb, 1993) and leaded to further opportunities for learners’ self

expressions (Kinkead, 1987).

Moreover, learner-to-learner exchanges in CMC appear to be more interactive

(Blake, 2000; Darhower, 2000; Kern, 1995; Warschauer, 1996) than learner-to-teacher

exchanges. Kern (1995) also indicated that direct student-to-student interaction could

greatly stimulate students’ interests in discussion and decrease students’ reliance on

teachers. He further showed that the most important of all, learner-to-learner

interaction can reduce communication anxiety because of the similar language

proficiency and nearly equal social status. In Kern’s open-ended questionnaires, quite

(20)

a few participants reported that they feel freer in the informal setting with peers as

interlocutors. In comparison with learner-to-teacher exchanges, Chun (1994) found

that second language students were in favor of the direct interaction with peers instead

of the teacher or the moderator in the networked text-based forum. In addition, the

results suggested that learners appear to display more interactive competence in the

context of CMC than in traditional face-to-face discussions. Although

learner-to-learner exchange is considered promising and brings about greater

interaction between learners, some classroom practitioners and researchers worry that

learner-to-learner discussion, especially participants belonging to the same level of

language proficiency, will reinforce non-target-like language, or “the blind lead the

blind” (Blake, 2000, p133). That is, learners may encounter the “blind” danger,

suffering from the poor quality of language without the grammatical correction from

teachers who are supposed to have superior knowledge of the target language (Kern,

1995). However, Porter (1986) pointed out that error incorporation in the data of his

study was quite rare. Furthermore, although Blake (2000) admitted that the ‘blind’

danger warrant close attention, he also reported that in the examination of all 50

participants’ transcripts on text-based CMC, no ill-formed language was acquired by

learners in their incidental negotiations. Even in Swain’s experiment (1998), she

stated that less than ten percent of the total negotiations recorded in his study

(21)

contained the transmission of incorrect solutions.

In subsequent years numerous studies were carried out on the effect of

synchronous CMC, reported that using synchronous conferencing leads to a shift in

authority from instructors to learners, which resembles the advantages of ACMC in

development of learners’ autonomy. In Pratt and Sullivan’s (1994) comparative study

between traditional oral discussion and electronic one done in EFL classroom at the

University of Puerto Rico, they discovered that approximately 85% of the

conversational turns were taken by learners in the CMC group, while learners only

took in charge of 35% of conversational turns in the traditional oral discussions.

Similar reports were made by Keln’s (1992) that up to 92% of comments in computer

conferencing in a Portuguese class were made by students themselves. Following

Keln’s research, Chun (1994) conducted a large-scale experiment on 14 computer

conferencing sessions in university German classes. The analysis showed that

approximately 88% of students’ comments and questions were directed to their peers

and only the remaining 12% were directed to the instructor, manifesting the fact the

student autonomy increases the interaction between peers in synchronous CMC as

well as in asynchronous CMC. Different from the traditional language learning

settings, the expansion of students’ autonomy in CMC context provides learners with

more independent opportunity to unfold the discussion on topics corresponding to

(22)

their interest. Furthermore, with the network communication, learners have the access

to carry out long distance communication, using the foreign or second language for

authentic communication with other language learners or native speakers throughout

the world, resulting the greater student enthusiasm, initiative, and personal

commitment (Paramaskas, 1993).

Besides students’ autonomy, several studies (Finholt, Kiesler and Sproull, 1986;

Selfe and Meyer, 1991; Sproull & Kiesler, 1991) have found that because learners

favored the atmosphere created in CMC, where everyone is treated fairly, more

balanced and equal participation of language learning among students in the context

of CMC. The computer as a new medium for conversation reduces the bias from

“static social contexts” (Warschauer, 1996, p5) such as gender, race, handicap and

different social status that might negatively influence the equal participation in

interaction during face-to-face conversation in the traditional classroom setting.

Flores’s (1990) and Selfe’s (1990) studies, for example, suggested that in the context

of CMC women’s participation in classroom discussion be equalized compared that of

men who generally are supposed to speak and interrupt more (Spender, 1980).

Moreover, Korenman & Wyatt’s (1996) questionnaire study also showed that CMC

occurred across social and hierarchical institutional boundaries; tenured and

untenured faculty, students, administrators all made great contributions to the

(23)

discussion forum because of the unique and nearly anxiety-free environment.

