• 沒有找到結果。

依據研究結果的發現,分為未來研究建議與實際應用兩部分進行陳述:

一、未來研究方面的建議

(一)結合觀察與身體練習的方式

研究結果皆顯示觀察與身體練習皆能促進動作學習,若兩者同時使用在動作學 習的過程中,是否會有更加成的效益存在?若結合式練習可更助於動作表現與學習,

那麼,順序的安排又是另一個待解的問題。

(二)觀察重點的提示

本研究因強調錯誤估計工作,因此觀察者會聚焦於動作的誤差角度上,因此,

未來研究可移除錯誤估計工作的指令,探討觀察者是否能自行在觀察期間獲得有 效訊息以助於自我動作的發展?

(三)回饋頻率的安排

本研究採用 100%KR 安排,但過去關於 KR 頻率的研究大多指出在動作習得 的過程中,較低 KR 頻率較利於動作表現與學習,因此未來研究中可安排較低頻率 KR 或是漸退的 KR 頻率以觀察 KR 頻率是否也會影響此空間性移動工作的表現與 學習。

(四)示範者的動作精熟度

過去文獻指出示範者的動作越精熟將更利於觀察者的學習,因此未來研究可 將動作困難度提升,以比較精熟與不精熟示範者對於觀察者動作學習的影響。

二、實際應用方面的建議

從本研究得知觀察的學習方式有助於動作學習,即便沒有實際參與行為的操作也可 獲得相同的學習效果,在教學場域中,若遇上新動作的教學,可先讓學習者使用觀察的 方式學習,除了降低嘗試新動作的恐懼之外,也可全面性的獲取新動作相關訊息,有助 於之後執行動作的表現與學習,此外,學習者從事觀察較不會有受傷的風險,在運動訓 練場域中可節省運動員過度的能量消耗,透過觀察發展的錯誤偵察能力不僅能觀察自我 動作也可偵測出他人的錯誤,這兩種動作錯誤的訊息都可以提醒學習者避免犯錯或是更 加精進自我動作表現。

參考文獻

中文部分:

陳玉芬、卓俊伶 (1997)。感覺回饋訊息對簡單動作表現與學習的影響。體育學報,23,

323-334。

張櫻玉、卓俊伶 (2003)。錯誤估計與結果獲知對空間移動性動作表現與學習的影響。臺 灣運動心理學報,2,77-92。

英文部分:

Adams, J. A. (1971). A closed-loop theory of motor learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 3, 111–150.

Adams, J. A. (1986). Use of the model’s knowledge of results to increase the observer’s performance. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 12, 89–98.

Adams, J. A. (1990). The changing face of motor learning. Human Movement Science, 9, 209–220.

Badets, A., & Blandin, Y. (2004). The role of knowledge of results frequency in learning through observation. Journal of Motor Behavior, 36, 62–70.

Badets, A., Blandin, Y., Wright, D. L., & Shea, C. H. (2006). Error detection processes during observational learning. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 77, 177–184.

Badets, A., Blandin, Y., & Shea, C. H. (2006). Intention in motor learning through observation.

The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 377–386.

Badets, A., & Blandin, Y. (2010). Feedback schedules for motor-skill learning: The similarities and differences between physical and observational practice. Journal of Motor Behavior, 42, 257–268.

Bandura, A. (1969). Principles of behavior modification. New York, YK: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory.

Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Beek, P. J., & Lewbel, A. (1995). The science of juggling. Scientific American, 273, 92–97.

Black, C. B., & Wright, D. L. (2000). Can observational practice facilitate error recognition and movement production? Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 71, 331–339.

Black, C. B., Wright, D. L., Magnuson, C. E., & Brueckner, S. (2005). Learning to detect error in movement timing using physical and observational practice. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 76, 28–41.

Blandin, Y., & Proteau, L. (2000). On the cognitive basis of observational learning:

Development of mechanisms for the detection and correction of errors. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53, 846–867.

Blandin, Y., Proteau, L., & Alain, C. (1994). On the cognitive processes underlying contextual interference and observational learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 26, 18–26.

Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G. R., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Seitz, R. J., Zilles, K., Rizzolatti, G., & Freund, H. J. (2001). Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: An fMRI study. European Journal Neuroscience, 13, 400–404.

Buchanan, J. J., Ryu, Y. U., Zihlman, K., & Wright, D. L. (2008). Observational practice of relative but not absolute motion features in a single-limb multi-joint coordination task.

Experimental Brain Research, 191, 157–169.

Carter, C. S., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Botvinick, M. M., Noll, D., & Cohen, J. D. (1998).

Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance.

Science, 280, 747–749.

Chew, R. A. (1976). Verbal, visual, and kinesthetic error feedback in the learning of a simple motor task. Research Quarterly, 47, 254–259.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Deakin, J. M., & Proteau, L. (2000). The role of scheduling in learning through observation.

