• 沒有找到結果。

Conceptual Metaphor in Gesture

CHAPTER 2 LITERATURE REVIEW

2.2 Conceptual Metaphor in Gesture

phrases can facilitate people’s immediate comprehension of the metaphors.

2.2 Conceptual Metaphor in Gesture

So far, we have discussed the studies on linguistic expressions of metaphors. However,

some scholars suspect that the language-based analysis can directly reflect the pattern of our

thought (Murphy 1996, 1997; Glucksberg 2001). It is suggested that evidence from different

aspects other than language is needed for the discussion about metaphor. Gestural studies

may help to enhance the cognitive reality of metaphors. Since metaphors are conceptual,

language is not the exclusive realization of metaphors. Gesture is regarded as an independent

non-verbal modality where we may find the metaphorical expressions (Cienki 2008; Cienki

& Müller 2008; Müller 2008; Gibbs 2008b). In McNeill’s Hand and Mind, he declared:

Gestures are like thoughts themselves. They belong, not to the outside world, but to the inside one of memory, thought, and mental images. Gesture images are complex, intricately interconnected, and not at all like photographs. Gestures open up a wholly new way of regarding thought process, language, and the interaction of people (McNeill 1992: 12).

Likewise, Cienki (2008) claims that spontaneous gestures are produced unconsciously while

speaking and that they can provide insight into thought formulation. According to McNeill

(1992: 14), metaphoric gestures are “like iconic gestures in that they are pictorial, but the

pictorial content presents an abstract idea rather than a concrete object or event...[and]

presents an image of the invisible—an image of an abstraction”. The imagery presented by a

metaphoric gesture serves as the source domain, and the abstract concept which the imagery

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

conveys is the target domain.

The research on gestures can provide insight for the issue about metaphor. Metaphoric

gestures do not merely duplicate what we find in metaphors conveyed in language; they may

profile different aspects of metaphorical concepts (Cienki 2008; Cienki & Müller 2008). For

instance, the temporal ordering may be realized via the metaphors with different spatial

orientations. In European languages, the future is thought to be located ahead of us and the

past event comes to us before the future event (Cienki 2008). We may say I did X before Y

where X is the past event, yet it is unusual to say I did X to the left of Y to mean the same idea.

However, Calbris (2008: 43) found a gestural data that the order of time is metaphorically

expressed via the left-right orientation. In the example offered by Calbris, the speaker’s left

index finger moves to the left to present the metaphor PAST IS LEFT; his right index finger

moves to the right to realize the metaphor FUTURE IS RIGHT. Although the metaphors based

on left-right orientation are rare in linguistic data, the gestural data can provide evidence for

the cognitive reality for the metaphors PAST IS LEFT and FUTURE IS RIGHT. The case

suggests that gestures can provide additional evidence of the source-to-target

correspondences.

Gesture is not only an important modality to reflect our thought but also an

independent source to reinforce the psychological reality of conceptual metaphors. Müller

(2008) examined data taken from narrative interviews and conversations and finds that

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

metaphor is presented in linguistic and gestural modalities at the same time. The finding

supports the view that metaphor is not a phenomenon of language. He also observed that

metaphors are not just single lexical terms or idioms. Gestural data shows that a metaphor can

persist over longer pieces of discourse and supports the assumption that a metaphor can be

the product of the general cognitive process structuring an entire argument.

Spontaneous gesture is “[a]nother important form of embodiment in language”

(Barsalou 2008: 629). In the past research on language, image schema has been studied as the

embodied source of metaphors. With regard to the gestural modality, Cienki’s (2005)

investigation proves that image schema could be used reliably to characterize co-speech

gesture. In his experiment, participants watch videos clips showing a person making hand

gestures with or without sound. After watching each clip, they need to choose an appropriate

description for the hand movement by circling a word from a given list which contains

several terms of image schema and the term other. Results show that the category of other

was seldom used. Although each stimulus provokes a variable response, there is a statistically

reliable agreement about what schema would be elicited by each gesture. Despite the

idiosyncratic forms of the gestures with the same meaning, most of them shared a basis in our

experience. Image schemas might provide common patterns underlying idiosyncratic gestures

(Cienki 2005). Furthermore, image schema is proposed to be one of the iconic symbols that

build the foundation for metaphoric projections in gesture by Mittelberg (2008). In her

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

research, semiotic theory and conceptual metaphor theory are combined in the analysis of the

lectures by linguistic professors.

Gestural expressions help us explore the embodied nature of conceptual metaphor. At

the same time, metaphoric gestures offer visible evidence for how metaphors are grounded in

non-metaphoric embodied patterns. Núñez (2008) examined the lectures by mathematicians

to look at the foundation of mathematics. He analyzes the mathematical expressions to

discuss pure mathematical ideas such as approaching limits and oscillating function. The

expressions are used to describe facts about numbers or the result of the operations with

numbers, and there is nothing really moving. For instance, Núñez observed that a professor

moves his right hand back and forth horizontally while he uttered still oscillate (2008: 108).

The linguistic expressions about mathematics may be accompanied with spontaneous

gestures depicting the fictive motions for the static numbers. Therefore, Núñez claimed that

metaphoric expressions about mathematics are not simply dead metaphors. He also concludes

that mathematics, the most abstract concept, is ultimately grounded in the human body.

There are studies on the metaphoric gestures in the conversation in Chinese as well. In

Chui’s (2011, 2013) studies on Chinese discourse, she maintained that gesture can provide

visible evidence for the presence of source domains in metaphorical correspondences. She

also claimed that many metaphors in language are substantiated by metaphoric gestures. The

manifestations of metaphors in the use of hand gesture demonstrate that metaphorical

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y

expressions are not lexicalized. The bodily experiences can affect the performance of

metaphors in gesture, revealing that “even the most clichéd metaphoric phrases are not

understood through simple retrieval of their meanings stored in a phrasal, mental lexicon”

(Gibbs 2008a: 295). Furthermore, Chui (2011) found that gestures may provide additional

information about which aspect of the concept gains a speaker’s focus of attention in

real-time communication. The metaphor TIME IS SPACE can be realized in different spatial

orientations which conveys different viewpoints. For instance, PAST is gestured forward with

time-moving perspective and backward with an ego-moving perspective at different moments

of speaking. In general, “gestures make knowledge and the situated-dynamic aspect of

cognition visible” (Chui 2013: 61).

Previous research on metaphors in gesture has provided considerable empirical

evidence for the reality of metaphorical thinking and the embodiment of metaphor in body

experiences and socio-culture practices. Different kinds of gestural expressions of metaphors

can be found in these studies as well. However, the past studies in gesture merely adopt

qualitative analysis. To discuss the habitual expressions of different metaphor types as well as

the source and target domains in metaphorical correspondences, the present study would like

to apply quantitative analysis in the investigation of metaphors in gesture in conversations.

The present study is then able to have reliable evidence from the quantitative data to compare

the metaphors in language and gesture and the metaphors in gesture-only.

‧ 國

立 政 治 大 學

N a tio na

l C h engchi U ni ve rs it y