• 沒有找到結果。

CHAPTER 6 CONCLUSION

6.1 Summary of the Thesis

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

115

CHAPTER 6

CONCLUSION

An important claim of the Conceptual Metaphor Theory is that the locus of metaphor is

in thought. This view indicates that metaphors can be realized in different modalities.

Following this view, the thesis examined the linguistic and gestural manifestations of

conceptual metaphors in conversational discourse. Section 6.1 provides a summary of the

thesis. Section 6.2 presents the limitations and direction for future study.

6.1 Summary of the Thesis

The thesis aimed to explore people’s habitual cross-modal expression of metaphors in

daily conversation, and to investigate the collaboration of language and gesture in the

expression of metaphorical thoughts. The present study focused on the metaphors realized

concurrently in language and gesture, and the metaphors realized in gesture exclusively.

These two groups of metaphors were investigated with regard to metaphor types,

source-domain concepts, target-domain concepts, source-to-target correspondences, and

temporal patterning of speech and gesture. The present study also examined whether the two

groups of metaphors were similar or different with regard to the above issues.

According to the past research (Lakoff &Johnson 1980c, 1999; McNeill 1992; Talmy

1996), nine metaphor types were identified. In the current data seven of them were found:

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

causation metaphor, conduit metaphor, container metaphor, entity metaphor, fictive-motion

metaphor, orientation metaphor, and complex metaphor. Entity metaphor is prone to be

conveyed in metaphors in the gesture-only group. In both the language-gesture and

gesture-only groups, entity metaphor and orientation metaphor are the most common

metaphor types to be expressed in daily communication. Our experience of discrete objects

offers “a further basis...that goes beyond mere orientation” (Lakoff and Johnson 1980c: 25).

Understanding abstract concepts in terms of objects then allow us to project various

experiences of object to the concepts. Thus, it is likely that entity metaphor is frequently used

to conceive abstract concepts. Spatial orientations provide an “extraordinary rich basis” for

reasoning about other concepts, and orientation metaphor are closely relate to our physical

and cultural experiences (Lakoff and Johnson 1980c: 25). In some cases, it is difficult to find

an alternative to talk about a concept without the help of the orientation metaphor. Hence, the

orientation metaphor is often utilized to conceptualize abstract concepts.

Based on the notion of image schemas (Johnson 1987; Cienki 1997; Clausner & Croft

1999; Santibáñez 2002), nine kinds of source-domain concepts were distinguished in

previous studies. Seven of them were found in the current data: ACTIVITY, CONTAINER,

FICTIVE-MOTION, FORCE, OBJECT, PATH, and SPACE. Metaphors in the language-gesture

group tend to employ SPACE as their sources; metaphors in the gesture-only group are apt to

utilize OBJECT as their sources. Taken the two groups together, OBJECT is the most frequent

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

source for entity metaphors; SPACE and PATH is the second most frequent source for

orientation metaphors. Concerning the target-domain concepts, the present study focused on

the targets that had at least five tokens in the data. Eight kinds of targets were identified:

GROUP, MENTAL ACTIVITY, (physical) ACTIVITY, DEGREE, SEQUENCE, SPEECH CONTENT,

STATE, and TIME. SPEECH CONTENT is inclined to be conceived by metaphors in language

and gesture. Targets like STATE, PHYSICAL ACTIVITY, GROUP and MENTAL ACTIVITY are

prone to be conceptualized by metaphors in gesture exclusively. Among all the 247

metaphoric expressions, STATE, TIME, and PHYSICAL ACTIVITY are the common targets to

be manifested through metaphors.

With regard to the metaphorical mappings between the two domains, the present study

analyzed the one-source-to-many-targets correspondences and the many-sources-to-one target

correspondences. Excluding FORCE which has merely a token, all the sources could be used

to conceive multiple targets. OBJECT can be employed to conceptualize a great variety of

targets; concepts like STATE and TIME are the common targets mapping to OBJECT. SPACE is

usually employed to conceive concepts like TIME, SEQUENCE, and DEGREE. The sources

PATH, FICTIVE-MOTION, ACTIVITY,and CONTAINER are utilized to conceptualize multiple

targets as well. According to Lakoff and Johnson (1980b), metaphors have partial nature in

the way that a metaphor only profiles a certain aspect of a concept. An abstract concept is

likely to be conceptualized in terms of more than one concrete concept. In the current data,

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

TIME, MENTAL ACTIVITY, SPEECH CONTENT, SEQUENCE, and DEGREE are the targets

which are conceived through multiple sources. These results demonstrate the common types

of metaphors, the source- and target-domain concepts, and the source-target correspondences

in daily conversations.

Furthermore, the collaboration of speech and gesture enables us to look at the

hypotheses of speech-gesture productions—the Lexical Semantic Hypothesis and the

Interface Hypothesis. The present study examined the two hypotheses from the following

aspects: the temporal patterning of speech and gesture, the linguistic unit to influence the

content of a metaphoric gesture, and the semantic coordination of speech and gesture in

conveying metaphors. According to the Lexical Semantic Hypothesis, gestures can help

lexical search when one has difficulty to retrieve a word in speech; hence, gesture strokes

may precede the associated speech. Since gestures are thought to be generated from the

semantics of the lexical items, it is predicted that gestures can only encode information that is

conveyed in accompanying speech and that the linguistic unit to determine the content of

gestures is a single word. However, the Interface Hypothesis suggests that gestural content is

shaped on-line by information which is exchanged between linguistic and spatio-motoric

thinking. Thus, gestures temporally synchronize with related speech, and the relevant unit to

influence the content of a gesture is a processing unit—which can be a clause to a certain

extent—for speech production. Since gestures are thought to be produced from an interface

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

between linguistic and spatio-motoric information, they may encode what is conveyed or not

conveyed in accompanying speech. Results from the current data show that many metaphoric

gestures synchronize with their associated speech, which supports the view of the Interface

Hypothesis more. Although most of the lexical affiliates of the metaphoric gestures are words,

a substantial portion has the gesture associated with a phrase rather than with a single word.

This portion of data provides evidence in contrast to the Lexical Semantic Hypothesis. In the

current data, over a half of the metaphoric expressions are gesture-only metaphors in which

language expresses the target-domain concepts and gesture expresses the sour-domain

concepts. Gesture-only metaphors reveal that the information encoded in speech and gesture

may differ from each other in conveying metaphors. This portion of data opposes to the

prediction of the Lexical Semantic Hypothesis but agrees with the Interface Hypothesis. In

general, results based on the current data support the Interface Hypothesis more than the

Lexical Semantic Hypothesis.