Journal of Motor Behavior, 32, 268–176.

Darling, W. G., & Cooke, J. D. (1987). Changes in the variability of movement trajectories with practice. Journal of Motor Behavior, 19, 291–309.

Fleishman, E. A., & Hempel, W. E., Jr. (1955). The relation between abilities and improvement with practice in a visual discrimination reaction task. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 301–312.

Franks, I. M., Wilberg, R. B., & Fishburne, G. J. (1982). Consistency and error in motor performance. Human Movement Science, 1, 109–123.

Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading.

Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 493–501.

Gehring, W. J., Goss, B., Coles, M. G. H., Meyer, D. E., & Donchin, E. (1993). A neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychological Science, 93, 385–390.

Gr`ezes, J., & Decety, J. (2001). Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation,

observation, and verb generation of actions: A meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 12, 1–19.

Guadagnoli, M. A., & Kohl, R. M. (2001). Knowledge of results for motor learning:

Relationship between error estimation and knowledge of results frequency. Journal of Motor Behavior, 33, 217–224.

Halsband, U., & Lange, R. K. (2006). Motor learning in man: A review of functional and clinical studies. Journal of Physiology, 99, 414–424.

Kimble, G. A. (1967). Foundations of conditioning and learning. New York: Meredith.

Liu, J., & Wrisberg, C. A. (1997). The effect of knowledge of results delay and the subjective estimation of movement form on the acquisition and retention of a motor skill. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 68, 145–151.

Magill, R. A. (2011). Motor learning and control: Concepts and applications (9th ed.). New York: McGraw–Hill.

McCullagh, P., Weiss, M. R., & Ross, D. (1989). Modeling considerations in motor skill acquisition and performance: An integrated approach. Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews, 17, 475–514.

Miltner, W. H. R., Braun, C. H., & Coles, M. G. H. (1997). Event-related brain potentials following incorrect feedback in a time-estimation task: Evidence for a “generic” neural system for error detection. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 788–798.

Mowbraya, G. H., & Rhoadesa, M. V. (1959). On the reduction of choice reaction times with practice. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11, 16–23.

Newell, K. M. (1974). Knowledge of results and motor learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 6, 235–244.

Oldfiled, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory.

Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–112.

Posner, M. I., & Konick, A. F. (1966). Short-term retention of visual and kinesthetic information. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 1, 71–86.

Pressing, J. (1998). Referential behavior theory: A framework for multiple perspectives on motor control. In J. P. Piek (Ed.), Motor behavior and human skill: A multidisciplinary approach (pp. 357–384). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 82, 225–260.

Schmidt, R. A. (1980). Past and future issues in motor programming. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 51, 122–140.

Schmidt, R. A., & White, J. L. (1972). Evidence for an error detection mechanism in motor skills: A test of Adams' closed-loop theory. Journal of Motor Behavior, 4, 143–153.

Schmidt, R. A., & Wrisberg, C. A. (2008). Motor learning and performance: A situation-based learning approach (4th ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Schmidt, R. A., & Lee, T. D. (2011). Motor control and learning: A behavioral emphasis (5th ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Shea, C. H., & Wulf, G. (1999). Enhancing motor learning through external-focus instructions and feedback. Human Movement Science, 18, 553–571.

Shea, C. H., Wright, D. L., Wulf, G., & Whitacre, C. (2000). Physical and observational practice afford unique learning opportunities. Journal of Motor Behavior, 32, 27–36.

Shea, C. H., Wulf, G., Park, J., & Gaunt, B. (2001). Effects of an auditory model on the learning of relative and absolute timing. Journal of Motor Behavior, 33, 127–138.

Sherwood, D. E. (1996). The benefits of random variable practice for spatial accuracy and error detection in a rapid aiming task. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 67, 35–43.

Sherwood, D. E. (2009). Spatial error detection in rapid unimanual and bimanual aiming movements. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 108, 3–14.

Sherwood, D. E. (2010). Detecting and correcting errors in rapid aiming movements: Effects of movement time, distance, and velocity. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 81, 300–309.

Singer, R. N., Hausenblas, H. A., & Janelle, C. M. (2001). Handbook of sport psychology (2nd ed.). New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Sparrow, W. A. (1983). The efficiency of skilled performance. Journal of Motor Behavior, 15, 237–261.

Swinnen, S. P. (1996). Information feedback for motor learning: A review. In H. N. Zelaznik (Ed.), Advances in motor learning and control (pp. 37–66). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Torriero, S., Oliveri, M., Koch, G., Caltagirone, C., & Petrosini, L. (2007). The what and how of observational learning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1656–1663.

Twining, W. E. (1949). Mental practice and physical practice in learning a motor skill.

Research Quarterly, 20, 432–435.

Willingham, D. B. (1998). A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 105, 558–584.