In regard to “dynamic social context” (Warschauer, 1996, p5) in the traditional

classroom, facial expressions like frowning and inactive attitude toward the

conversation such as hesitation may hinder introverted learners from expressing

themselves freely when they sense that their comments are being evaluated (Finholt,

Kiesler and Sproull, 1986). Contrarily, CMC allows extroverted as well as introverted

learners enough time and space to make personal contributions to the discussion in

language courses, waving the enormous pressure in the traditional classroom where

students who answer questions quickly and correctly tend to be favored (Tella, 1992).

Consequently, CMC is believed to have the ability of neutralizing the effect from the

dynamic social context and classroom (Warschauer, 1996). Several studies have

reached the agreement on the effect of CMC on developing learners’ equality in

language learning. In Pratt and Sullivan’s (1994) study, overwhelmingly 100% of

students participated in the CMC groups whereas only half of students got involved in

face-to-face discussion groups. Kern (1993) likewise found that compared with all

students participated in the CMC groups, five students dominated the face-to-face

discussion and the other four students did nothing during the period of discussion.

Moreover, Kroonenberg (1995) also pointed out that shy students were likely to refuse

speak up in the traditional class but tended to have ‘fingers flying across the

(24)

keyboard’, becoming more willing to participate in the subsequent oral discussions on

the same topics.

So far, learners’ general perceptions of CMC, either in ACMC or SCMC and the

comparison between learners’ perceptions of face-to-face traditional class and those of

CMC have been widely examined. Nevertheless, few studies focus on clarifying

learners’ possibly different perceptions of voice-bvased ACMC and SCMC. That is,

according to the literature, in the comparison between the two modes of CMC, the

development of learners’ language proficiency in stead of learners’ attitudes toward

SCMC and ACMC tends to be one and the only main target to be explored. Therefore,

the present study not only examines the effect of SCMC and ACMC on the

enhancement of learners’ linguistic competence but also investigates learners’

perception of the two modes of CMC.

2.4 Research Questions

On the basis of the literature just reviewed, the effect of CMC on enhancing

language learning, especially in respect of oral proficiency, has been positively

recognized when compared with the face-to-face discussion. In addition, some studies

further indicate that in terms of oral proficiency, learners in synchronous text-based

CMC seemingly tend to outperform those in asynchronous text-based CMC (Abrams,

2003). Consequently, it is hypothesized in the present study that in line with

(25)

text-based CMC, even in the voice-based CMC, learners in the synchronous context

would outperform those in the asynchronous context. Moreover, in terms of learners’

affective domain, while there is an assumption that learners express positive attitudes

towards the use of text-based CMC (D., Coniam & R., Wong, 2004; Hsu, 2005), little

empirical evidence has been found to focus on users’ attitudes toward voice-based

CMC. Therefore, the effect and learners’ attitude about voice-based CMC will be

taken into consideration within the present study. The following are the three research

questions.

1. Is EFL learners’ general oral proficiency significantly enhanced after the treatment

of voice-based CMC?

a. Is EFL learners’ oral proficiency in terms of pronunciation and fluency

significantly improved after the treatment of voice-based CMC?

b. Is EFL learners’ oral proficiency in terms of the accuracy of grammar and

content significantly improved after the treatment of voice-based CMC?

2. Is there a significant difference of the effects that voice-based SCMC and ACMC

had on EFL learners’ general oral proficiency?

a. In terms of pronunciation and fluency, is there a significant difference

between voice-based ACMC group and SCMC group?

b. In terms of the accuracy of grammar and content, is there a significant

(26)

difference between voice-based ACMC group and SCMC group?

3. What is EFL learners’ perception of voice-based CMC?

參考文獻

相關文件

help students develop the reading skills and strategies necessary for understanding and analysing language use in English texts (e.g. text structures and

• e‐Learning Series: Effective Use of Multimodal Materials in Language Arts to Enhance the Learning and Teaching of English at the Junior Secondary Level. Language across

• To explore the roles of English Language curriculum leaders in planning the school-based curriculum in primary schools under Learning to Learn 2.0.. • To introduce

Enhance English Language Learning, Teaching and Assessment in Reading at Primary Level. • Designing Quality English Language Papers to Enhance Learning, Teaching

• Assessment Literacy Series: Effective Use of the Learning Progression Framework to Enhance English Language Learning, Teaching and Assessment in Writing at Primary Level. •

• Assessment Literacy Series - Effective Use of the Learning Progression Framework to Enhance English Language Learning, Teaching and Assessment in Writing at Primary Level.

(2013) The ‘Art’ of Teaching Creative Story Writing In (Eds., Janice Bland and Christiane Lütge) Children’s Literature in Second Language Education. (2004) Language and

• helps teachers collect learning evidence to provide timely feedback & refine teaching strategies.. AaL • engages students in reflecting on & monitoring their progress