Wright, D. L., Li, Y., & Cody, W. (1997). Cognitive processes related to contextual

interference and observational learning: A replication of Blandin, Proteau, and Alain.

Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 68, 106–109.

Yando, R., Seitz, & Zigler, E. (1978). Imitation: A developmental perspective. New York:

Wiley and Sons.

Young, D. E., & Schmidt, R. A. (1992). Augmented feedback for enhanced skill acquisition.

In G. E. Stelmach, & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior (pp. 677–693).

Amsterdam: North-Holland.

附 錄

附錄一

參加者須知及同意書

您好:

我是國立臺灣師範大學體育學系碩士班的研究生,目前正在進行關於動作學習的研 究,非常榮幸能邀請您參與本次的研究,希望能先徵求您的同意並簽名。

本實驗過程共需兩天的時間,實驗工作為簡單的空間移動工作,第一天需要您執行 該動作(或觀察)60 次,過程約一個小時;第二天同樣以此動作進行 12 的次試作,過 程約 5~10 分鐘。本實驗純屬動作學習的研究,不會對人體健康造成任何傷害。實驗結 果僅對團體結果做比較,不做個人分析,您可以自由選擇是否參與此次研究。

在您簽署同意參與本實驗,若於實驗期間有任何不舒服的感覺、或改變心意想退出,

您可以隨時告知研究者,退出實驗而不受任何約束。同時為確保研究參與者的隱私權,

任何有關您的資料均會妥善保管,旁人無從獲知。若將來本篇研究公開發表或出版時亦 不會顯現您的資料。

最後,誠摯的感謝您的熱情參與協助本研究。

研 究 者:郭 安 婕 (H)02-28276982 (M)0938-828394 指導老師:卓 俊 伶 博士 (O)02-77343201

單 位: 國 立 台 灣 師 範 大 學 體 育 學 系

『參加者同意書』 編號 ______ 組別_____日期_______

經過詳細閱讀以上的陳述,我已了解且同意參與上述研究。

學生姓名(簽名):___________________,性別:______

出 生 日 期:_________年_____月_____日

聯 絡 電 話:_________________________

附錄二

慣用手問卷

No.

謝謝您參與本研究,請依照您個人平時的習慣勾選回答下列問題

項目 勾選欄

用筷子吃飯時,我會用 □右手 □左手

簽名時,我會用 □右手 □左手

畫圖時,我會用 □右手 □左手

需要切水果時,我會用 □右手 □左手

需要用湯匙喝湯時,我會用 □右手 □左手

喝飲料需插吸管時,我會用 □右手 □左手

使用打火機時,我會用 □右手 □左手

掃地時,我會用 □右手 □左手

刷牙時,我會用 □右手 □左手

投擲垃圾,我會用 □右手 □左手

附錄三

表 4 保留測驗之客觀與主觀錯誤絕對誤差值-單因子變異數分析摘要表

變異來源 SS df MS F η2 組間 (B) 15.803 2 7.902 8.052* .040 組內 (W) 379.789 387 .981

全體(Total) 395.592 389

*p < .05,單位:度(°)

表5 保留測驗之客觀與主觀錯誤絕對誤差值-組別主要效果事後比較表

組別 M 觀察組 身體練習組 控制組

觀察組 0.72 - - *

身體練習組 0.84 - *

控制組 1.19 -

*p < .05,單位:度(°)

表 6 保留測驗之絕對誤差值-單因子變異數分析摘要表

變異來源 SS df MS F η2 組間 (B) 110.496 2 55.248 13.553* .065 組內 (W) 1577.548 387 4.076

全體(Total) 1688.044 389

*p < .05

表7 保留測驗之絕對誤差值-組別主要效果事後比較表

組別 M 觀察組 身體練習組 控制組

觀察組 1.77 - - *

身體練習組 1.88 - *

控制組 2.95 -

*p < .05,單位:度(°)

表 8 保留測驗之變異誤差值-單因子變異數分析摘要表

變異來源 SS df MS F η2 組間 (B) 9.702 2 4.851 4.493* .200 組內 (W) 38.872 36 1.080

全體(Total) 48.574 38

*p < .05

表9 保留測驗之變異誤差值-組別主要效果事後比較表

組別 M 觀察組 身體練習組 控制組

觀察組 2.00 - - *

身體練習組 1.99 - *

控制組 3.05 -

*p < .05,單位:度(°)

表 10 保留測驗之錯誤修正誤差值-單因子變異數分析摘要表

變異來源 SS df MS F η2 組間 (B) 62.232 2 31.116 9.920* .054 組內 (W) 1091.564 348 3.137

全體(Total) 1153.796 350

*p < .05

表11 保留測驗之錯誤修正誤差值-組別主要效果事後比較表

組別 M 觀察組 身體練習組 控制組

觀察組 1.79 - - *

身體練習組 1.91 - *

控制組 2.73 -

*p < .05,單位:度(°)

相關文件